SlideShare una empresa de Scribd logo
1 de 106
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Learning
Chapter Five
This multimedia product and its contents are protected under copyright law.
The following are prohibited by law:
• Any public performance or display, including transmission of any image
over a network;
• Preparation of any derivative work, including the extraction, in whole or in
part, of any images;
• Any rental, lease, or lending of the program.
Slide author:
Cynthia K.
Shinabarger Reed
Book authors:
Samuel Wood
Ellen G. Wood
Denise Boyd
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Chapter Five Overview
Classical Conditioning: The Original View
• Pavlov and Classical Conditioning
• The Process of Classical Conditioning
• Changing Conditioned Responses
• John Watson and Emotional Conditioning
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Chapter Five Overview
Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary
View
• The Cognitive Perspective
• Biological Predispositions
• Classical Conditioning in Everyday Life
• Factors Influencing Classical Conditioning
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Chapter Five Overview
Operant Conditioning
• Thorndike and the Law of Effect
• B.F. Skinner: A Pioneer in Operant Conditioning
• The Process of Operant Conditioning
• Reinforcement
• Schedules of Reinforcement
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Chapter Five Overview
Operant Conditioning (continued)
• Factors Influencing Operant Conditioning
• Punishment
• Escape and Avoidance Learning
• Applications of Operant Conditioning
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Chapter Five Overview
Cognitive Learning
• Learning by Insight
• Latent Learning and Cognitive Maps
• Observational Learning
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Learning
• A relatively permanent change in
behavior, knowledge, capability, or
attitude that is acquired through
experience and cannot be attributed to
illness, injury, or maturation
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning: The Original View
Classical Conditioning
• Classical conditioning is a type of learning
through which an organism learns to associate
one stimulus with another.
• Stimulus (the plural is stimuli): any event or
object in the environment to which an organism
responds.
• Classical conditioning is sometimes referred to
as respondent conditioning, or Pavlovian
conditioning.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning: The Original View
Pavlov and Classical Conditioning
• Ivan Pavlov organized and directed
research in physiology at the Institute of
Experimental Medicine in St. Petersburg,
Russia, from 1891 until his death 45 years
later.
• The Institute of Experimental Medicine is
where he conducted his classic
experiments on the physiology of
digestion, which won him a Nobel Prize in
1904.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning: The Original
View
Pavlov and Classical Conditioning (continued)
• Pavlov conducted a study on dogs where he
collected the saliva that the dogs would
secrete naturally in response to food placed
inside the mouth; he observed saliva
collecting when the dogs heard their food
dishes rattling, when they heard the
laboratory assistants coming to feed them,
and when they saw the attendant who fed
them.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning: The Original View
Pavlov and Classical Conditioning (continued)
Ivan Pavlov’s laboratory
• The dogs were isolated inside
soundproof cubicles and placed in
harnesses to restrain their movements.
• The experimenter observed the dogs
through a one-way mirror.
• Food and other stimuli were presented
and the flow of saliva measured by
remote control.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning: The Original View
The Process of Classical Conditioning
• Reflex: an involuntary response to a particular
stimulus, such as the eyeblink response to a
puff of air or salivation when food is placed in
the mouth
• Two types of reflexes
• Conditioned reflexes (learned): a
learned reflex rather than a naturally
occurring one.
• Unconditioned reflexes (unlearned):
inborn, automatic, unlearned response to
a particular stimulus.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning: The Original
View
The Conditioned and Unconditioned Stimulus
and Response
• Pavlov used tones, bells, buzzers, lights,
geometric shapes, electric shocks, and
metronomes in his conditioning experiments.
• Food powder was placed in the dog’s mouth,
causing salivation.
• Because dogs do not need to be conditioned to
salivate to food, salivation to food is an
unlearned response.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning: The Original View
The Conditioned and Unconditioned
Stimulus and Response (continued)
• Unconditioned response (UR): a
response that is elicited by an
unconditioned stimulus without prior
learning.
• Unconditioned stimulus (US): any
stimulus, such as food, that without prior
learning will automatically elicit, or bring
forth, an unconditioned response.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning: The Original View
The Conditioned and Unconditioned Stimulus and
Response (continued)
• Pavlov demonstrated that dogs could be
conditioned to salivate to a variety of stimuli never
before associated with food.
• During the conditioning process, the researcher
would present a neutral stimulus such as a musical
tone shortly before placing the food powder in the
dog’s mouth.
• Pavlov found that after the tone and the food were
paired many times, usually 20 or more, the tone
alone would elicit salivation.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning: The Original
View
The Conditioned and Unconditioned Stimulus
and Response (continued)
• Conditioned stimulus (CS): a neutral stimulus
that, after repeated pairing with an
unconditioned stimulus, becomes associated
with it and elicits a conditioned response.
• Conditioned response (CR): the learned
response that comes to be elicited by a
conditioned stimulus as a result of its repeated
pairing with an unconditioned stimulus.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning: The Original View
Higher-order conditioning
• Conditioning that occurs when
conditioned stimuli are linked
together to form a series of
signals
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning: The Original View
Changing Conditioned Responses
• Extinction: in classical conditioning, the
weakening and eventual disappearance of a
conditioned response as a result of
repeated presentation of the conditioned
stimulus without the unconditioned stimulus.
• Spontaneous recovery: the reappearance
of an extinguished response (in a weaker
form) when an organism is exposed to the
original conditioned stimulus following a rest
period.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning: The Original View
Changing Conditioned Responses (continued)
• When a conditioned response is extinguished in
one setting, it can still be elicited in other settings
where extinction training has not occurred.
• Pavlov found that a tone similar to the original
conditioned stimulus would produce the
conditioned response (salivation).
• Generalization: in classical conditioning, the
tendency to make a conditioned response to a
stimulus similar to the original conditioned
stimulus.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning: The Original View
Changing Conditioned Responses
(continued)
• Discrimination: the learned ability to
distinguish between similar stimuli so that
the conditioned response occurs only to the
original conditioned stimulus, but not to
similar stimuli.
• Generalization and discrimination have
survival value.
• Discriminating between a rattlesnake and a
garter snake could save your life.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning: The Original View
John Watson and Emotional Conditioning
• John Watson and his assistant, Rosalie
Rayner, conducted a study to prove that
fear could be classically conditioned.
• The subject of the study, known as Little
Albert, was a healthy and emotionally stable
11-month-old infant.
• Little Albert showed no fear except of the
loud noise Watson made by striking a
hammer against a steel bar near Albert’s
head.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning: The Original View
John Watson and Emotional Conditioning
(continued)
• Rayner presented Little Albert with a white
rat; as Albert reached for the rat, Watson
struck the steel bar with a hammer.
• This procedure was repeated several times.
• This procedure caused Albert to begin to
cry at the sight of a rat.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning: The Original View
John Watson and Emotional
Conditioning (continued)
• Watson also had ideas for removing
fears and laid the groundwork for
some behavior therapies used today.
• Watson and a colleague, Mary Cover
Jones, found 3-year-old Peter, who
was afraid of rabbits, and tried
Watson's fear-removal techniques on
him.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning: The Original View
John Watson and Emotional
Conditioning (continued)
• Peter was put in a high chair and given
candy while a rabbit was in a cage at a
safe distance from him.
• The rabbit was moved closer with each
session and eventually placed in Peter’s
lap.
• By the final session, Peter had grown
fond of the rabbit.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
The Cognitive Perspective
• Robert Rescorla is largely responsible for
changing how psychologists view classical
conditioning.
• Rescorla demonstrated that the critical element in
classical conditioning is not the repeated pairing
of the conditioned stimulus and unconditioned
stimulus.
• Rather, the important factor is whether the
conditioned stimulus provides information that
enables the organism to reliably predict the
occurrence of the unconditioned stimulus.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
The Cognitive Perspective (continued)
• Using rats as his subjects, Rescorla used a
tone as the conditioned stimulus and a shock
as the unconditioned stimulus.
• For one group of rats, the tone and shock were
paired 20 times – the shock always occurred
during the tone.
• The other group of rats also received a shock
20 times while the tone was sounding, but this
group also received 20 shocks that were not
paired with the tone.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
The Cognitive Perspective (continued)
• Only the first group, for which the tone
was a reliable predictor of the shock,
developed a conditioned fear response
to the tone.
• The second group showed little evidence
of conditioning, because the shock was
just as likely to occur without the tone as
with it.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
Biological Predispositions
• Research has shown that humans are more
easily conditioned to fear stimuli, such as snakes,
that can have very real negative effects on their
well-being
• Martin Seligman said that most common fears
“are related to the survival of the human species
through the long course of evolution.”
• He suggested that humans and other animals are
prepared to associate only certain stimuli with
particular consequences.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
Biological Predispositions (continued)
• Taste aversions: the intense dislike and/or
avoidance of particular foods that have
been associated with nausea or discomfort.
• Taste aversions can be classically
conditioned when the delay between the
conditioned stimulus (food or drink) and the
unconditioned stimulus (nausea) is as long
as 12 hours.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
Biological Predispositions (continued)
• Garcia and Koelling exposed rats to a three-
way conditioned stimulus: a bright light, a
clicking noise, and flavored water.
• For one group of rats, the unconditioned
stimulus was being exposed to X-rays or
lithium chloride, either of which produces
nausea and vomiting several hours after
exposure; for the other group, the
unconditioned stimulus was an electric
shock to the feet.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
Biological Predispositions
(continued)
• The rats in one group associated
nausea only with the flavored water;
those in the other group associated
electric shock only with the light and
the sound.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
Biological Predispositions (continued)
• Garcia and Koelling’s research established two
exceptions to traditional ideas of classical
conditioning.
– First, the finding that rats formed an
association between nausea and flavored
water ingested several hours earlier
contradicted the principle that the conditioned
stimulus must be presented shortly before the
unconditioned stimulus.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
Biological Predispositions (continued)
– Second, the finding that rats associated
electric shock only with noise and light and
nausea only with flavored water revealed that
animals are apparently biologically
predisposed to make certain associations and
that associations cannot be readily
conditioned between just any two stimuli.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
Biological Predispositions (continued)
• Gustavson and others used taste aversion
conditioning to stop wild coyotes from attacking
lambs in the western United States.
• They set out lamb flesh laced with lithium
chloride, a poison that made the coyotes
