SlideShare una empresa de Scribd logo
1 de 18
COGNITIVE
GRAMMAR
developed by Ronald Langacker



            Discussant:
          Joy A. Verzosa
Cognitive grammar is a
 cognitive approach to language
developed by Ronald Langacker,
         which considers
    the basic units of language to be
              symbols
 or conventional pairings of a semantic
 structure with a phonological label.
Like construction grammar (developed by
Langacker's student Adele Goldberg), and
unlike many mainstream linguistic
theories,
cognitive grammar extends the notion
of symbolic units to the grammar of
languages.
Langacker develops the central ideas of cognitive grammar
in his seminal, two-volume Foundations of cognitive
grammar, which became a major departure point for the
emerging field of Cognitive Linguistics.

 he further assumes that linguistic structures are
 motivated by general cognitive processes.

In formulating his theory, he makes extensive
use of principles of gestalt psychology and
draws analogies between linguistic structure
and aspects of visual perception.
Cognitive Grammar:   A Basic
Introduction
        Ronald Langacker
Orientation

     Grammar forms a continuum with lexicon and is fully
     describable as assemblies of symbolic structures (form-
                       meaning pairings)

 Lexicon varies with respect to the complexity of expressions
 and the degree of specifity of the meanings symbolized


Cognitive Grammar is a usage-based approach, in which
linguistic structure is seen as emerging by abstraction from
usage events, i.e. the reinforcement of what is common across
multiple instances of language use in interactive contexts.
Conceptual Semantics

Linguistic meanings do not reflect the world in any
  direct or straightforward manner, but rather
  embody particular ways of construing the
  situations described, often involving imagination
  and mental constructions.

An expression derives its meaning by flexibly
invoking an open-ended set of cognitive domains,
i.e. concepts or conceptual complexes of any
degree of complexity.
CONSTRUAL



 An expression's meaning depends not only on
   the conceptual content it evokes but also on
   the construal it imposes on that content.

Broad classes of construal phenomena include specificity,
focusing, prominence, and perspective. Specificity (or its
inverse, schematicity) is the degree of precision and
detail at which a situation is characterized.
Grammatical Classes

     The standard doctrine that basic
grammatical classes (parts of speech) are
     not semantically definable rests on
 erroneous assumptions about the nature
 of linguistic meaning. With a proper view
  of meaning, basic categories—notably
       noun and verb—have plausible
 conceptual characterizations at both the
prototype level (for typical examples) and
the schema level (valid for all instances).
The schemas are independent of any particular conceptual content,
  residing instead in basic cognitive abilities immanent in the archetypes:
   for nouns, grouping and reification; in the case of verbs, the ability to
     apprehend relationships and to track their evolution through time.

 An expression's grammatical category specifically depends on the
 nature of its profile (not its overall content). Thus a noun profiles a thing
 (defined abstractly as any product of grouping and reification), while a
 verb profiles a process (a relationship tracked through time).

 Expressions that profile non-process relationships include adjectives,
 adverbs, prepositions, infinitives, and participles. Relational expressions
 can be categorized in different ways, depending on factors like the
 number and type of focused participants, whether the profiled relation is
 simple or complex, and whether it is apprehended holistically or
 sequentially.

These characterizations prove efficacious in describing how relational
expressions function as noun modifiers and in clausal organization.
M AJOR SUBCLASSES
       Despite being polar opposites conceptually, the two
 most fundamental grammatical classes—noun and verb—
 show extensive parallelism.

One similarity is that both divide into two major subclasses:
count vs. mass for nouns, perfective vs. imperfective for
verbs. Allowing for the intrinsic conceptual difference
between nouns and verbs, these oppositions are precisely the
same.
The essential feature of count nouns and perfective verbs is that the
profiled thing or process is construed as being bounded within the
immediate scope in a particular cognitive domain: the domain of
instantiation, characterized as the domain where instances of a type are
primarily conceived as residing and are distinguished from one another
by their locations
For nouns, the domain of instantiation varies, although space is
prototypical; for verbs, the relevant domain is always time. Correlated with
bounding are other distinguishing properties: internal heterogeneity (for
count and perfective) vs. homogeneity (for mass and imperfective);
contractibility (the property of masses and imperfectives whereby any
subpart of an instance is itself an instance of its type); and expansibility
(whereby combining two mass or imperfective instances yields a single,
larger instance).



