SlideShare una empresa de Scribd logo
1 de 6
Descargar para leer sin conexión
1181
Reprinted from Family Law Journal September, 2013, Vol 43, Pages 1181-1186
DECISION-MAKING WITHIN A CHILD'S TIMEFRAME: A RESPONSE
Harriet Ward, Research Professor and Director of the Centre for Child and Family Research,
Loughborough University
Rebecca Brown, Senior research associate at the Centre for Child and Family Research,
Loughborough University
Decision-making within a Child's Timeframe (R Brown and H Ward, Working Paper 16
(Childhood Wellbeing Research Centre (2012)) is an overview of research evidence, funded
by the Department for Education with support from the Family Justice Council in response to
the Family Justice Review recommendation for consistent training and development for
family justice professionals, including greater emphasis on child development. The overview
summarises the recent research evidence, which shows how abuse and neglect can impact
on early childhood development and considers how timeframes for responses by local
authorities and courts are often inconsistent with those for children. It is not intended as a
summary of evidence to be drawn upon in making judicial decisions but as a resource to
underpin the judicial training programme. To this end, a second edition was published in
2013 with an additional chapter providing training materials.
In the August issue of Family Law at p 1053 Edward Lloyd-Jones summarised a seminar
based on a critique of this overview. At least three versions of this critique, produced for
different audiences, have been widely distributed as unpublished papers: D Wastell and S
White, ‘The child's timeframe: what the neuroscience really says', (unpublished, 2013); S
White, D Wastell, B Featherstone and K Morris, ‘Briefing document: Decision-Making Within
a Child's Timeframe' (R Brown and H Ward, 2013) – an academic analysis, unpublished; S
White and D Wastell, ‘A response to R Brown and H Ward, Decision-Making Within a Child's
Timeframe', (DFE, 2013, unpublished). None of these is published in a peer-reviewed
academic journal where external oversight is offered by an editor or referees.
The critique of Brown and Ward is linked to others produced by this group, the most heavily
publicised being: D Wastell and S White, (2012) ‘Blinded by neuroscience: social policy, the
family and the infant brain', Families, Relationships and Societies, 1(3): 397–414. These
papers, in our opinion, make considerable use of emotive language; however, in this article
we assert and focus on two other issues: their major and systematic misrepresentation of the
research findings and our account of them and also their selective use of evidence to prove
their case that the neuroscience is too weak and too flawed to provide a sufficient basis for
policy and practice. Our aim has been to produce a sound evidence base to support practice
and we do not believe that these papers identify serious flaws in it.
Given the gravity of the claims made, we feel duty bound to respond, as well as we can, to
not always fully explicit assertions about our report. We will first clarify and restate its key
messages and then respond to the specific points captured in Lloyd-Jones' summary. A key
point to note at the outset is that the academic governance of Brown and Ward was
consistent with that required of such research reports: it was overseen by a steering group,
and was then peer-reviewed by three independent academics, all of professorial status and
each with expertise in a relevant field (early years, law and neuroscience). Peer ….
© Jordan Publishing 2013
1182
reviewing was not blind – many academic journals have now discontinued this process in
order to give reviewers an opportunity to declare an interest, which none did. The report
benefitted from their suggestions for change and improvement. Neither the referees nor the
steering group criticised our approach, which they regarded as both balanced and consistent
with current evidence.
WHAT BROWN AND WARD (2012) SAYS AND WHAT IT DOES NOT SAY
The research summarised in Brown and Ward shows that the first 3 years are an important
phase in early childhood, that neurobiological development is shaped by the environment
both before and after birth and that, because infants are so dependent on their caregivers for
survival, a key feature of the environment is the attachment relationship. It also shows how
extreme abuse and neglect in these early years may shape the way in which children
develop in all areas: physically, emotionally, socially and cognitively. Children who
experience extreme abuse and neglect in these years are more likely to fall behind their
peers and develop a wide range of problems in later life. In addition, the longer the
maltreatment continues the more likely it is to have a negative impact on development and
the more difficult will it be to overcome the consequences.
The report does not say that these developments are inevitable or irreversible. It does not
say that all children who experience poor parenting or grow up in poverty will develop
problems. It does not say that children whose development is compromised by abuse and
neglect cannot overcome the consequences. It does not say that parents with problems such
as substance misuse, domestic violence and poor mental health cannot change. It certainly
does not make the claim that courts should remove children from ‘a home environment
where their brains are “shrunken” as a result of abuse and/or neglect' (D Wastell, S White
and A Lorek, ‘The child's timeframe – a neuroscientific perspective' (unpublished, 2013), at p
45). What the report does suggest is that if these children are to remain at home, proactive
engagement with social workers and other professionals needs to begin early.
RESPONSES TO SPECIFIC POINTS IN THE LLOYD-JONES SUMMARY
1. ‘Brown and Ward's report at no point attempts to produce a balanced argument:
whilst it begins with a caveat about the knowledge base, in the body of the report, all
contradictory evidence or any hint of controversy within the neuroscientific field is
ignored.'(p 1054)
We were not commissioned to undertake a systematic scientific review of the research on
early childhood development nor to ascertain the validity of the neurobiological evidence.
Both of these areas have been extensively reviewed and presented in a range of scientific
reports, most of which are included in the 482 published and peer-reviewed papers that were
identified by our search and on which our summary is based.
Furthermore, we were not commissioned to present ‘contradictory' or ‘controversial'
evidence, rather to produce the best possible account of the accepted consensus currently
within the field. Moreover, none of the 482 papers we identified provided any evidence of a
controversy within the field; indeed the only controversy in relation to this issue would appear
to be one being created by White, Wastell and colleagues. They frequently cite Bruer ( The