extremely ill, but was not fatal.
• After only one or two experiences, the coyotes
would get sick even at the sight of a lamb.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
Biological Predispositions (continued)
• Bernstein and others devised a
technique to help cancer patients avoid
developing aversions to desirable foods.
• A group of cancer patients were given a
novel-tasting, maple-flavored ice cream
before chemotherapy.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
Biological Predispositions (continued)
• The nausea caused by the treatment
resulted in a taste aversion to the ice
cream.
• Researchers found that when an unusual or
unfamiliar food becomes the “scapegoat,” or
target for a taste aversion, other foods in
the patient's diet may be protected and the
patient will continue to eat them regularly.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
Classical Conditioning in Everyday Life
• Research suggests that the inability to acquire
classically conditioned responses may be the
first sign of Alzheimer’s disease, a sign that
appears prior to any memory loss.
• Through classical conditioning, environmental
cues associated with drug use can become
conditioned stimuli and later produce the
conditioned responses of drug craving.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
Classical Conditioning in Everyday Life
(continued)
• Research indicates that even the immune
system is subject to classical conditioning.
• In the mid-1970s Robert Ader was
conducting an experiment with rats,
conditioning them to avoid saccharin-
sweetened water.
• After drinking the sweet water, the rats
were injected with a tasteless drug that
causes severe nausea.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
Classical Conditioning in Everyday Life
(continued)
• The conditioning worked, and the rats would
not drink sweet water.
• Attempting to reverse the conditioned response,
Ader had to force-feed the sweet water to the
rats for many days; many of them died.
• Ader learned that the tasteless drug suppresses
the immune system.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
Classical conditioning in everyday life
(continued)
• A few doses of an immune-suppressing
drug paired with sweetened water had
produced a conditioned response.
• As a result, the sweet water alone continued
to suppress the immune system, causing
the rats to die.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
Classical Conditioning in Everyday Life
(continued)
• Bovbjerg and others found that in some cancer
patients undergoing chemotherapy, environmental
cues in the treatment setting eventually came to
elicit nausea and immune suppression.
• Other researchers showed that classical
conditioning could be used to suppress the immune
system in order to prolong the survival of mice
heart tissue transplants.
• Classically conditioned stimuli can also be used to
boost the immune system.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
Neurological Basis of Classical Conditioning
• An intact amygdala is required for the
conditioning of fear in both humans and
animals, and context fear conditioning also
depends on the hippocampus.
• Research clearly indicates that the cerebellum
is the essential brain structure for motor
(movement) conditioning and also the storage
site for the memory traces formed during such
conditioning.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Classical Conditioning:
The Contemporary View
Factors Influencing Classical Conditioning
• How reliably the conditioned stimulus
predicts the unconditioned stimulus
• The number of pairings of the conditioned
stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus
• The intensity of the unconditioned stimulus
• The temporal relationship between the
conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned
stimulus
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Thorndike and the Law of Effect
• Edward Thorndike believed trial-and-
error learning was the basis of most
behavioral changes.
• Trial-and-error learning: learning
that occurs when a response is
associated with a successful solution
to a problem after a number of
unsuccessful responses.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Thorndike and the Law of Effect (continued)
• Law of effect: Thorndike’s law of learning,
which states that the consequence, or effect,
of a response will determine whether the
tendency to respond in the same way in the
future will be strengthened or weakened.
• In Thorndike’s best-known experiments, a
hungry cat was placed in a wooden box with
slats, which was called a puzzle box.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Thorndike and the Law of Effect (continued)
• The box was designed so that the animal had
to manipulate a simple mechanism – pressing
a pedal or pulling down a loop – to escape and
claim a food reward that lay just outside the
box.
• After many trials, the cat learned to open the
door almost immediately after being placed in
the box.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
B.F. Skinner: A Pioneer in Operant
Conditioning
• Skinner believed that the causes of
behavior are in the environment and do not
result from inner mental events, such as
thoughts, feelings, or perceptions.
• He claimed that these inner mental events
are themselves behaviors and, like any
other behaviors, are shaped and
determined by environmental forces.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
The Process of Operant Conditioning
• Operant conditioning: a type of learning in
which the consequences of behavior are
manipulated in order to increase or
decrease the frequency of an existing
response or to shape an entirely new
response.
• Reinforcer: anything that strengthens or
increases the probability of the response
that it follows.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Shaping Behavior
• Shaping: an operant conditioning technique
that consists of gradually molding a desired
behavior (response) by reinforcing any
movement in the direction of the desired
response, thereby gradually guiding the
responses toward the ultimate goal.
• B.F. Skinner demonstrated that shaping is
particularly effective in conditioning complex
behaviors.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Shaping Behavior (continued)
• Skinner box: a soundproof apparatus
with a device for delivering food to an
animal subject; designed by Skinner.
• Successive approximations: a
series of gradual steps, each of which
is more like the final desired response.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Superstitious Behavior
• Superstitious behavior occurs if an
individual falsely believes that a
connection exists between an act and
its consequences.
• It is not confined to humans.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Superstitious Behavior (continued)
• Skinner developed superstitious behavior in
pigeons by giving food rewards every 15
seconds regardless of the pigeons’ behavior.
• Whatever response the pigeons happened to be
making was reinforced, and before long each
pigeon developed its own ritual, such as turning
counterclockwise in the cage several times or
making pendulum movements with its head.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Extinction
• Extinction occurs when reinforcers are
withheld and the conditioned response
weakens and eventually disappears.
• In humans and other animals, the
withholding of reinforcement can lead to
frustration or even rage.
• Spontaneous recovery also occurs in
operant conditioning.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Generalization and Discrimination
• Skinner found that generalization occurs in
operant conditioning, just as in classical
conditioning.
• Discrimination in operant conditioning involves
learning to distinguish between a stimulus that
has been reinforced and other stimuli that may
be very similar.
• Discriminative stimulus: a stimulus that
signals whether a certain response or behavior
is likely to be rewarded, ignored, or punished.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Reinforcement
Positive and Negative Reinforcement
• Reinforcement: any event that follows a
response and strengthens or increases the
probability of the response being repeated.
• Positive reinforcement: any pleasant or
desirable consequence that follows a
response and increases the probability that
the response will be repeated.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Positive and Negative Reinforcement
(continued)
• Negative reinforcement: a person’s or
animal’s behavior is reinforced by the
termination or avoidance of an unpleasant
condition.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Primary and Secondary Reinforcers
• Primary reinforcer: a reinforcer that
fulfills a basic physical need for
survival and does not depend on
learning.
• Food, water, sleep and termination of
pain are examples of primary
reinforcers.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Primary and Secondary Reinforcers
(continued)
• Secondary reinforcer: a reinforcer that is
acquired or learned through association with
other reinforcers.
• Some secondary reinforcers (money, for
example) can be exchanged at a later time for
other reinforcers.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Schedules of Reinforcement
• Continuous reinforcement: reinforcement
that is administered after every desired or
correct response; the most effective method
of conditioning a new response.
• Partial reinforcement: a pattern of
reinforcement in which some, but not all,
correct responses are reinforced.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Schedules of Reinforcement (continued)
• Partial reinforcement may be
administered according to any of several
types of schedules of reinforcement.
• Different schedules produce distinct rates
and patterns of responses, as well as
varying degrees of resistance to
extinction when reinforcement is
discontinued.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Schedules of Reinforcement
(continued)
• Ratio schedules: require that a
certain number of responses be made
before one of the responses is
reinforced.
• Interval schedules: a given amount
of time must pass before a reinforcer
is administered.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Schedules of Reinforcement
(continued)
• Fixed-ratio schedule: A schedule in which
a reinforcer is given after a fixed number of
correct responses.
• The fixed-ratio schedule is a very effective
way to maintain a high response rate,
because the number of reinforcers depends
directly on the response rate.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Schedules of Reinforcement (continued)
• Variable-ratio schedule: a schedule in which a
reinforcer is given after a varying number of
nonreinforced responses based on an average
ratio.
• Variable-ratio schedules result in higher, more
stable rates of responding than fixed-ratio
schedules.
• In general, the variable-ratio schedule produces
the highest response rate and the most
resistance to extinction.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Schedules of Reinforcement (continued)
• Fixed-interval schedule: a schedule in which a
specific period of time must pass before a
response is reinforced.
• Reinforcement does not depend on the number
of responses made, only on the one correct
response made after the time interval has
passed.
• Characteristic of the fixed-interval schedule is a
pause or a sharp decline in responding
immediately after each reinforcement and a rapid
acceleration in responding just before the next
reinforcer is due.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Schedules of Reinforcement (continued)
• Variable-interval schedule: a schedule in which
a reinforcer is given after the first correct
response following a varying time of
nonreinforced responses, based on an average
time.
• This schedule maintains remarkably stable and
uniform rates of responding, but the response
rate is typically lower than that of the ratio
schedules, because reinforcement is not tied
directly to the number of responses made.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
The Effect of Continuous and Partial
Reinforcement on Extinction
• On a continuous schedule, a reinforcer is expected
without fail after each correct response; when a
reinforcer is withheld, it is noticed immediately.
• On a partial-reinforcement schedule, a reinforcer is
not expected after every response; no immediate
difference is apparent between the partial-
reinforcement schedule and the onset of extinction.
• Partial reinforcement results in a greater resistance
to extinction than does continuous reinforcement.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
The Effect of Continuous and Partial
Reinforcement on Extinction
(continued)
• There is an inverse relationship between
the percentage of responses that have
been reinforced and resistance to
extinction.
• The lower the percentage of responses
that are reinforced, the longer extinction
will take when reinforcement is withheld.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Factors Influencing Operant
Conditioning
1. The magnitude of reinforcement
2. The immediacy of reinforcement
3. The level of motivation of the
learner
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Comparing Classical and Operant Conditioning
• Processes of generalization, discrimination,
extinction, and spontaneous recovery occur in