 Count vs. mass and perfective vs. imperfective are not rigid lexical
distinctions, but are malleable owing to alternate construals as well as
systematic patterns of extension. The conceptual characterization of
perfective and imperfective verbs explains their contrasting behavior with
respect to the English progressive and present tense.
Constructions
             General Characterization
Grammar consists in patterns for assembling symbolically complex expressions.
Such expressions are characterized as assemblies of symbolic structures, also called
constructions. In large measure, symbolic assemblies are hierarchically arranged: at
a given level of organization, component symbolic structures are integrated to form a
composite symbolic structure, which can in turn function as component structure at a
higher level, and so on.

Component structures are integrated both semantically and phonologically, the
phonological integration serving to symbolize the semantic integration. Although
linguistic meanings are only partially compositional, compositional patterns are
essential to the formation and understanding of novel expressions. These patterns
are themselves symbolic assemblies, differing from expressions just by virtue of
being schematic rather than specific; they are thus referred to as constructional
schemas
Constructions
                  Descriptive Factors
  Constructions are characterized in terms of four basic factors: correspondences,
  profiling, elaboration, and constituency. Correspondences are the basis for semantic
  and grammatical integration; they specify the conceptual and phonological overlap
  between component structures, as well as between the component and composite
  structures. Semantic integration often involves multiple correspondences. Semantic
  anomaly arises when corresponding elements have inconsistent properties.

A component which makes salient schematic reference to another in this manner is
said to be dependent on it. Organization in relationships of autonomy/dependence
(A/D-alignment) is a basic feature of language structure. The difference between
complements and modifiers is a matter of whether these component structures are
autonomous or dependent with respect to the constructional head. Constituency is
the hierarchical aspect of symbolic assemblies. Contrary to standard views,
constituency is neither fundamental nor essential to grammar, and while it does
emerge, it is neither invariant nor exhaustive of grammatical structure.
Grammatical relations (like subject and object) are defined on the basis of semantic
factors and correspondences, and are thus independent of particular constituency
configurations.
Rules and Restrictions
Language is both cognitive and sociocultural, consisting in conventionally
sanctioned patterns of communicative activity. These patterns take the form of
schemas abstracted from usage events by the reinforcement of recurring
commonalities. Conventional linguistic units are linked by relationships of
composition and categorization (either elaboration or extension) and thus form
intersecting networks of great complexity.

Expressions are interpreted and assessed for well-formedness through
categorization by linguistic units. Through a process of interactive activation,
particular units are selected to categorize particular facets of an expression. The
total set of categorizing relationships constitutes the expression's structural
description, and whether the categorizations involve elaboration or extension
determines its degree of conventionality. Despite the absence of explicit
prohibitions, this model affords an account of distribution, restrictions, and
judgments of ungrammaticality.
Engaging the World
 Because it unfolds through time, conceptualization (and hence linguistic meaning)
 is inherently dynamic. There are numerous natural paths that it tends to follow,
 and which tend to coalign in linguistic structure. In one kind of path, a salient
 reference point provides mental access to a target. Certain basic grammatical
 phenomena are analyzed in terms of reference point relationships, including
 possessives, pronominal anaphora, topic constructions, and trajector/ landmark
 organization (subject and object).

A subject differs from a discourse topic by being structurally internal to a clause
and conceptually intrinsic to the clausal process. Trajector and landmark are
characterized dynamically as the first and second reference points evoked in
building up to the full conception of a profiled relationship. This explains their
general grammatical accessibility as well as their role in certain specific
constructions.
The mental world we construct is grounded in our experience as creatures with
bodies who engage in motor and sensory interactions (embodiment). In
constructing it, we transcend direct experience through abstraction, conceptual
integration, and subjectification: the application of mental operations immanent in
certain conceptions to situations for which their occurrence is extrinsic. Examples
include fictive motion, fictive change, and the covert invocation of imagined
scenarios.

Mental simulation is a fundamental aspect of conception and linguistic meaning.
Subjectification is an important factor in grammaticization (the evolution of
grammatical elements from lexical sources). Many grammatical notions are
subjective counterparts of basic aspects of everyday experience.