Myth of the First Three Years, The Free Press, 1999) to support their argument, but this
publication is not a scientific review of the evidence in relation to brain development and
does not provide an overview of research on the consequences for children of abusive
parenting.
2. ‘Despite the display of scientific trappings and references to an impressive volume
of peer-reviewed papers, the neuroscience review relies heavily on a small number of
secondary non peer-reviewed books and reports, some from organisations with an
apparent ideological orientation and campaigning mission.' (p 1054)
We were commissioned to produce a summary of research evidence to be used ….
1183
in training family justice professionals, most of whom will not have had a formal research
background. We did not rely on text books and summary papers to produce this review but we
did draw the readers' attention to a selection of such publications that provide an accessible
(and readable) way into the sometimes complicated literature. Indeed, at the request of the
steering group, we also simplified some of the language of the original draft in order to ensure
that it would be readily understood. Elsewhere Wastell and White (2013) claim that Brown and
Ward (2012) represents ‘a simplified version of the neuroscience' (p 10); that is precisely what
we were commissioned to do. Wastell and White's claim that some of the summary papers
come from campaigning organisations implies that Brown and Ward (2012) have been biased
by an ideological orientation. They focus their critique explicitly on the series of short working
papers produced by the Center on the Developing Child, based at Harvard University. They
state:
‘The Harvard Center synthesises selective samples of neuroscientific studies; although linked
with Harvard University, a cursory visit to the website reveals a campaigning ethos, rather
than a centre for objective science. So it is unsurprising that the Center assembles studies
and produces reports which serve an a priori agenda. The egregious titles of some of its
reports rather give the game away: ‘Building the Brain's Air Traffic Control System' provides
one lurid example. The key players are ubiquitous figures in the early interventionist project;
Shonkoff (its chair) is a veteran campaigner.' (Wastell, White and Lorek, 2013, pp 51–52).
The Center on the Developing Child was established by academics who are experts in the
field of neuroscience. The purpose of the site is to ensure that the findings of a deeply
complex science are disseminated clearly and appropriately to practitioners and policy
makers. The scientists involved are at the top of their field and their research is published in
the most prestigious journals. To describe this site as ‘selective', ‘campaigning', ‘a priori' and
‘lurid' is a travesty; no doubt Harvard will be dealing appropriately with these allegations.
3. ‘The conflation of statistical significance and predictive validity; statistical
significance merely means that an observed result is not a chance finding; it does not
imply the level of predictive power for practical application.' (p 1054)
Ward and Brown (2012) does not conflate statistical significance and predictive validity. Our
report summarises evidence taken from meta-analyses and systematic reviews, both of which
represent the highest level of evidence, primarily because they involve the use of a rigorous
methodology for identifying and synthesising all of the available research findings on a
particular topic. Much of the evidence in these reviews is epidemiological in nature and is
therefore on the whole observational and thereby correlational. Such correlational evidence
does not show causation but it is the best that it is possible to obtain in many areas and forms
the basis, for example, of our understanding about the relationship between poverty and poor
health. The aim of such evidence is not to predict outcomes but to identify consistent patterns
and relationships. Once there is sufficient evidence available from such correlational studies,
they are summarised in the type of meta-analyses and systematic reviews on which we drew,
which is then used for the purpose of policymaking (see M Marmot, Fair Society, Healthy
Lives (The Marmot Review, 2010)).
This is in contrast to the White and Wastell (2012) paper ‘Blinded by neuroscience' in which
the authors have picked just one of the many systematic reviews on the neuroscience (E
McCory, S A De Brito, and E Viding, ‘The link between child abuse and psychopathology: a
review of neurobiological and genetic research' (2012) Journal of the Royal Society of
Medicine, 105:151–6), extracted those elements that support their case and ignored others,
and in so doing have, in our opinion, misrepresented to the reader the overall findings of that
review and the wider evidence. Similarly, they quote the Belsky and de Haan (2011) statement
to support their claim that the knowledge base is insufficiently established: ‘the study …
© Jordan Publishing 2013
1184
of parenting and brain development is not even yet in its infancy; it would be more
appropriate to conclude that it is still in the embryonic stage' (p 409–410). They do not quote
the next sentence, which reads: ‘Nevertheless, as we plan to show, there is emerging
evidence that experience shapes the developing brain in humans.' (p 410); nor the following:
‘[N]ow that so much progress has been made in the first stage of inquiry, it seems
appropriate to conclude that research in this area of inquiry has reached “the end of the
beginning”.' p 423 (J Belsky and M de Haan, ‘Annual research review: parenting and
children's brain development: the end of the beginning' (2011) Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry 52 (4): 409–428).
4. ‘The neuroscientific strand of the argument is a resurrection of the ‘myth of the first
three years', the idea that the brain is highly vulnerable to irreversible damage in the
early years of life. The science, in fact, says quite the opposite.' (p 1054)
The neuroscience evidence that has been undertaken over the past two decades has now
been systematically summarised in a range of highly rigorous and closely scrutinised, peer-
reviewed scientific papers. This evidence shows beyond doubt that severely suboptimal and
abusive parenting that takes place during the first 3 years of life has a disproportionately
large impact on the infant/toddler brain. Although much of the evidence about this particular
period refers to the ‘global neglect' experienced by infants in orphanages, this research
helps us to understand the extensive evidence about the emotional and behavioural
development of very young children in the UK who experience neglect and maltreatment,
including for example, the persistent fear response, hyper-arousal, dissociation and
disorganised attachments that have been widely identified. As far as we are aware, none of
this evidence suggests that the damage that so clearly occurs as a result of abuse during