both classical and operant conditioning.
• Both types of conditioning depend on associative
learning.
• In classical conditioning, an association is formed
between two stimuli.
• In operant conditioning, the association is
established between a response and its
consequences.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Comparing Classical and Operant
Conditioning
• In classical conditioning, the focus is on what
precedes the response.
• In operant conditioning, the focus is on what
follows the response.
• In classical conditioning, the subject is passive
and responds to the environment rather than
acting on it.
• In operant conditioning, the subject is active
and operates on the environment.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Punishment
• Punishment is the opposite of
reinforcement.
• Punishment can be accomplished by
either adding an unpleasant stimulus or
removing a pleasant stimulus.
• It is common to confuse punishment and
negative reinforcement.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Punishment (continued)
• With punishment, an unpleasant condition
may be added, but with negative
reinforcement, an unpleasant condition is
terminated or avoided.
• The two have opposite effects.
• Unlike punishment, negative reinforcement
increases the probability of a desired
response.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Disadvantages of Punishment
1. According to Skinner, punishment
does not extinguish an undesirable
behavior; rather, it suppresses that
behavior when the punishing agent is
present. But the behavior is apt to
continue when the threat of
punishment is removed and in
settings where punishment is
unlikely.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Disadvantages of Punishment (continued)
2. Punishment indicates that a behavior is
unacceptable, but does not help people
develop more appropriate behaviors. If
punishment is used, it should be
administered in conjunction with
reinforcement or rewards for appropriate
behavior.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Disadvantages of Punishment (continued)
3. The person who is severely punished often
becomes fearful and feels angry and
hostile toward the punisher. These
reactions may be accompanied by a desire
to retaliate or to avoid or escape from the
punisher and the punishing situation.
4. Punishment frequently leads to
aggression. Those who administer
physical punishment may become models
of aggressive behavior.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Alternatives to Punishment
• Many psychologists believe that removing
the rewarding consequences of undesirable
behavior is the best way to extinguish a
problem behavior.
• Using positive reinforcement, such as
praise, will make good behavior more
rewarding for children.
• It is probably unrealistic to believe that
punishment will ever become unnecessary.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Making Punishment More Effective
1. Punishment is most effective when it is
applied during the misbehavior or as soon
afterward as possible. Interrupting the
problem behavior is most effective because
doing so abruptly halts its rewarding aspects.
2. Ideally, punishment should be of the minimum
severity necessary to suppress the problem
behavior. The intensity of the punishment
should match the seriousness of the misdeed.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Making Punishment More Effective
(continued)
3. To be effective, punishment must be
applied consistently. A parent cannot
ignore misbehavior one day and
punish the same act the next.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Culture and Punishment
• Punishment is used in every culture to
control and suppress people’s
behavior.
• U.S. citizens traveling in other
countries need to be aware of how
different cultures view and administer
punishment.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Culture and Punishment (continued)
• Michael Fay, an American living in Singapore,
was arrested and charged with 53 counts of
vandalism.
• He was fined approximately $2,000, sentenced
to 4 months in jail, and received four lashes
with a rattan cane, an agonizingly painful
experience.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Escape and Avoidance Learning
• Escape learning: learning to perform a
behavior because it prevents or terminates
an aversive event.
• Avoidance learning: learning to avoid
events or conditions associated with
aversive consequences or phobias.
• Many avoidance behaviors are maladaptive
and occur in response to phobias.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Escape and Avoidance Learning
• Learned helplessness: a passive
resignation to aversive conditions,
learned by repeated exposure to
aversive events that are inescapable
or unavoidable.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Escape and Avoidance Learning
• Overmeier and Seligman did an experiment
with dogs in which the dogs learned to be
helpless after exposure to inescapable
shocks.
• Seligman reasoned that humans who have
suffered painful experiences they could
neither avoid nor escape may also
experience learned helplessness.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Applications of Operant Conditioning
Shaping the behavior of animals
• The principles of operant conditioning
are used effectively to train animals
not only to perform entertaining tricks,
but also to help physically challenged
people lead more independent lives.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Applications of Operant Conditioning
Shaping the behavior of animals
• When an animal’s instinctual behavior
runs counter to the behavior being
conditioned, the animal will eventually
resume its instinctual behavior, a
phenomenon known as instinctual
drift.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Applications of operant conditioning
(continued)
Biofeedback
• Biofeedback is the use of sensitive equipment
to give people precise feedback about internal
physiological processes so that they can learn,
with practice, to exercise control over them.
• It has been used to regulate heart rate and to
control migraine and tension headaches,
gastrointestinal disorders, asthma, anxiety
tension states, epilepsy, sexual dysfunctions,
and neuromuscular disorders.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Applications of operant conditioning
(continued)
Behavior modification
• Behavior modification is a method of
changing behavior through a systematic
program based on learning principles of
classical conditioning, operant conditioning, or
observational learning.
• Token economy: a program that motivates
socially desirable behavior by reinforcing it with
tokens that can later be exchanged for desired
items or privileges.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Operant Conditioning
Applications of operant conditioning
(continued)
Behavior modification (continued)
• Token economies have been used effectively in
mental hospitals to encourage patients to attend
to grooming, to interact with other patients, and to
carry out housekeeping tasks.
• Many classroom teachers and parents use time
out – a behavior modification technique in which
a child who is misbehaving is removed for a short
time from sources of positive reinforcement.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Cognitive Learning
• Cognitive processes: mental
processes such as thinking, knowing,
problem solving, remembering, and
forming mental representations.
• According to cognitive theorists, these
processes are critically important in a
more complete, more comprehensive
view of learning.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Cognitive Learning
Learning by Insight
• Wolfgang Köhler
• Wrote The Mentality of Apes
• Did experiments on chimpanzees confined
in caged areas
• Observed the chimps’ unsuccessful
attempts to reach a bunch of bananas
inside the caged area that were overhead,
out of reach of the chimps
• Eventually the chimps solved the problem
by piling the boxes one on top of the other
and climbing on the boxes until they could
reach the bananas
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Cognitive Learning
Learning by Insight (continued)
• Insight: the sudden realization of the
relationship between elements in a problem
situation, which makes the solution apparent.
• A solution gained through insight is more
easily learned, less likely to be forgotten, and
more readily transferred to new problems
than a solution learned through rote
memorization.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Cognitive Learning
Latent Learning and Cognitive Maps
• Edward Tolman believed that learning could
take place without reinforcement.
• Latent learning is learning that occurs
without apparent reinforcement, but that is
not demonstrated until the organism is
motivated to do so.
• Cognitive map: a mental representation of
a spatial arrangement, such as a maze.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Cognitive Learning
Observational Learning
• Albert Bandura contends that many
behaviors or responses are acquired
through observational learning, or as he
more often calls it now, social-cognitive
learning.
• Observational learning (sometimes
called modeling): learning by observing
the behavior of others and the
consequences of that behavior; learning by
imitation.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Cognitive Learning
Observational Learning (continued)
• A model is the individual who
demonstrates a behavior or serves as
an example in observational learning.
• The effectiveness of a model is related
to his or her status, competence, and
power.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Cognitive Learning
Observational Learning (continued)
• Recent research has also shown that
observational learning is improved when
several sessions of observation precede
attempts to perform the behavior and are
also repeated in the early stages of
practicing it.
• An observer must also be physically and
cognitively capable of performing the
behavior in order to learn it.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Cognitive Learning
Observational Learning (continued)
• Modeling effect: learning a new behavior from a
model through the acquisition of new responses.
• Elicitation effect: exhibiting a behavior similar to
that of a model in an unfamiliar situation.
• Disinhibitory effect: displaying a previously
suppressed behavior because a model does so
without receiving punishment.
• Inhibitory effect: suppressing a behavior
because a model is punished for displaying the
behavior.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Cognitive Learning
Learning from Television and Other Media
• Albert Bandura
– Suspected that aggression and violence on
television programs tend to increase aggression
in children
– Demonstrated how children are influenced by
exposure to aggressive models
– His research sparked interest in studying the
effects of violence and aggression portrayed in
other entertainment media
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Cognitive Learning
Learning from Television and Other Media
• Recently published longitudinal evidence
shows that the effects of childhood exposure
to violence persist well into the adult years.
• Just as children imitate the aggressive
behavior they observe on television, they also
imitate the prosocial, or helping, behavior
they see there.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Review of Learning Objectives
Classical Conditioning: The Original View
1. What kind of learning did Pavlov discover?
2. How is classical conditioning accomplished?
3. What kinds of changes in stimuli and learning
conditions lead to changes in conditioned
responses?
4. How did Watson demonstrate that fear could
be classically conditioned?
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Review of Learning Objectives
Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary
View
1. According to Rescorla, what is the critical
element in classical conditioning?
2. What did Garcia and Koelling discover about
classical conditioning?
3. What types of everyday responses can be
subject to classical conditioning?
4. Why doesn’t classical conditioning result every
time unconditioned and conditioned stimuli
occur together?
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Review of Learning Objectives
Operant Conditioning
1. What did Thorndike conclude about learning by
watching cats try to escape from his puzzle
box?
2. What was Skinner’s major contribution to
psychology?
3. What is the process by which responses are
acquired through operant conditioning?
4. What is the goal of both positive reinforcement
and negative reinforcement, and how is that
goal accomplished with each?
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Review of Learning Objectives
Operant Conditioning
5. What are the four types of schedules of
reinforcement, and which type is most effective?
6. Why don’t consequences always cause changes in
behavior?
7. How does punishment differ from negative
reinforcement?
8. When is avoidance learning desirable, and when is it
maladaptive?
9. What are some applications of operant conditioning?
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
Review of Learning Objectives
Cognitive Learning
1. What is insight, and how does it affect
learning?
2. What did Tolman discover about the
necessity of reinforcement?
3. How do we learn by observing others?