Grammar reflects the means of
disengagement through which we
transcend immediate experience
and construct our mental world. It is
thus a key to conceptual analysis.
References:


    http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cognitive_grammar
    Langacker, Ronald W. (1982) 'Space Grammar, Analysability, and the English Passive',
     Language, 58, 1, 22-80.
    Langacker, Ronald W. (1987) Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, Volume 1, Theoretical
     Prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
    Langacker, Ronald W. (1990) Concept, Image, and Symbol: The Cognitive Basis of Grammar .
     (Cognitive Linguistics Research 1.) Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter. [paperback edition
     1991]
    Langacker, Ronald W. (1991) Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, Volume 2, Descriptive
     Application. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
    Langacker, Ronald W. (2008) Cognitive Grammar: A Basic Introduction. New York: Oxford
     University Press.
    Taylor, John R. (2002) Cognitive Grammar. Oxford Textbooks in Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford
     University Press.

Más contenido relacionado

La actualidad más candente

Transformational-Generative Grammar
Transformational-Generative GrammarTransformational-Generative Grammar
Transformational-Generative Grammar
Ruth Ann Llego
 
Transformational generative grammar
Transformational  generative grammarTransformational  generative grammar
Transformational generative grammar
Baishakhi Amin
 
Equivalencein translation
Equivalencein translationEquivalencein translation
Equivalencein translation
Dorina Moisa
 
The lexical approach and lexical priming(1)
The lexical approach and lexical priming(1)The lexical approach and lexical priming(1)
The lexical approach and lexical priming(1)
walkea
 

La actualidad más candente (20)

Lexicography
LexicographyLexicography
Lexicography
 
Cognitive linguistics
Cognitive linguisticsCognitive linguistics
Cognitive linguistics
 
Transformational-Generative Grammar
Transformational-Generative GrammarTransformational-Generative Grammar
Transformational-Generative Grammar
 
Systemic Functional Linguistics
Systemic Functional LinguisticsSystemic Functional Linguistics
Systemic Functional Linguistics
 
Presentation on cda
Presentation on cdaPresentation on cda
Presentation on cda
 
COGNITIVE FACTORS OF SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING
COGNITIVE FACTORS OF SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNINGCOGNITIVE FACTORS OF SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING
COGNITIVE FACTORS OF SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING
 
Transformational generative grammar
Transformational  generative grammarTransformational  generative grammar
Transformational generative grammar
 
Language Shift and Language Maintenance
Language Shift and Language MaintenanceLanguage Shift and Language Maintenance
Language Shift and Language Maintenance
 
Intro to-stylistics
Intro to-stylisticsIntro to-stylistics
Intro to-stylistics
 
Semantics: Seven types of meaning
Semantics: Seven types of meaningSemantics: Seven types of meaning
Semantics: Seven types of meaning
 
Sociolinguistics language variations
Sociolinguistics language variationsSociolinguistics language variations
Sociolinguistics language variations
 
Unit of Translation.pptx
Unit of Translation.pptxUnit of Translation.pptx
Unit of Translation.pptx
 
Equivalencein translation
Equivalencein translationEquivalencein translation
Equivalencein translation
 
The lexical approach and lexical priming(1)
The lexical approach and lexical priming(1)The lexical approach and lexical priming(1)
The lexical approach and lexical priming(1)
 
grammar and style
grammar and stylegrammar and style
grammar and style
 
Halliday's model of language and discousre
Halliday's model of language and discousreHalliday's model of language and discousre
Halliday's model of language and discousre
 
6) discourse grammar
6) discourse grammar6) discourse grammar
6) discourse grammar
 
Discourse analysis (Linguistics Forms and Functions)
Discourse analysis (Linguistics Forms and Functions)Discourse analysis (Linguistics Forms and Functions)
Discourse analysis (Linguistics Forms and Functions)
 
Systemic functional linguistics
Systemic functional  linguisticsSystemic functional  linguistics
Systemic functional linguistics
 
Language deth, language shift, marker, micro/macro sociolinguistics
Language deth, language shift, marker, micro/macro sociolinguisticsLanguage deth, language shift, marker, micro/macro sociolinguistics
Language deth, language shift, marker, micro/macro sociolinguistics
 

Destacado

когнитивная лингвистика
когнитивная лингвистикакогнитивная лингвистика
когнитивная лингвистика
Marina Skoromnaya
 