this period is irreversible. It does, however, indicate that the longer the exposure to
maltreatment, the harder it is to overcome the consequences. It is not, therefore, acceptable
to leave children in environments that we now recognise to have this sort of impact on both
their physical (brain) and emotional (attachment) wellbeing in the hope that something can
be done to reverse it later in their life. Lloyd-Jones alludes at p 1053 to the Munro review, of
which White and Wastell were expert members, as supporting their stance; but the Munro
report states:
‘Neuroscience also offers lessons on the importance of the early years. A recent paper by
the Royal Society (The Royal Society, 2011) on the implications of neuroscience for
education policy, highlights that there are changes in the brain taking place throughout life,
but the number decreases with age. The worst and deepest brain damage occurs before
birth and in the first 18 months of life when the emotional circuits are forming'. (E Munro, The
Munro Review of Child Protection: Final Report – A child-centred system (Department for
Education, 2011), at p 71).
5. ‘The core argument, that maltreatment is intrinsically bad and can cause lasting
damage, is only weakened by this apparent misrepresentation of neuroscience.
Invocation of attachment theory also does not aid the cause – attachment theory is
superfluous to the core argument and its reliability and predictive power are
questionable.' (p 1054)
Like the evidence about the impact of abuse on children's brains, attachment research is
now a highly developed body of knowledge, much of which has again been summarised in
rigorous systematic reviews. These reviews clearly show that the type of ‘disorganised
attachment', which results from abuse during the early years, is strongly associated with
later psychopathology (J Green and R Goldwyn, ‘Annotation: attachment disorganisation and
psychopathology: new findings in attachment research and their potential implications for
developmental psychopathology in childhood', (2002) Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 43 (7): 835–846). This observational evidence is similar to
that for a wide range of health outcomes (eg cigarette smoking where we know that not
every smoker will develop lung cancer but many will suffer seriously as a result of it) about
which we now have very clear policy directives.
1185
6. ‘The repeated claim that emotional abuse is more serious than either physical or
sexual abuse seemed perverse to the professors: where was the evidence for that,
indeed for the existence of such abuse as a well-defined category? The authors seemed
unconcerned about the elevation of such a category to take precedence over other forms
of abuse. Another “slippery but potent” concept was Shonkoff's tautological notion of
“toxic stress”.' (p 1054)
The extensive evidence concerning the existence and consequences of emotional abuse is
summarised by Barlow and Schrader McMillan ( Safeguarding Children from Emotional
Maltreatment(Jessica Kingsley Publishers, 2010), at pp 20–36). Some researchers consider this
to be the most damaging of all forms of maltreatment in early childhood because the perpetrator
is almost always the primary caregiver and their behaviour represents a direct negation of the
child's need for safety, love, belonging and self-esteem (see D Ivaniec, ‘An overview of
emotional maltreatment and failure to thrive' (1997) Child Abuse Review, 6: 370–388). We did
not claim that it is more serious than other forms of abuse throughout childhood, and we did not
recommend that emotional abuse be given precedence over other forms of abuse. And as to
Shonkoff's supposedly tautological notion of ‘toxic stress', the supporting evidence was
published in the highly prestigious Journal of the American Medical Association (JP Shonkoff,
WT Boyce, BS McEwen, ‘Neuroscience, molecular biology and the childhood roots of health
disparities. Building a new framework for health promotion and disease prevention',
(2009) JAMA 301(21): 2252–2259), which we think speaks for itself.
CONCLUSION
Wastell and White's papers are not academic analyses of research data. They claim that Ward
and Brown (2012) is ‘designed to prop up a moral mission' (Wastell and White, 2013, p 10): the
mission being the current government agenda on adoption and the care system which they
apparently view as an attack on the vulnerable and the poor. We too have challenged
government policies that focus on adoption and other forms of care without also strengthening
support for families and addressing the factors that render abuse and therefore separation more
likely (see H Ward, ‘Again the most vulnerable suffer' and ‘Taking children into care can only be
a sticking-plaster solution' The Guardian, 7 September 2012 and 21 November 2012
respectively:http://www.guardian.co.uk/society/2012/sep/07/again-the-most-vulnerable-
suffer; http://www.guardian.co.uk/society/2012/nov/21/taking-children-care-sticking-plaster-
solution). Like our critics we are well aware that current neurobiological research could be
misrepresented to inform policies that may damage families (for instance by feeding into the
political discourse about ‘strivers and skivers'). However, it is not appropriate to attempt to
discredit research because it could be misused in this way.
Lloyd-Jones states at p 1055 that:
‘The permanent removal of a child from his birth family is one of the most draconian actions of
the state with life-long consequences. Surely we as a society have a responsibility to our most
vulnerable children to ensure that such momentous decisions are made in a timely manner on
the basis of the best available evidence'.
The decision of the Family Justice Board to commission the training that is underpinned by our
rigorous overview of the evidence should enable this to happen. In the meantime it is, in our
opinion, incumbent upon White and Wastell to produce a scholarly and evidence-based critique
of the neuroscientific and related research, which would involve them systematically reviewing
the evidence. They also need to provide an alternative explanation for those questions that the
neuroscientific research helps us understand, such as why some children may find it difficult to
control anger and frustration and why some who are placed for adoption may fail to develop
secure attachments with loving parents. Recent research provides conclusive evidence of the
damaging consequences to children's life chances of delayed decisions in cases where
significant harm is an issue (see C Davies and H Ward, Safeguarding Children Across
Services (Jessica Kingsley Publishers, 2012)). It should be of concern to us all that ….
1186
the significance of these findings is being undermined by arguments that we think fail to
engage with the evidence in this field. The potential consequence is that more cases will go
to the Court of Appeal and the timeframe for the resolution of children's cases will be further
extended, to the detriment of their wellbeing.
© Jordan Publishing 2013