Más contenido relacionado

Similar a Chapter5woodpowerpoint 090918015012-phpapp01

Classical conditioning theory and social learning theory
Classical conditioning theory and social learning theoryClassical conditioning theory and social learning theory
Classical conditioning theory and social learning theorytarravandana
 
Learning- Classical Conditioning.pptx
Learning- Classical Conditioning.pptxLearning- Classical Conditioning.pptx
Learning- Classical Conditioning.pptxMsMaryamShahzadi
 
Classical Conditioning according to Pavlov and J.b Waston
Classical Conditioning according to Pavlov and J.b Waston Classical Conditioning according to Pavlov and J.b Waston
Classical Conditioning according to Pavlov and J.b Waston UmairaNasim
 
THEORIES OF LEARNING JENCIA.pptx
THEORIES OF LEARNING  JENCIA.pptxTHEORIES OF LEARNING  JENCIA.pptx
THEORIES OF LEARNING JENCIA.pptxWVINITHAVINS
 
New microsoft office power point presentation
New microsoft office power point presentationNew microsoft office power point presentation
New microsoft office power point presentationMD NAFEES
 
Theory Of Learning (Psychology)
Theory Of Learning (Psychology)Theory Of Learning (Psychology)
Theory Of Learning (Psychology)Dr Kaushik Nandy
 
Signature Assignment Part 1,2,3- Pavlov1
Signature Assignment Part 1,2,3- Pavlov1Signature Assignment Part 1,2,3- Pavlov1
Signature Assignment Part 1,2,3- Pavlov1Michelle Lamon-Paredes
 
Pavlov Classical Conditioning.pdf
Pavlov Classical Conditioning.pdfPavlov Classical Conditioning.pdf
Pavlov Classical Conditioning.pdfPratibha Sagar
 
Chapter 2.behavioural.learning.theories (1) http://www.cheapassignmenthelp.co...
Chapter 2.behavioural.learning.theories (1) http://www.cheapassignmenthelp.co...Chapter 2.behavioural.learning.theories (1) http://www.cheapassignmenthelp.co...
Chapter 2.behavioural.learning.theories (1) http://www.cheapassignmenthelp.co...Assignment Help
 
Classical conditioning
Classical conditioningClassical conditioning
Classical conditioningEnu Sambyal
 
Learning in Organisational Behaviour
Learning in Organisational BehaviourLearning in Organisational Behaviour
Learning in Organisational BehaviourPrem Pradeep
 
Learning theories 1
Learning theories 1Learning theories 1
Learning theories 1IAU Dent
 
Classical & Operant Conditioning.pptx
Classical & Operant Conditioning.pptxClassical & Operant Conditioning.pptx
Classical & Operant Conditioning.pptxRaniMartalisa2
 

Similar a Chapter5woodpowerpoint 090918015012-phpapp01 (20)

BEHAVIORISM
BEHAVIORISMBEHAVIORISM
BEHAVIORISM
 
Learning
Learning Learning
Learning
 
Classical conditioning theory and social learning theory
Classical conditioning theory and social learning theoryClassical conditioning theory and social learning theory
Classical conditioning theory and social learning theory
 
Learning theories and curve
Learning theories and curveLearning theories and curve
Learning theories and curve
 
Learning- Classical Conditioning.pptx
Learning- Classical Conditioning.pptxLearning- Classical Conditioning.pptx
Learning- Classical Conditioning.pptx
 
Classical Conditioning according to Pavlov and J.b Waston
Classical Conditioning according to Pavlov and J.b Waston Classical Conditioning according to Pavlov and J.b Waston
Classical Conditioning according to Pavlov and J.b Waston
 
Psychology of Learning
Psychology of LearningPsychology of Learning
Psychology of Learning
 
THEORIES OF LEARNING JENCIA.pptx
THEORIES OF LEARNING  JENCIA.pptxTHEORIES OF LEARNING  JENCIA.pptx
THEORIES OF LEARNING JENCIA.pptx
 
New microsoft office power point presentation
New microsoft office power point presentationNew microsoft office power point presentation
New microsoft office power point presentation
 
Theory Of Learning (Psychology)
Theory Of Learning (Psychology)Theory Of Learning (Psychology)
Theory Of Learning (Psychology)
 
Signature Assignment Part 1,2,3- Pavlov1
Signature Assignment Part 1,2,3- Pavlov1Signature Assignment Part 1,2,3- Pavlov1
Signature Assignment Part 1,2,3- Pavlov1
 
Pavlov Classical Conditioning.pdf
Pavlov Classical Conditioning.pdfPavlov Classical Conditioning.pdf
Pavlov Classical Conditioning.pdf
 
Chapter 2.behavioural.learning.theories (1) http://www.cheapassignmenthelp.co...
Chapter 2.behavioural.learning.theories (1) http://www.cheapassignmenthelp.co...Chapter 2.behavioural.learning.theories (1) http://www.cheapassignmenthelp.co...
Chapter 2.behavioural.learning.theories (1) http://www.cheapassignmenthelp.co...
 