Grammatical case of fillmore
Grammatical case of fillmoreGrammatical case of fillmore
Grammatical case of fillmore
Ricky Jara
 
Charles fillmore s cases
Charles fillmore s casesCharles fillmore s cases
Charles fillmore s cases
ajosrdzr
 

Destacado (20)

Cognitive Grammar: Word Network
Cognitive Grammar: Word NetworkCognitive Grammar: Word Network
Cognitive Grammar: Word Network
 
Gramatyka kognitywna Ronalda Langackera
Gramatyka kognitywna Ronalda LangackeraGramatyka kognitywna Ronalda Langackera
Gramatyka kognitywna Ronalda Langackera
 
Językoznawstwo kognitywne - przewodnik po paradygmacie
Językoznawstwo kognitywne - przewodnik po paradygmacieJęzykoznawstwo kognitywne - przewodnik po paradygmacie
Językoznawstwo kognitywne - przewodnik po paradygmacie
 
Cognitive Linguistics: The Case Of Find
Cognitive Linguistics: The Case Of FindCognitive Linguistics: The Case Of Find
Cognitive Linguistics: The Case Of Find
 
Cognitive Grammar: teaching the verb 'to be'
Cognitive Grammar:  teaching the verb 'to be'Cognitive Grammar:  teaching the verb 'to be'
Cognitive Grammar: teaching the verb 'to be'
 
Cognitive Linguistics
Cognitive LinguisticsCognitive Linguistics
Cognitive Linguistics
 
Beyond the Sentence in Cognitive Grammar
Beyond the Sentence in Cognitive GrammarBeyond the Sentence in Cognitive Grammar
Beyond the Sentence in Cognitive Grammar
 
когнитивная лингвистика
когнитивная лингвистикакогнитивная лингвистика
когнитивная лингвистика
 
Когнитивная лингвистика. (Cognitive linguistics)
Когнитивная лингвистика. (Cognitive linguistics)Когнитивная лингвистика. (Cognitive linguistics)
Когнитивная лингвистика. (Cognitive linguistics)
 
Ncn preludium-2013-kj
Ncn preludium-2013-kjNcn preludium-2013-kj
Ncn preludium-2013-kj
 
Językoznawstwo kognitywne i generatywne w kontekście idealizacyjnej teorii nauki
Językoznawstwo kognitywne i generatywne w kontekście idealizacyjnej teorii naukiJęzykoznawstwo kognitywne i generatywne w kontekście idealizacyjnej teorii nauki
Językoznawstwo kognitywne i generatywne w kontekście idealizacyjnej teorii nauki
 
Pragmatic3
Pragmatic3Pragmatic3
Pragmatic3
 
Lingwistyka 24 Semantyka 4 kategoryzacja kognitywna
Lingwistyka 24 Semantyka 4 kategoryzacja kognitywnaLingwistyka 24 Semantyka 4 kategoryzacja kognitywna
Lingwistyka 24 Semantyka 4 kategoryzacja kognitywna
 
Lingwistyka 27 leksykologia kognitywna
Lingwistyka 27 leksykologia kognitywnaLingwistyka 27 leksykologia kognitywna
Lingwistyka 27 leksykologia kognitywna
 
Ciało w języku
Ciało w językuCiało w języku
Ciało w języku
 
Grammatical case of fillmore
Grammatical case of fillmoreGrammatical case of fillmore
Grammatical case of fillmore
 
Kogni2012 12
Kogni2012 12Kogni2012 12
Kogni2012 12
 
Chapter 02 hurley 12e
Chapter 02 hurley 12eChapter 02 hurley 12e
Chapter 02 hurley 12e
 
Charles fillmore s cases
Charles fillmore s casesCharles fillmore s cases
Charles fillmore s cases
 
L. Talmy's Cognitive Linguistics
L. Talmy's Cognitive LinguisticsL. Talmy's Cognitive Linguistics
L. Talmy's Cognitive Linguistics
 

Similar a Langacker's cognitive grammar

The role of vocabulary
The role of vocabulary The role of vocabulary
The role of vocabulary
Oscar Ririn
 
meaning as use a functional view of semantics
meaning as use a functional view of semanticsmeaning as use a functional view of semantics
meaning as use a functional view of semantics
Gabriela Quezada
 
melt104-functionalgrammar-121027003950-phpapp02.pdf
melt104-functionalgrammar-121027003950-phpapp02.pdfmelt104-functionalgrammar-121027003950-phpapp02.pdf
melt104-functionalgrammar-121027003950-phpapp02.pdf
AliAwan652291
 