Más contenido relacionado

Más de Sir Martin Narey

Icpa forget the bogus cures. just make prisons decent
Icpa forget the bogus cures. just make prisons decentIcpa forget the bogus cures. just make prisons decent
Icpa forget the bogus cures. just make prisons decentSir Martin Narey
 
Forget about rehabilitation, concentrate on making prisons decent.
Forget about rehabilitation, concentrate on making prisons decent.Forget about rehabilitation, concentrate on making prisons decent.
Forget about rehabilitation, concentrate on making prisons decent.Sir Martin Narey
 
Joining the prison service brief reflections
Joining the prison service brief reflectionsJoining the prison service brief reflections
Joining the prison service brief reflectionsSir Martin Narey
 
Extracts about HCPC from Narey report on Sw education
Extracts about HCPC from Narey report on Sw educationExtracts about HCPC from Narey report on Sw education
Extracts about HCPC from Narey report on Sw educationSir Martin Narey
 
Pcc, guardian and medomsley
Pcc, guardian and medomsleyPcc, guardian and medomsley
Pcc, guardian and medomsleySir Martin Narey
 
Adoption: Resignation as Chair of the Leadership Board
Adoption: Resignation as Chair of the Leadership BoardAdoption: Resignation as Chair of the Leadership Board
Adoption: Resignation as Chair of the Leadership BoardSir Martin Narey
 
Rainsbrook report for publication
Rainsbrook report for publicationRainsbrook report for publication
Rainsbrook report for publicationSir Martin Narey
 
Adoption and my ADCS speech
Adoption and my ADCS speechAdoption and my ADCS speech
Adoption and my ADCS speechSir Martin Narey
 
2903652 rotherham response_acc2
2903652 rotherham response_acc22903652 rotherham response_acc2
2903652 rotherham response_acc2Sir Martin Narey
 
Need we panic about the fall in placement orders?
Need we panic about the fall in placement orders?Need we panic about the fall in placement orders?
Need we panic about the fall in placement orders?Sir Martin Narey
 
Adoption muddle is costing children their futures
Adoption muddle is costing children their futuresAdoption muddle is costing children their futures
Adoption muddle is costing children their futuresSir Martin Narey
 
Adoption luckock and broadhurst
Adoption luckock and broadhurstAdoption luckock and broadhurst
Adoption luckock and broadhurstSir Martin Narey
 
The case for adoption in a nutshell from Sir James Munby, December 2014
The case for adoption in a nutshell from Sir James Munby, December 2014The case for adoption in a nutshell from Sir James Munby, December 2014
The case for adoption in a nutshell from Sir James Munby, December 2014Sir Martin Narey
 
Munby judgement on adoption: December 2014
Munby judgement on adoption: December 2014Munby judgement on adoption: December 2014
Munby judgement on adoption: December 2014Sir Martin Narey
 
Adoption cypn nov 2014 (1)
Adoption cypn nov 2014 (1)Adoption cypn nov 2014 (1)
Adoption cypn nov 2014 (1)Sir Martin Narey
 
Alb impact of court judgments on adoption - november 2014 (1) (1)
Alb   impact of court judgments on adoption - november 2014 (1) (1)Alb   impact of court judgments on adoption - november 2014 (1) (1)
Alb impact of court judgments on adoption - november 2014 (1) (1)Sir Martin Narey
 