14812 learning
14812 learning14812 learning
14812 learning
 
Ivan pavlov
Ivan pavlovIvan pavlov
Ivan pavlov
 
Classical conditioning
Classical conditioningClassical conditioning
Classical conditioning
 
Learning in Organisational Behaviour
Learning in Organisational BehaviourLearning in Organisational Behaviour
Learning in Organisational Behaviour
 
Learning theories 1
Learning theories 1Learning theories 1
Learning theories 1
 
PSY101 Week 2 Learning
PSY101 Week 2 LearningPSY101 Week 2 Learning
PSY101 Week 2 Learning
 
Classical & Operant Conditioning.pptx
Classical & Operant Conditioning.pptxClassical & Operant Conditioning.pptx
Classical & Operant Conditioning.pptx
 

Último

Group Discussion and panel Discussion
Group Discussion  and   panel DiscussionGroup Discussion  and   panel Discussion
Group Discussion and panel DiscussionAbdulGhaffarGhori
 
Uttoxeter & Cheadle Voice, Issue 122.pdf
Uttoxeter & Cheadle Voice, Issue 122.pdfUttoxeter & Cheadle Voice, Issue 122.pdf
Uttoxeter & Cheadle Voice, Issue 122.pdfNoel Sergeant
 
Masjid Ishaq The Mosque of Babo Dehri Swabi
Masjid Ishaq The Mosque of Babo Dehri SwabiMasjid Ishaq The Mosque of Babo Dehri Swabi
Masjid Ishaq The Mosque of Babo Dehri SwabiAlhamdulillah 33
 
12 Week Weight Loss Planner to help with planning weight loss
12 Week Weight Loss Planner to help with planning weight loss12 Week Weight Loss Planner to help with planning weight loss
12 Week Weight Loss Planner to help with planning weight lossSimpleMoneyMaker
 
FUNDAMENTALS OF ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS
FUNDAMENTALS OF ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS ARNISFUNDAMENTALS OF ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS
FUNDAMENTALS OF ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS ARNISe98298697
 
Living in the Light_ A guide to personal transformation ( PDFDrive ).pdf
Living in the Light_ A guide to personal transformation ( PDFDrive ).pdfLiving in the Light_ A guide to personal transformation ( PDFDrive ).pdf
Living in the Light_ A guide to personal transformation ( PDFDrive ).pdfkalpana413121
 

Último (6)

Group Discussion and panel Discussion
Group Discussion  and   panel DiscussionGroup Discussion  and   panel Discussion
Group Discussion and panel Discussion
 
Uttoxeter & Cheadle Voice, Issue 122.pdf
Uttoxeter & Cheadle Voice, Issue 122.pdfUttoxeter & Cheadle Voice, Issue 122.pdf
Uttoxeter & Cheadle Voice, Issue 122.pdf
 
Masjid Ishaq The Mosque of Babo Dehri Swabi
Masjid Ishaq The Mosque of Babo Dehri SwabiMasjid Ishaq The Mosque of Babo Dehri Swabi
Masjid Ishaq The Mosque of Babo Dehri Swabi
 
12 Week Weight Loss Planner to help with planning weight loss
12 Week Weight Loss Planner to help with planning weight loss12 Week Weight Loss Planner to help with planning weight loss
12 Week Weight Loss Planner to help with planning weight loss
 
FUNDAMENTALS OF ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS
FUNDAMENTALS OF ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS ARNISFUNDAMENTALS OF ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS
FUNDAMENTALS OF ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS ARNIS
 
Living in the Light_ A guide to personal transformation ( PDFDrive ).pdf
Living in the Light_ A guide to personal transformation ( PDFDrive ).pdfLiving in the Light_ A guide to personal transformation ( PDFDrive ).pdf
Living in the Light_ A guide to personal transformation ( PDFDrive ).pdf
 