Natural-Language-Processing-by-Dr-A-Nagesh.pdf
Natural-Language-Processing-by-Dr-A-Nagesh.pdfNatural-Language-Processing-by-Dr-A-Nagesh.pdf
Natural-Language-Processing-by-Dr-A-Nagesh.pdf
theboysaiml
 
Semantic and other disciplines
Semantic and other disciplinesSemantic and other disciplines
Semantic and other disciplines
Gustina Savhira
 
05 linguistic theory meets lexicography
05 linguistic theory meets lexicography05 linguistic theory meets lexicography
05 linguistic theory meets lexicography
Duygu Aşıklar
 
Vocabularyconnections
VocabularyconnectionsVocabularyconnections
Vocabularyconnections
ingridbelloa
 

Similar a Langacker's cognitive grammar (20)

англ
англангл
англ
 
MELT 104 Functional Grammar
MELT 104   Functional GrammarMELT 104   Functional Grammar
MELT 104 Functional Grammar
 
The role of vocabulary
The role of vocabulary The role of vocabulary
The role of vocabulary
 
meaning as use a functional view of semantics
meaning as use a functional view of semanticsmeaning as use a functional view of semantics
meaning as use a functional view of semantics
 
melt104-functionalgrammar-121027003950-phpapp02.pdf
melt104-functionalgrammar-121027003950-phpapp02.pdfmelt104-functionalgrammar-121027003950-phpapp02.pdf
melt104-functionalgrammar-121027003950-phpapp02.pdf
 
Lexicology as a science
Lexicology as a scienceLexicology as a science
Lexicology as a science
 
Discourse analysis
Discourse analysisDiscourse analysis
Discourse analysis
 
Meaning as use
Meaning as useMeaning as use
Meaning as use
 
Book review of analyzing grammar an introduction
Book review of analyzing grammar  an introductionBook review of analyzing grammar  an introduction
Book review of analyzing grammar an introduction
 
Functional grammar
Functional grammarFunctional grammar
Functional grammar
 
Functional grammar
Functional grammarFunctional grammar
Functional grammar
 
Natural-Language-Processing-by-Dr-A-Nagesh.pdf
Natural-Language-Processing-by-Dr-A-Nagesh.pdfNatural-Language-Processing-by-Dr-A-Nagesh.pdf
Natural-Language-Processing-by-Dr-A-Nagesh.pdf
 
Semantic and other disciplines
Semantic and other disciplinesSemantic and other disciplines
Semantic and other disciplines
 
05 linguistic theory meets lexicography
05 linguistic theory meets lexicography05 linguistic theory meets lexicography
05 linguistic theory meets lexicography
 
Scopes of linguistic description 1
Scopes of linguistic description 1Scopes of linguistic description 1
Scopes of linguistic description 1
 
APPLYING TEXTUAL METAFUNCTION IN EFL WRITING AN APPROACH TO TEACH WRITING
APPLYING TEXTUAL METAFUNCTION IN EFL WRITING  AN APPROACH TO TEACH WRITINGAPPLYING TEXTUAL METAFUNCTION IN EFL WRITING  AN APPROACH TO TEACH WRITING
APPLYING TEXTUAL METAFUNCTION IN EFL WRITING AN APPROACH TO TEACH WRITING
 
Vocabularyconnections
VocabularyconnectionsVocabularyconnections
Vocabularyconnections
 
discourse analysis
discourse analysisdiscourse analysis
discourse analysis
 
Form and function.
Form and function.Form and function.
Form and function.
 