Mythbuster op ed final mon 10 nov
Mythbuster op ed final mon 10 novMythbuster op ed final mon 10 nov
Mythbuster op ed final mon 10 novSir Martin Narey
 

Más de Sir Martin Narey (20)

Icpa forget the bogus cures. just make prisons decent
Icpa forget the bogus cures. just make prisons decentIcpa forget the bogus cures. just make prisons decent
Icpa forget the bogus cures. just make prisons decent
 
Forget about rehabilitation, concentrate on making prisons decent.
Forget about rehabilitation, concentrate on making prisons decent.Forget about rehabilitation, concentrate on making prisons decent.
Forget about rehabilitation, concentrate on making prisons decent.
 
Joining the prison service brief reflections
Joining the prison service brief reflectionsJoining the prison service brief reflections
Joining the prison service brief reflections
 
Rcr jchr extract
Rcr jchr extractRcr jchr extract
Rcr jchr extract
 
1966
19661966
1966
 
Extracts about HCPC from Narey report on Sw education
Extracts about HCPC from Narey report on Sw educationExtracts about HCPC from Narey report on Sw education
Extracts about HCPC from Narey report on Sw education
 
Pcc, guardian and medomsley
Pcc, guardian and medomsleyPcc, guardian and medomsley
Pcc, guardian and medomsley
 
Adoption: Resignation as Chair of the Leadership Board
Adoption: Resignation as Chair of the Leadership BoardAdoption: Resignation as Chair of the Leadership Board
Adoption: Resignation as Chair of the Leadership Board
 
Rainsbrook report for publication
Rainsbrook report for publicationRainsbrook report for publication
Rainsbrook report for publication
 
Adoption and my ADCS speech
Adoption and my ADCS speechAdoption and my ADCS speech
Adoption and my ADCS speech
 
Adcs speech july 2015
Adcs speech july 2015Adcs speech july 2015
Adcs speech july 2015
 
2903652 rotherham response_acc2
2903652 rotherham response_acc22903652 rotherham response_acc2
2903652 rotherham response_acc2
 
Need we panic about the fall in placement orders?
Need we panic about the fall in placement orders?Need we panic about the fall in placement orders?
Need we panic about the fall in placement orders?
 
Adoption muddle is costing children their futures
Adoption muddle is costing children their futuresAdoption muddle is costing children their futures
Adoption muddle is costing children their futures
 
Adoption luckock and broadhurst
Adoption luckock and broadhurstAdoption luckock and broadhurst
Adoption luckock and broadhurst
 
The case for adoption in a nutshell from Sir James Munby, December 2014
The case for adoption in a nutshell from Sir James Munby, December 2014The case for adoption in a nutshell from Sir James Munby, December 2014
The case for adoption in a nutshell from Sir James Munby, December 2014
 
Munby judgement on adoption: December 2014
Munby judgement on adoption: December 2014Munby judgement on adoption: December 2014
Munby judgement on adoption: December 2014
 
Adoption cypn nov 2014 (1)
Adoption cypn nov 2014 (1)Adoption cypn nov 2014 (1)
Adoption cypn nov 2014 (1)
 
Alb impact of court judgments on adoption - november 2014 (1) (1)
Alb   impact of court judgments on adoption - november 2014 (1) (1)Alb   impact of court judgments on adoption - november 2014 (1) (1)
Alb impact of court judgments on adoption - november 2014 (1) (1)
 
Mythbuster op ed final mon 10 nov
Mythbuster op ed final mon 10 novMythbuster op ed final mon 10 nov
Mythbuster op ed final mon 10 nov
 

Último

Merck Moving Beyond Passwords: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Merck Moving Beyond Passwords: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxMerck Moving Beyond Passwords: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Merck Moving Beyond Passwords: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxLoriGlavin3
 
Zeshan Sattar- Assessing the skill requirements and industry expectations for...
Zeshan Sattar- Assessing the skill requirements and industry expectations for...Zeshan Sattar- Assessing the skill requirements and industry expectations for...
Zeshan Sattar- Assessing the skill requirements and industry expectations for...itnewsafrica
 
Long journey of Ruby standard library at RubyConf AU 2024
Long journey of Ruby standard library at RubyConf AU 2024Long journey of Ruby standard library at RubyConf AU 2024
Long journey of Ruby standard library at RubyConf AU 2024Hiroshi SHIBATA
 
Microsoft 365 Copilot: How to boost your productivity with AI – Part one: Ado...
Microsoft 365 Copilot: How to boost your productivity with AI – Part one: Ado...Microsoft 365 Copilot: How to boost your productivity with AI – Part one: Ado...
Microsoft 365 Copilot: How to boost your productivity with AI – Part one: Ado...Nikki Chapple
 
Potential of AI (Generative AI) in Business: Learnings and Insights
Potential of AI (Generative AI) in Business: Learnings and InsightsPotential of AI (Generative AI) in Business: Learnings and Insights
Potential of AI (Generative AI) in Business: Learnings and InsightsRavi Sanghani
 