Chapter5woodpowerpoint 090918015012-phpapp01

  • 1. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Learning Chapter Five This multimedia product and its contents are protected under copyright law. The following are prohibited by law: • Any public performance or display, including transmission of any image over a network; • Preparation of any derivative work, including the extraction, in whole or in part, of any images; • Any rental, lease, or lending of the program. Slide author: Cynthia K. Shinabarger Reed Book authors: Samuel Wood Ellen G. Wood Denise Boyd
  • 2. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Chapter Five Overview Classical Conditioning: The Original View • Pavlov and Classical Conditioning • The Process of Classical Conditioning • Changing Conditioned Responses • John Watson and Emotional Conditioning
  • 3. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Chapter Five Overview Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View • The Cognitive Perspective • Biological Predispositions • Classical Conditioning in Everyday Life • Factors Influencing Classical Conditioning
  • 4. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Chapter Five Overview Operant Conditioning • Thorndike and the Law of Effect • B.F. Skinner: A Pioneer in Operant Conditioning • The Process of Operant Conditioning • Reinforcement • Schedules of Reinforcement
  • 5. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Chapter Five Overview Operant Conditioning (continued) • Factors Influencing Operant Conditioning • Punishment • Escape and Avoidance Learning • Applications of Operant Conditioning
  • 6. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Chapter Five Overview Cognitive Learning • Learning by Insight • Latent Learning and Cognitive Maps • Observational Learning
  • 7. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Learning • A relatively permanent change in behavior, knowledge, capability, or attitude that is acquired through experience and cannot be attributed to illness, injury, or maturation
  • 8. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Original View Classical Conditioning • Classical conditioning is a type of learning through which an organism learns to associate one stimulus with another. • Stimulus (the plural is stimuli): any event or object in the environment to which an organism responds. • Classical conditioning is sometimes referred to as respondent conditioning, or Pavlovian conditioning.
  • 9. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Original View Pavlov and Classical Conditioning • Ivan Pavlov organized and directed research in physiology at the Institute of Experimental Medicine in St. Petersburg, Russia, from 1891 until his death 45 years later. • The Institute of Experimental Medicine is where he conducted his classic experiments on the physiology of digestion, which won him a Nobel Prize in 1904.
  • 10. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Original View Pavlov and Classical Conditioning (continued) • Pavlov conducted a study on dogs where he collected the saliva that the dogs would secrete naturally in response to food placed inside the mouth; he observed saliva collecting when the dogs heard their food dishes rattling, when they heard the laboratory assistants coming to feed them, and when they saw the attendant who fed them.
  • 11. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Original View Pavlov and Classical Conditioning (continued) Ivan Pavlov’s laboratory • The dogs were isolated inside soundproof cubicles and placed in harnesses to restrain their movements. • The experimenter observed the dogs through a one-way mirror. • Food and other stimuli were presented and the flow of saliva measured by remote control.
  • 12. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
  • 13. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Original View The Process of Classical Conditioning • Reflex: an involuntary response to a particular stimulus, such as the eyeblink response to a puff of air or salivation when food is placed in the mouth • Two types of reflexes • Conditioned reflexes (learned): a learned reflex rather than a naturally occurring one. • Unconditioned reflexes (unlearned): inborn, automatic, unlearned response to a particular stimulus.
  • 14. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Original View The Conditioned and Unconditioned Stimulus and Response • Pavlov used tones, bells, buzzers, lights, geometric shapes, electric shocks, and metronomes in his conditioning experiments. • Food powder was placed in the dog’s mouth, causing salivation. • Because dogs do not need to be conditioned to salivate to food, salivation to food is an unlearned response.
  • 15. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Original View The Conditioned and Unconditioned Stimulus and Response (continued) • Unconditioned response (UR): a response that is elicited by an unconditioned stimulus without prior learning. • Unconditioned stimulus (US): any stimulus, such as food, that without prior learning will automatically elicit, or bring forth, an unconditioned response.
  • 16. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Original View The Conditioned and Unconditioned Stimulus and Response (continued) • Pavlov demonstrated that dogs could be conditioned to salivate to a variety of stimuli never before associated with food. • During the conditioning process, the researcher would present a neutral stimulus such as a musical tone shortly before placing the food powder in the dog’s mouth. • Pavlov found that after the tone and the food were paired many times, usually 20 or more, the tone alone would elicit salivation.
  • 17. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Original View The Conditioned and Unconditioned Stimulus and Response (continued) • Conditioned stimulus (CS): a neutral stimulus that, after repeated pairing with an unconditioned stimulus, becomes associated with it and elicits a conditioned response. • Conditioned response (CR): the learned response that comes to be elicited by a conditioned stimulus as a result of its repeated pairing with an unconditioned stimulus.
  • 18. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
  • 19. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Original View Higher-order conditioning • Conditioning that occurs when conditioned stimuli are linked together to form a series of signals
  • 20. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Original View Changing Conditioned Responses • Extinction: in classical conditioning, the weakening and eventual disappearance of a conditioned response as a result of repeated presentation of the conditioned stimulus without the unconditioned stimulus. • Spontaneous recovery: the reappearance of an extinguished response (in a weaker form) when an organism is exposed to the original conditioned stimulus following a rest period.
  • 21. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Original View Changing Conditioned Responses (continued) • When a conditioned response is extinguished in one setting, it can still be elicited in other settings where extinction training has not occurred. • Pavlov found that a tone similar to the original conditioned stimulus would produce the conditioned response (salivation). • Generalization: in classical conditioning, the tendency to make a conditioned response to a stimulus similar to the original conditioned stimulus.
  • 22. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Original View Changing Conditioned Responses (continued) • Discrimination: the learned ability to distinguish between similar stimuli so that the conditioned response occurs only to the original conditioned stimulus, but not to similar stimuli. • Generalization and discrimination have survival value. • Discriminating between a rattlesnake and a garter snake could save your life.
  • 23. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Original View John Watson and Emotional Conditioning • John Watson and his assistant, Rosalie Rayner, conducted a study to prove that fear could be classically conditioned. • The subject of the study, known as Little Albert, was a healthy and emotionally stable 11-month-old infant. • Little Albert showed no fear except of the loud noise Watson made by striking a hammer against a steel bar near Albert’s head.
  • 24. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Original View John Watson and Emotional Conditioning (continued) • Rayner presented Little Albert with a white rat; as Albert reached for the rat, Watson struck the steel bar with a hammer. • This procedure was repeated several times. • This procedure caused Albert to begin to cry at the sight of a rat.
  • 25. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006
  • 26. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Original View John Watson and Emotional Conditioning (continued) • Watson also had ideas for removing fears and laid the groundwork for some behavior therapies used today. • Watson and a colleague, Mary Cover Jones, found 3-year-old Peter, who was afraid of rabbits, and tried Watson's fear-removal techniques on him.
  • 27. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Original View John Watson and Emotional Conditioning (continued) • Peter was put in a high chair and given candy while a rabbit was in a cage at a safe distance from him. • The rabbit was moved closer with each session and eventually placed in Peter’s lap. • By the final session, Peter had grown fond of the rabbit.
  • 28. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View The Cognitive Perspective • Robert Rescorla is largely responsible for changing how psychologists view classical conditioning. • Rescorla demonstrated that the critical element in classical conditioning is not the repeated pairing of the conditioned stimulus and unconditioned stimulus. • Rather, the important factor is whether the conditioned stimulus provides information that enables the organism to reliably predict the occurrence of the unconditioned stimulus.
  • 29. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View The Cognitive Perspective (continued) • Using rats as his subjects, Rescorla used a tone as the conditioned stimulus and a shock as the unconditioned stimulus. • For one group of rats, the tone and shock were paired 20 times – the shock always occurred during the tone. • The other group of rats also received a shock 20 times while the tone was sounding, but this group also received 20 shocks that were not paired with the tone.
  • 30. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View The Cognitive Perspective (continued) • Only the first group, for which the tone was a reliable predictor of the shock, developed a conditioned fear response to the tone. • The second group showed little evidence of conditioning, because the shock was just as likely to occur without the tone as with it.
  • 31. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View Biological Predispositions • Research has shown that humans are more easily conditioned to fear stimuli, such as snakes, that can have very real negative effects on their well-being • Martin Seligman said that most common fears “are related to the survival of the human species through the long course of evolution.” • He suggested that humans and other animals are prepared to associate only certain stimuli with particular consequences.
  • 32. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View Biological Predispositions (continued) • Taste aversions: the intense dislike and/or avoidance of particular foods that have been associated with nausea or discomfort. • Taste aversions can be classically conditioned when the delay between the conditioned stimulus (food or drink) and the unconditioned stimulus (nausea) is as long as 12 hours.
  • 33. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View Biological Predispositions (continued) • Garcia and Koelling exposed rats to a three- way conditioned stimulus: a bright light, a clicking noise, and flavored water. • For one group of rats, the unconditioned stimulus was being exposed to X-rays or lithium chloride, either of which produces nausea and vomiting several hours after exposure; for the other group, the unconditioned stimulus was an electric shock to the feet.
  • 34. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View Biological Predispositions (continued) • The rats in one group associated nausea only with the flavored water; those in the other group associated electric shock only with the light and the sound.
  • 35. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View Biological Predispositions (continued) • Garcia and Koelling’s research established two exceptions to traditional ideas of classical conditioning. – First, the finding that rats formed an association between nausea and flavored water ingested several hours earlier contradicted the principle that the conditioned stimulus must be presented shortly before the unconditioned stimulus.
  • 36. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View Biological Predispositions (continued) – Second, the finding that rats associated electric shock only with noise and light and nausea only with flavored water revealed that animals are apparently biologically predisposed to make certain associations and that associations cannot be readily conditioned between just any two stimuli.
  • 37. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View Biological Predispositions (continued) • Gustavson and others used taste aversion conditioning to stop wild coyotes from attacking lambs in the western United States. • They set out lamb flesh laced with lithium chloride, a poison that made the coyotes extremely ill, but was not fatal. • After only one or two experiences, the coyotes would get sick even at the sight of a lamb.
  • 38. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View Biological Predispositions (continued) • Bernstein and others devised a technique to help cancer patients avoid developing aversions to desirable foods. • A group of cancer patients were given a novel-tasting, maple-flavored ice cream before chemotherapy.
  • 39. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View Biological Predispositions (continued) • The nausea caused by the treatment resulted in a taste aversion to the ice cream. • Researchers found that when an unusual or unfamiliar food becomes the “scapegoat,” or target for a taste aversion, other foods in the patient's diet may be protected and the patient will continue to eat them regularly.
  • 40. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View Classical Conditioning in Everyday Life • Research suggests that the inability to acquire classically conditioned responses may be the first sign of Alzheimer’s disease, a sign that appears prior to any memory loss. • Through classical conditioning, environmental cues associated with drug use can become conditioned stimuli and later produce the conditioned responses of drug craving.
  • 41. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View Classical Conditioning in Everyday Life (continued) • Research indicates that even the immune system is subject to classical conditioning. • In the mid-1970s Robert Ader was conducting an experiment with rats, conditioning them to avoid saccharin- sweetened water. • After drinking the sweet water, the rats were injected with a tasteless drug that causes severe nausea.
  • 42. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View Classical Conditioning in Everyday Life (continued) • The conditioning worked, and the rats would not drink sweet water. • Attempting to reverse the conditioned response, Ader had to force-feed the sweet water to the rats for many days; many of them died. • Ader learned that the tasteless drug suppresses the immune system.
  • 43. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View Classical conditioning in everyday life (continued) • A few doses of an immune-suppressing drug paired with sweetened water had produced a conditioned response. • As a result, the sweet water alone continued to suppress the immune system, causing the rats to die.
  • 44. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View Classical Conditioning in Everyday Life (continued) • Bovbjerg and others found that in some cancer patients undergoing chemotherapy, environmental cues in the treatment setting eventually came to elicit nausea and immune suppression. • Other researchers showed that classical conditioning could be used to suppress the immune system in order to prolong the survival of mice heart tissue transplants. • Classically conditioned stimuli can also be used to boost the immune system.
  • 45. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View Neurological Basis of Classical Conditioning • An intact amygdala is required for the conditioning of fear in both humans and animals, and context fear conditioning also depends on the hippocampus. • Research clearly indicates that the cerebellum is the essential brain structure for motor (movement) conditioning and also the storage site for the memory traces formed during such conditioning.
  • 46. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View Factors Influencing Classical Conditioning • How reliably the conditioned stimulus predicts the unconditioned stimulus • The number of pairings of the conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus • The intensity of the unconditioned stimulus • The temporal relationship between the conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus
  • 47. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Thorndike and the Law of Effect • Edward Thorndike believed trial-and- error learning was the basis of most behavioral changes. • Trial-and-error learning: learning that occurs when a response is associated with a successful solution to a problem after a number of unsuccessful responses.
  • 48. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Thorndike and the Law of Effect (continued) • Law of effect: Thorndike’s law of learning, which states that the consequence, or effect, of a response will determine whether the tendency to respond in the same way in the future will be strengthened or weakened. • In Thorndike’s best-known experiments, a hungry cat was placed in a wooden box with slats, which was called a puzzle box.
  • 49. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Thorndike and the Law of Effect (continued) • The box was designed so that the animal had to manipulate a simple mechanism – pressing a pedal or pulling down a loop – to escape and claim a food reward that lay just outside the box. • After many trials, the cat learned to open the door almost immediately after being placed in the box.
  • 50. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning B.F. Skinner: A Pioneer in Operant Conditioning • Skinner believed that the causes of behavior are in the environment and do not result from inner mental events, such as thoughts, feelings, or perceptions. • He claimed that these inner mental events are themselves behaviors and, like any other behaviors, are shaped and determined by environmental forces.
  • 51. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning The Process of Operant Conditioning • Operant conditioning: a type of learning in which the consequences of behavior are manipulated in order to increase or decrease the frequency of an existing response or to shape an entirely new response. • Reinforcer: anything that strengthens or increases the probability of the response that it follows.
  • 52. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Shaping Behavior • Shaping: an operant conditioning technique that consists of gradually molding a desired behavior (response) by reinforcing any movement in the direction of the desired response, thereby gradually guiding the responses toward the ultimate goal. • B.F. Skinner demonstrated that shaping is particularly effective in conditioning complex behaviors.
  • 53. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Shaping Behavior (continued) • Skinner box: a soundproof apparatus with a device for delivering food to an animal subject; designed by Skinner. • Successive approximations: a series of gradual steps, each of which is more like the final desired response.
  • 54. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Superstitious Behavior • Superstitious behavior occurs if an individual falsely believes that a connection exists between an act and its consequences. • It is not confined to humans.