Class3 - What is This Language Structure
Class3 - What is This Language StructureClass3 - What is This Language Structure
Class3 - What is This Language Structure
 

Langacker's cognitive grammar

  • 1. COGNITIVE GRAMMAR developed by Ronald Langacker Discussant: Joy A. Verzosa
  • 2. Cognitive grammar is a cognitive approach to language developed by Ronald Langacker, which considers the basic units of language to be symbols or conventional pairings of a semantic structure with a phonological label.
  • 3. Like construction grammar (developed by Langacker's student Adele Goldberg), and unlike many mainstream linguistic theories, cognitive grammar extends the notion of symbolic units to the grammar of languages.
  • 4. Langacker develops the central ideas of cognitive grammar in his seminal, two-volume Foundations of cognitive grammar, which became a major departure point for the emerging field of Cognitive Linguistics. he further assumes that linguistic structures are motivated by general cognitive processes. In formulating his theory, he makes extensive use of principles of gestalt psychology and draws analogies between linguistic structure and aspects of visual perception.
  • 5. Cognitive Grammar: A Basic Introduction Ronald Langacker
  • 6. Orientation Grammar forms a continuum with lexicon and is fully describable as assemblies of symbolic structures (form- meaning pairings) Lexicon varies with respect to the complexity of expressions and the degree of specifity of the meanings symbolized Cognitive Grammar is a usage-based approach, in which linguistic structure is seen as emerging by abstraction from usage events, i.e. the reinforcement of what is common across multiple instances of language use in interactive contexts.
  • 7. Conceptual Semantics Linguistic meanings do not reflect the world in any direct or straightforward manner, but rather embody particular ways of construing the situations described, often involving imagination and mental constructions. An expression derives its meaning by flexibly invoking an open-ended set of cognitive domains, i.e. concepts or conceptual complexes of any degree of complexity.
  • 8. CONSTRUAL An expression's meaning depends not only on the conceptual content it evokes but also on the construal it imposes on that content. Broad classes of construal phenomena include specificity, focusing, prominence, and perspective. Specificity (or its inverse, schematicity) is the degree of precision and detail at which a situation is characterized.
  • 9. Grammatical Classes The standard doctrine that basic grammatical classes (parts of speech) are not semantically definable rests on erroneous assumptions about the nature of linguistic meaning. With a proper view of meaning, basic categories—notably noun and verb—have plausible conceptual characterizations at both the prototype level (for typical examples) and the schema level (valid for all instances).
  • 10. The schemas are independent of any particular conceptual content, residing instead in basic cognitive abilities immanent in the archetypes: for nouns, grouping and reification; in the case of verbs, the ability to apprehend relationships and to track their evolution through time. An expression's grammatical category specifically depends on the nature of its profile (not its overall content). Thus a noun profiles a thing (defined abstractly as any product of grouping and reification), while a verb profiles a process (a relationship tracked through time). Expressions that profile non-process relationships include adjectives, adverbs, prepositions, infinitives, and participles. Relational expressions can be categorized in different ways, depending on factors like the number and type of focused participants, whether the profiled relation is simple or complex, and whether it is apprehended holistically or sequentially. These characterizations prove efficacious in describing how relational expressions function as noun modifiers and in clausal organization.
  • 11. M AJOR SUBCLASSES Despite being polar opposites conceptually, the two most fundamental grammatical classes—noun and verb— show extensive parallelism. One similarity is that both divide into two major subclasses: count vs. mass for nouns, perfective vs. imperfective for verbs. Allowing for the intrinsic conceptual difference between nouns and verbs, these oppositions are precisely the same. The essential feature of count nouns and perfective verbs is that the profiled thing or process is construed as being bounded within the immediate scope in a particular cognitive domain: the domain of instantiation, characterized as the domain where instances of a type are primarily conceived as residing and are distinguished from one another by their locations
  • 12. For nouns, the domain of instantiation varies, although space is prototypical; for verbs, the relevant domain is always time. Correlated with bounding are other distinguishing properties: internal heterogeneity (for count and perfective) vs. homogeneity (for mass and imperfective); contractibility (the property of masses and imperfectives whereby any subpart of an instance is itself an instance of its type); and expansibility (whereby combining two mass or imperfective instances yields a single, larger instance). Count vs. mass and perfective vs. imperfective are not rigid lexical distinctions, but are malleable owing to alternate construals as well as systematic patterns of extension. The conceptual characterization of perfective and imperfective verbs explains their contrasting behavior with respect to the English progressive and present tense.