Decarbonising Buildings: Making a net-zero built environment a reality
Decarbonising Buildings: Making a net-zero built environment a realityDecarbonising Buildings: Making a net-zero built environment a reality
Decarbonising Buildings: Making a net-zero built environment a realityIES VE
 
TeamStation AI System Report LATAM IT Salaries 2024
TeamStation AI System Report LATAM IT Salaries 2024TeamStation AI System Report LATAM IT Salaries 2024
TeamStation AI System Report LATAM IT Salaries 2024Lonnie McRorey
 
Bridging Between CAD & GIS: 6 Ways to Automate Your Data Integration
Bridging Between CAD & GIS:  6 Ways to Automate Your Data IntegrationBridging Between CAD & GIS:  6 Ways to Automate Your Data Integration
Bridging Between CAD & GIS: 6 Ways to Automate Your Data Integrationmarketing932765
 
Design pattern talk by Kaya Weers - 2024 (v2)
Design pattern talk by Kaya Weers - 2024 (v2)Design pattern talk by Kaya Weers - 2024 (v2)
Design pattern talk by Kaya Weers - 2024 (v2)Kaya Weers
 
Connecting the Dots for Information Discovery.pdf
Connecting the Dots for Information Discovery.pdfConnecting the Dots for Information Discovery.pdf
Connecting the Dots for Information Discovery.pdfNeo4j
 
Moving Beyond Passwords: FIDO Paris Seminar.pdf
Moving Beyond Passwords: FIDO Paris Seminar.pdfMoving Beyond Passwords: FIDO Paris Seminar.pdf
Moving Beyond Passwords: FIDO Paris Seminar.pdfLoriGlavin3
 
A Deep Dive on Passkeys: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
A Deep Dive on Passkeys: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxA Deep Dive on Passkeys: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
A Deep Dive on Passkeys: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxLoriGlavin3
 
Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxUse of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxLoriGlavin3
 
TrustArc Webinar - How to Build Consumer Trust Through Data Privacy
TrustArc Webinar - How to Build Consumer Trust Through Data PrivacyTrustArc Webinar - How to Build Consumer Trust Through Data Privacy
TrustArc Webinar - How to Build Consumer Trust Through Data PrivacyTrustArc
 
Digital Identity is Under Attack: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Digital Identity is Under Attack: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxDigital Identity is Under Attack: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Digital Identity is Under Attack: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxLoriGlavin3
 
QCon London: Mastering long-running processes in modern architectures
QCon London: Mastering long-running processes in modern architecturesQCon London: Mastering long-running processes in modern architectures
QCon London: Mastering long-running processes in modern architecturesBernd Ruecker
 
So einfach geht modernes Roaming fuer Notes und Nomad.pdf
So einfach geht modernes Roaming fuer Notes und Nomad.pdfSo einfach geht modernes Roaming fuer Notes und Nomad.pdf
So einfach geht modernes Roaming fuer Notes und Nomad.pdfpanagenda
 
A Framework for Development in the AI Age
A Framework for Development in the AI AgeA Framework for Development in the AI Age
A Framework for Development in the AI AgeCprime
 
Genislab builds better products and faster go-to-market with Lean project man...
Genislab builds better products and faster go-to-market with Lean project man...Genislab builds better products and faster go-to-market with Lean project man...
Genislab builds better products and faster go-to-market with Lean project man...Farhan Tariq
 
Varsha Sewlal- Cyber Attacks on Critical Critical Infrastructure
Varsha Sewlal- Cyber Attacks on Critical Critical InfrastructureVarsha Sewlal- Cyber Attacks on Critical Critical Infrastructure
Varsha Sewlal- Cyber Attacks on Critical Critical Infrastructureitnewsafrica
 

Último (20)

Merck Moving Beyond Passwords: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Merck Moving Beyond Passwords: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxMerck Moving Beyond Passwords: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Merck Moving Beyond Passwords: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
 
Zeshan Sattar- Assessing the skill requirements and industry expectations for...
Zeshan Sattar- Assessing the skill requirements and industry expectations for...Zeshan Sattar- Assessing the skill requirements and industry expectations for...
Zeshan Sattar- Assessing the skill requirements and industry expectations for...
 
Long journey of Ruby standard library at RubyConf AU 2024
Long journey of Ruby standard library at RubyConf AU 2024Long journey of Ruby standard library at RubyConf AU 2024
Long journey of Ruby standard library at RubyConf AU 2024
 
Microsoft 365 Copilot: How to boost your productivity with AI – Part one: Ado...
Microsoft 365 Copilot: How to boost your productivity with AI – Part one: Ado...Microsoft 365 Copilot: How to boost your productivity with AI – Part one: Ado...
Microsoft 365 Copilot: How to boost your productivity with AI – Part one: Ado...
 