  • 55. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Superstitious Behavior (continued) • Skinner developed superstitious behavior in pigeons by giving food rewards every 15 seconds regardless of the pigeons’ behavior. • Whatever response the pigeons happened to be making was reinforced, and before long each pigeon developed its own ritual, such as turning counterclockwise in the cage several times or making pendulum movements with its head.
  • 56. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Extinction • Extinction occurs when reinforcers are withheld and the conditioned response weakens and eventually disappears. • In humans and other animals, the withholding of reinforcement can lead to frustration or even rage. • Spontaneous recovery also occurs in operant conditioning.
  • 57. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Generalization and Discrimination • Skinner found that generalization occurs in operant conditioning, just as in classical conditioning. • Discrimination in operant conditioning involves learning to distinguish between a stimulus that has been reinforced and other stimuli that may be very similar. • Discriminative stimulus: a stimulus that signals whether a certain response or behavior is likely to be rewarded, ignored, or punished.
  • 58. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Reinforcement Positive and Negative Reinforcement • Reinforcement: any event that follows a response and strengthens or increases the probability of the response being repeated. • Positive reinforcement: any pleasant or desirable consequence that follows a response and increases the probability that the response will be repeated.
  • 59. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Positive and Negative Reinforcement (continued) • Negative reinforcement: a person’s or animal’s behavior is reinforced by the termination or avoidance of an unpleasant condition.
  • 60. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Primary and Secondary Reinforcers • Primary reinforcer: a reinforcer that fulfills a basic physical need for survival and does not depend on learning. • Food, water, sleep and termination of pain are examples of primary reinforcers.
  • 61. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Primary and Secondary Reinforcers (continued) • Secondary reinforcer: a reinforcer that is acquired or learned through association with other reinforcers. • Some secondary reinforcers (money, for example) can be exchanged at a later time for other reinforcers.
  • 62. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Schedules of Reinforcement • Continuous reinforcement: reinforcement that is administered after every desired or correct response; the most effective method of conditioning a new response. • Partial reinforcement: a pattern of reinforcement in which some, but not all, correct responses are reinforced.
  • 63. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Schedules of Reinforcement (continued) • Partial reinforcement may be administered according to any of several types of schedules of reinforcement. • Different schedules produce distinct rates and patterns of responses, as well as varying degrees of resistance to extinction when reinforcement is discontinued.
  • 64. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Schedules of Reinforcement (continued) • Ratio schedules: require that a certain number of responses be made before one of the responses is reinforced. • Interval schedules: a given amount of time must pass before a reinforcer is administered.
  • 65. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Schedules of Reinforcement (continued) • Fixed-ratio schedule: A schedule in which a reinforcer is given after a fixed number of correct responses. • The fixed-ratio schedule is a very effective way to maintain a high response rate, because the number of reinforcers depends directly on the response rate.
  • 66. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Schedules of Reinforcement (continued) • Variable-ratio schedule: a schedule in which a reinforcer is given after a varying number of nonreinforced responses based on an average ratio. • Variable-ratio schedules result in higher, more stable rates of responding than fixed-ratio schedules. • In general, the variable-ratio schedule produces the highest response rate and the most resistance to extinction.
  • 67. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Schedules of Reinforcement (continued) • Fixed-interval schedule: a schedule in which a specific period of time must pass before a response is reinforced. • Reinforcement does not depend on the number of responses made, only on the one correct response made after the time interval has passed. • Characteristic of the fixed-interval schedule is a pause or a sharp decline in responding immediately after each reinforcement and a rapid acceleration in responding just before the next reinforcer is due.
  • 68. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Schedules of Reinforcement (continued) • Variable-interval schedule: a schedule in which a reinforcer is given after the first correct response following a varying time of nonreinforced responses, based on an average time. • This schedule maintains remarkably stable and uniform rates of responding, but the response rate is typically lower than that of the ratio schedules, because reinforcement is not tied directly to the number of responses made.
  • 69. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning The Effect of Continuous and Partial Reinforcement on Extinction • On a continuous schedule, a reinforcer is expected without fail after each correct response; when a reinforcer is withheld, it is noticed immediately. • On a partial-reinforcement schedule, a reinforcer is not expected after every response; no immediate difference is apparent between the partial- reinforcement schedule and the onset of extinction. • Partial reinforcement results in a greater resistance to extinction than does continuous reinforcement.
  • 70. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning The Effect of Continuous and Partial Reinforcement on Extinction (continued) • There is an inverse relationship between the percentage of responses that have been reinforced and resistance to extinction. • The lower the percentage of responses that are reinforced, the longer extinction will take when reinforcement is withheld.
  • 71. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Factors Influencing Operant Conditioning 1. The magnitude of reinforcement 2. The immediacy of reinforcement 3. The level of motivation of the learner
  • 72. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Comparing Classical and Operant Conditioning • Processes of generalization, discrimination, extinction, and spontaneous recovery occur in both classical and operant conditioning. • Both types of conditioning depend on associative learning. • In classical conditioning, an association is formed between two stimuli. • In operant conditioning, the association is established between a response and its consequences.
  • 73. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Comparing Classical and Operant Conditioning • In classical conditioning, the focus is on what precedes the response. • In operant conditioning, the focus is on what follows the response. • In classical conditioning, the subject is passive and responds to the environment rather than acting on it. • In operant conditioning, the subject is active and operates on the environment.
  • 74. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Punishment • Punishment is the opposite of reinforcement. • Punishment can be accomplished by either adding an unpleasant stimulus or removing a pleasant stimulus. • It is common to confuse punishment and negative reinforcement.
  • 75. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Punishment (continued) • With punishment, an unpleasant condition may be added, but with negative reinforcement, an unpleasant condition is terminated or avoided. • The two have opposite effects. • Unlike punishment, negative reinforcement increases the probability of a desired response.
  • 76. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Disadvantages of Punishment 1. According to Skinner, punishment does not extinguish an undesirable behavior; rather, it suppresses that behavior when the punishing agent is present. But the behavior is apt to continue when the threat of punishment is removed and in settings where punishment is unlikely.
  • 77. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Disadvantages of Punishment (continued) 2. Punishment indicates that a behavior is unacceptable, but does not help people develop more appropriate behaviors. If punishment is used, it should be administered in conjunction with reinforcement or rewards for appropriate behavior.
  • 78. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Disadvantages of Punishment (continued) 3. The person who is severely punished often becomes fearful and feels angry and hostile toward the punisher. These reactions may be accompanied by a desire to retaliate or to avoid or escape from the punisher and the punishing situation. 4. Punishment frequently leads to aggression. Those who administer physical punishment may become models of aggressive behavior.
  • 79. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Alternatives to Punishment • Many psychologists believe that removing the rewarding consequences of undesirable behavior is the best way to extinguish a problem behavior. • Using positive reinforcement, such as praise, will make good behavior more rewarding for children. • It is probably unrealistic to believe that punishment will ever become unnecessary.
  • 80. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Making Punishment More Effective 1. Punishment is most effective when it is applied during the misbehavior or as soon afterward as possible. Interrupting the problem behavior is most effective because doing so abruptly halts its rewarding aspects. 2. Ideally, punishment should be of the minimum severity necessary to suppress the problem behavior. The intensity of the punishment should match the seriousness of the misdeed.
  • 81. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Making Punishment More Effective (continued) 3. To be effective, punishment must be applied consistently. A parent cannot ignore misbehavior one day and punish the same act the next.
  • 82. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Culture and Punishment • Punishment is used in every culture to control and suppress people’s behavior. • U.S. citizens traveling in other countries need to be aware of how different cultures view and administer punishment.
  • 83. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Culture and Punishment (continued) • Michael Fay, an American living in Singapore, was arrested and charged with 53 counts of vandalism. • He was fined approximately $2,000, sentenced to 4 months in jail, and received four lashes with a rattan cane, an agonizingly painful experience.
  • 84. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Escape and Avoidance Learning • Escape learning: learning to perform a behavior because it prevents or terminates an aversive event. • Avoidance learning: learning to avoid events or conditions associated with aversive consequences or phobias. • Many avoidance behaviors are maladaptive and occur in response to phobias.
  • 85. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Escape and Avoidance Learning • Learned helplessness: a passive resignation to aversive conditions, learned by repeated exposure to aversive events that are inescapable or unavoidable.
  • 86. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Escape and Avoidance Learning • Overmeier and Seligman did an experiment with dogs in which the dogs learned to be helpless after exposure to inescapable shocks. • Seligman reasoned that humans who have suffered painful experiences they could neither avoid nor escape may also experience learned helplessness.
  • 87. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Applications of Operant Conditioning Shaping the behavior of animals • The principles of operant conditioning are used effectively to train animals not only to perform entertaining tricks, but also to help physically challenged people lead more independent lives.
  • 88. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Applications of Operant Conditioning Shaping the behavior of animals • When an animal’s instinctual behavior runs counter to the behavior being conditioned, the animal will eventually resume its instinctual behavior, a phenomenon known as instinctual drift.
  • 89. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Applications of operant conditioning (continued) Biofeedback • Biofeedback is the use of sensitive equipment to give people precise feedback about internal physiological processes so that they can learn, with practice, to exercise control over them. • It has been used to regulate heart rate and to control migraine and tension headaches, gastrointestinal disorders, asthma, anxiety tension states, epilepsy, sexual dysfunctions, and neuromuscular disorders.
  • 90. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Applications of operant conditioning (continued) Behavior modification • Behavior modification is a method of changing behavior through a systematic program based on learning principles of classical conditioning, operant conditioning, or observational learning. • Token economy: a program that motivates socially desirable behavior by reinforcing it with tokens that can later be exchanged for desired items or privileges.
  • 91. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Operant Conditioning Applications of operant conditioning (continued) Behavior modification (continued) • Token economies have been used effectively in mental hospitals to encourage patients to attend to grooming, to interact with other patients, and to carry out housekeeping tasks. • Many classroom teachers and parents use time out – a behavior modification technique in which a child who is misbehaving is removed for a short time from sources of positive reinforcement.
  • 92. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Cognitive Learning • Cognitive processes: mental processes such as thinking, knowing, problem solving, remembering, and forming mental representations. • According to cognitive theorists, these processes are critically important in a more complete, more comprehensive view of learning.
  • 93. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Cognitive Learning Learning by Insight • Wolfgang Köhler • Wrote The Mentality of Apes • Did experiments on chimpanzees confined in caged areas • Observed the chimps’ unsuccessful attempts to reach a bunch of bananas inside the caged area that were overhead, out of reach of the chimps • Eventually the chimps solved the problem by piling the boxes one on top of the other and climbing on the boxes until they could reach the bananas
  • 94. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Cognitive Learning Learning by Insight (continued) • Insight: the sudden realization of the relationship between elements in a problem situation, which makes the solution apparent. • A solution gained through insight is more easily learned, less likely to be forgotten, and more readily transferred to new problems than a solution learned through rote memorization.
  • 95. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Cognitive Learning Latent Learning and Cognitive Maps • Edward Tolman believed that learning could take place without reinforcement. • Latent learning is learning that occurs without apparent reinforcement, but that is not demonstrated until the organism is motivated to do so. • Cognitive map: a mental representation of a spatial arrangement, such as a maze.
  • 96. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Cognitive Learning Observational Learning • Albert Bandura contends that many behaviors or responses are acquired through observational learning, or as he more often calls it now, social-cognitive learning. • Observational learning (sometimes called modeling): learning by observing the behavior of others and the consequences of that behavior; learning by imitation.
  • 97. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Cognitive Learning Observational Learning (continued) • A model is the individual who demonstrates a behavior or serves as an example in observational learning. • The effectiveness of a model is related to his or her status, competence, and power.
  • 98. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Cognitive Learning Observational Learning (continued) • Recent research has also shown that observational learning is improved when several sessions of observation precede attempts to perform the behavior and are also repeated in the early stages of practicing it. • An observer must also be physically and cognitively capable of performing the behavior in order to learn it.
  • 99. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Cognitive Learning Observational Learning (continued) • Modeling effect: learning a new behavior from a model through the acquisition of new responses. • Elicitation effect: exhibiting a behavior similar to that of a model in an unfamiliar situation. • Disinhibitory effect: displaying a previously suppressed behavior because a model does so without receiving punishment. • Inhibitory effect: suppressing a behavior because a model is punished for displaying the behavior.
  • 100. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Cognitive Learning Learning from Television and Other Media • Albert Bandura – Suspected that aggression and violence on television programs tend to increase aggression in children – Demonstrated how children are influenced by exposure to aggressive models – His research sparked interest in studying the effects of violence and aggression portrayed in other entertainment media
  • 101. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Cognitive Learning Learning from Television and Other Media • Recently published longitudinal evidence shows that the effects of childhood exposure to violence persist well into the adult years. • Just as children imitate the aggressive behavior they observe on television, they also imitate the prosocial, or helping, behavior they see there.
  • 102. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Review of Learning Objectives Classical Conditioning: The Original View 1. What kind of learning did Pavlov discover? 2. How is classical conditioning accomplished? 3. What kinds of changes in stimuli and learning conditions lead to changes in conditioned responses? 4. How did Watson demonstrate that fear could be classically conditioned?
  • 103. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Review of Learning Objectives Classical Conditioning: The Contemporary View 1. According to Rescorla, what is the critical element in classical conditioning? 2. What did Garcia and Koelling discover about classical conditioning? 3. What types of everyday responses can be subject to classical conditioning? 4. Why doesn’t classical conditioning result every time unconditioned and conditioned stimuli occur together?
  • 104. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Review of Learning Objectives Operant Conditioning 1. What did Thorndike conclude about learning by watching cats try to escape from his puzzle box? 2. What was Skinner’s major contribution to psychology? 3. What is the process by which responses are acquired through operant conditioning? 4. What is the goal of both positive reinforcement and negative reinforcement, and how is that goal accomplished with each?
  • 105. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Review of Learning Objectives Operant Conditioning 5. What are the four types of schedules of reinforcement, and which type is most effective? 6. Why don’t consequences always cause changes in behavior? 7. How does punishment differ from negative reinforcement? 8. When is avoidance learning desirable, and when is it maladaptive? 9. What are some applications of operant conditioning?
  • 106. Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2006 Review of Learning Objectives Cognitive Learning 1. What is insight, and how does it affect learning? 2. What did Tolman discover about the necessity of reinforcement? 3. How do we learn by observing others?