  • 13. Constructions General Characterization Grammar consists in patterns for assembling symbolically complex expressions. Such expressions are characterized as assemblies of symbolic structures, also called constructions. In large measure, symbolic assemblies are hierarchically arranged: at a given level of organization, component symbolic structures are integrated to form a composite symbolic structure, which can in turn function as component structure at a higher level, and so on. Component structures are integrated both semantically and phonologically, the phonological integration serving to symbolize the semantic integration. Although linguistic meanings are only partially compositional, compositional patterns are essential to the formation and understanding of novel expressions. These patterns are themselves symbolic assemblies, differing from expressions just by virtue of being schematic rather than specific; they are thus referred to as constructional schemas
  • 14. Constructions Descriptive Factors Constructions are characterized in terms of four basic factors: correspondences, profiling, elaboration, and constituency. Correspondences are the basis for semantic and grammatical integration; they specify the conceptual and phonological overlap between component structures, as well as between the component and composite structures. Semantic integration often involves multiple correspondences. Semantic anomaly arises when corresponding elements have inconsistent properties. A component which makes salient schematic reference to another in this manner is said to be dependent on it. Organization in relationships of autonomy/dependence (A/D-alignment) is a basic feature of language structure. The difference between complements and modifiers is a matter of whether these component structures are autonomous or dependent with respect to the constructional head. Constituency is the hierarchical aspect of symbolic assemblies. Contrary to standard views, constituency is neither fundamental nor essential to grammar, and while it does emerge, it is neither invariant nor exhaustive of grammatical structure. Grammatical relations (like subject and object) are defined on the basis of semantic factors and correspondences, and are thus independent of particular constituency configurations.
  • 15. Rules and Restrictions Language is both cognitive and sociocultural, consisting in conventionally sanctioned patterns of communicative activity. These patterns take the form of schemas abstracted from usage events by the reinforcement of recurring commonalities. Conventional linguistic units are linked by relationships of composition and categorization (either elaboration or extension) and thus form intersecting networks of great complexity. Expressions are interpreted and assessed for well-formedness through categorization by linguistic units. Through a process of interactive activation, particular units are selected to categorize particular facets of an expression. The total set of categorizing relationships constitutes the expression's structural description, and whether the categorizations involve elaboration or extension determines its degree of conventionality. Despite the absence of explicit prohibitions, this model affords an account of distribution, restrictions, and judgments of ungrammaticality.
  • 16. Engaging the World Because it unfolds through time, conceptualization (and hence linguistic meaning) is inherently dynamic. There are numerous natural paths that it tends to follow, and which tend to coalign in linguistic structure. In one kind of path, a salient reference point provides mental access to a target. Certain basic grammatical phenomena are analyzed in terms of reference point relationships, including possessives, pronominal anaphora, topic constructions, and trajector/ landmark organization (subject and object). A subject differs from a discourse topic by being structurally internal to a clause and conceptually intrinsic to the clausal process. Trajector and landmark are characterized dynamically as the first and second reference points evoked in building up to the full conception of a profiled relationship. This explains their general grammatical accessibility as well as their role in certain specific constructions.
  • 17. The mental world we construct is grounded in our experience as creatures with bodies who engage in motor and sensory interactions (embodiment). In constructing it, we transcend direct experience through abstraction, conceptual integration, and subjectification: the application of mental operations immanent in certain conceptions to situations for which their occurrence is extrinsic. Examples include fictive motion, fictive change, and the covert invocation of imagined scenarios. Mental simulation is a fundamental aspect of conception and linguistic meaning. Subjectification is an important factor in grammaticization (the evolution of grammatical elements from lexical sources). Many grammatical notions are subjective counterparts of basic aspects of everyday experience. Grammar reflects the means of disengagement through which we transcend immediate experience and construct our mental world. It is thus a key to conceptual analysis.
  • 18. References: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cognitive_grammar  Langacker, Ronald W. (1982) 'Space Grammar, Analysability, and the English Passive', Language, 58, 1, 22-80.  Langacker, Ronald W. (1987) Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, Volume 1, Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.  Langacker, Ronald W. (1990) Concept, Image, and Symbol: The Cognitive Basis of Grammar . (Cognitive Linguistics Research 1.) Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter. [paperback edition 1991]  Langacker, Ronald W. (1991) Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, Volume 2, Descriptive Application. Stanford: Stanford University Press.  Langacker, Ronald W. (2008) Cognitive Grammar: A Basic Introduction. New York: Oxford University Press.  Taylor, John R. (2002) Cognitive Grammar. Oxford Textbooks in Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.