Potential of AI (Generative AI) in Business: Learnings and Insights
Potential of AI (Generative AI) in Business: Learnings and InsightsPotential of AI (Generative AI) in Business: Learnings and Insights
Potential of AI (Generative AI) in Business: Learnings and Insights
 
Decarbonising Buildings: Making a net-zero built environment a reality
Decarbonising Buildings: Making a net-zero built environment a realityDecarbonising Buildings: Making a net-zero built environment a reality
Decarbonising Buildings: Making a net-zero built environment a reality
 
TeamStation AI System Report LATAM IT Salaries 2024
TeamStation AI System Report LATAM IT Salaries 2024TeamStation AI System Report LATAM IT Salaries 2024
TeamStation AI System Report LATAM IT Salaries 2024
 
Bridging Between CAD & GIS: 6 Ways to Automate Your Data Integration
Bridging Between CAD & GIS:  6 Ways to Automate Your Data IntegrationBridging Between CAD & GIS:  6 Ways to Automate Your Data Integration
Bridging Between CAD & GIS: 6 Ways to Automate Your Data Integration
 
Design pattern talk by Kaya Weers - 2024 (v2)
Design pattern talk by Kaya Weers - 2024 (v2)Design pattern talk by Kaya Weers - 2024 (v2)
Design pattern talk by Kaya Weers - 2024 (v2)
 
Connecting the Dots for Information Discovery.pdf
Connecting the Dots for Information Discovery.pdfConnecting the Dots for Information Discovery.pdf
Connecting the Dots for Information Discovery.pdf
 
Moving Beyond Passwords: FIDO Paris Seminar.pdf
Moving Beyond Passwords: FIDO Paris Seminar.pdfMoving Beyond Passwords: FIDO Paris Seminar.pdf
Moving Beyond Passwords: FIDO Paris Seminar.pdf
 
A Deep Dive on Passkeys: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
A Deep Dive on Passkeys: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxA Deep Dive on Passkeys: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
A Deep Dive on Passkeys: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
 
Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxUse of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
 
TrustArc Webinar - How to Build Consumer Trust Through Data Privacy
TrustArc Webinar - How to Build Consumer Trust Through Data PrivacyTrustArc Webinar - How to Build Consumer Trust Through Data Privacy
TrustArc Webinar - How to Build Consumer Trust Through Data Privacy
 
Digital Identity is Under Attack: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Digital Identity is Under Attack: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxDigital Identity is Under Attack: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Digital Identity is Under Attack: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
 
QCon London: Mastering long-running processes in modern architectures
QCon London: Mastering long-running processes in modern architecturesQCon London: Mastering long-running processes in modern architectures
QCon London: Mastering long-running processes in modern architectures
 
So einfach geht modernes Roaming fuer Notes und Nomad.pdf
So einfach geht modernes Roaming fuer Notes und Nomad.pdfSo einfach geht modernes Roaming fuer Notes und Nomad.pdf
So einfach geht modernes Roaming fuer Notes und Nomad.pdf
 
A Framework for Development in the AI Age
A Framework for Development in the AI AgeA Framework for Development in the AI Age
A Framework for Development in the AI Age
 
Genislab builds better products and faster go-to-market with Lean project man...
Genislab builds better products and faster go-to-market with Lean project man...Genislab builds better products and faster go-to-market with Lean project man...
Genislab builds better products and faster go-to-market with Lean project man...
 
Varsha Sewlal- Cyber Attacks on Critical Critical Infrastructure
Varsha Sewlal- Cyber Attacks on Critical Critical InfrastructureVarsha Sewlal- Cyber Attacks on Critical Critical Infrastructure
Varsha Sewlal- Cyber Attacks on Critical Critical Infrastructure
 