Notas del editor

  1. 2 2
  2. 2 2
  3. 2 2
  4. 2 2
  5. 2 2
  6. 2 2
  7. 2 2
  8. 2 2
  9. 2 2
  10. 2 2
  11. 2 2
  12. 2 2
  13. 2 2
  14. 2 2
  15. 2 2
  16. 2 2
  17. 2 2
  18. 2 2
  19. 2 2
  20. 2 2
  21. 2 2
  22. 2 2
  23. 2 2
  24. 2 2
  25. 2 2
  26. 2 2
  27. 2 2
  28. 2 2
  29. 2 2
  30. 2 2
  31. 2 2
  32. 2 2
  33. 2 2
  34. 2 2
  35. 2 2
  36. 2 2
  37. 2 2
  38. 2 2
  39. 2 2
  40. 2 2
  41. 2 2
  42. 2 2
  43. 2 2
  44. 2 2
  45. 2 2
  46. 2 2
  47. 2 2
  48. 2 2
  49. 2 2
  50. 2 2
  51. 2 2
  52. 2 2
  53. 2 2
  54. 2 2
  55. 2 2
  56. 2 2
  57. 2 2
  58. 2 2
  59. 2 2
  60. 2 2
  61. 2 2
  62. 2 2
  63. 2 2
  64. 2 2
  65. 2 2
  66. 2 2
  67. 2 2
  68. 2 2
  69. 2 2
  70. 2 2
  71. 2 2
  72. 2 2
  73. 2 2
  74. 2 2
  75. 2 2
  76. 2 2
  77. 2 2
  78. 2 2