Ward and brown_family_law_sept_2013_vol_43_pp1181-1186_

  • 1. 1181 Reprinted from Family Law Journal September, 2013, Vol 43, Pages 1181-1186 DECISION-MAKING WITHIN A CHILD'S TIMEFRAME: A RESPONSE Harriet Ward, Research Professor and Director of the Centre for Child and Family Research, Loughborough University Rebecca Brown, Senior research associate at the Centre for Child and Family Research, Loughborough University Decision-making within a Child's Timeframe (R Brown and H Ward, Working Paper 16 (Childhood Wellbeing Research Centre (2012)) is an overview of research evidence, funded by the Department for Education with support from the Family Justice Council in response to the Family Justice Review recommendation for consistent training and development for family justice professionals, including greater emphasis on child development. The overview summarises the recent research evidence, which shows how abuse and neglect can impact on early childhood development and considers how timeframes for responses by local authorities and courts are often inconsistent with those for children. It is not intended as a summary of evidence to be drawn upon in making judicial decisions but as a resource to underpin the judicial training programme. To this end, a second edition was published in 2013 with an additional chapter providing training materials. In the August issue of Family Law at p 1053 Edward Lloyd-Jones summarised a seminar based on a critique of this overview. At least three versions of this critique, produced for different audiences, have been widely distributed as unpublished papers: D Wastell and S White, ‘The child's timeframe: what the neuroscience really says', (unpublished, 2013); S White, D Wastell, B Featherstone and K Morris, ‘Briefing document: Decision-Making Within a Child's Timeframe' (R Brown and H Ward, 2013) – an academic analysis, unpublished; S White and D Wastell, ‘A response to R Brown and H Ward, Decision-Making Within a Child's Timeframe', (DFE, 2013, unpublished). None of these is published in a peer-reviewed academic journal where external oversight is offered by an editor or referees. The critique of Brown and Ward is linked to others produced by this group, the most heavily publicised being: D Wastell and S White, (2012) ‘Blinded by neuroscience: social policy, the family and the infant brain', Families, Relationships and Societies, 1(3): 397–414. These papers, in our opinion, make considerable use of emotive language; however, in this article we assert and focus on two other issues: their major and systematic misrepresentation of the research findings and our account of them and also their selective use of evidence to prove their case that the neuroscience is too weak and too flawed to provide a sufficient basis for policy and practice. Our aim has been to produce a sound evidence base to support practice and we do not believe that these papers identify serious flaws in it. Given the gravity of the claims made, we feel duty bound to respond, as well as we can, to not always fully explicit assertions about our report. We will first clarify and restate its key messages and then respond to the specific points captured in Lloyd-Jones' summary. A key point to note at the outset is that the academic governance of Brown and Ward was consistent with that required of such research reports: it was overseen by a steering group, and was then peer-reviewed by three independent academics, all of professorial status and each with expertise in a relevant field (early years, law and neuroscience). Peer ….
  • 2. © Jordan Publishing 2013 1182 reviewing was not blind – many academic journals have now discontinued this process in order to give reviewers an opportunity to declare an interest, which none did. The report benefitted from their suggestions for change and improvement. Neither the referees nor the steering group criticised our approach, which they regarded as both balanced and consistent with current evidence. WHAT BROWN AND WARD (2012) SAYS AND WHAT IT DOES NOT SAY The research summarised in Brown and Ward shows that the first 3 years are an important phase in early childhood, that neurobiological development is shaped by the environment both before and after birth and that, because infants are so dependent on their caregivers for survival, a key feature of the environment is the attachment relationship. It also shows how extreme abuse and neglect in these early years may shape the way in which children develop in all areas: physically, emotionally, socially and cognitively. Children who experience extreme abuse and neglect in these years are more likely to fall behind their peers and develop a wide range of problems in later life. In addition, the longer the maltreatment continues the more likely it is to have a negative impact on development and the more difficult will it be to overcome the consequences. The report does not say that these developments are inevitable or irreversible. It does not say that all children who experience poor parenting or grow up in poverty will develop problems. It does not say that children whose development is compromised by abuse and neglect cannot overcome the consequences. It does not say that parents with problems such as substance misuse, domestic violence and poor mental health cannot change. It certainly does not make the claim that courts should remove children from ‘a home environment where their brains are “shrunken” as a result of abuse and/or neglect' (D Wastell, S White and A Lorek, ‘The child's timeframe – a neuroscientific perspective' (unpublished, 2013), at p 45). What the report does suggest is that if these children are to remain at home, proactive engagement with social workers and other professionals needs to begin early. RESPONSES TO SPECIFIC POINTS IN THE LLOYD-JONES SUMMARY 1. ‘Brown and Ward's report at no point attempts to produce a balanced argument: whilst it begins with a caveat about the knowledge base, in the body of the report, all contradictory evidence or any hint of controversy within the neuroscientific field is ignored.'(p 1054) We were not commissioned to undertake a systematic scientific review of the research on early childhood development nor to ascertain the validity of the neurobiological evidence. Both of these areas have been extensively reviewed and presented in a range of scientific reports, most of which are included in the 482 published and peer-reviewed papers that were identified by our search and on which our summary is based. Furthermore, we were not commissioned to present ‘contradictory' or ‘controversial' evidence, rather to produce the best possible account of the accepted consensus currently within the field. Moreover, none of the 482 papers we identified provided any evidence of a controversy within the field; indeed the only controversy in relation to this issue would appear to be one being created by White, Wastell and colleagues. They frequently cite Bruer ( The Myth of the First Three Years, The Free Press, 1999) to support their argument, but this publication is not a scientific review of the evidence in relation to brain development and does not provide an overview of research on the consequences for children of abusive parenting. 2. ‘Despite the display of scientific trappings and references to an impressive volume of peer-reviewed papers, the neuroscience review relies heavily on a small number of secondary non peer-reviewed books and reports, some from organisations with an apparent ideological orientation and campaigning mission.' (p 1054) We were commissioned to produce a summary of research evidence to be used ….
  • 3. 1183 in training family justice professionals, most of whom will not have had a formal research background. We did not rely on text books and summary papers to produce this review but we did draw the readers' attention to a selection of such publications that provide an accessible (and readable) way into the sometimes complicated literature. Indeed, at the request of the steering group, we also simplified some of the language of the original draft in order to ensure that it would be readily understood. Elsewhere Wastell and White (2013) claim that Brown and Ward (2012) represents ‘a simplified version of the neuroscience' (p 10); that is precisely what we were commissioned to do. Wastell and White's claim that some of the summary papers come from campaigning organisations implies that Brown and Ward (2012) have been biased by an ideological orientation. They focus their critique explicitly on the series of short working papers produced by the Center on the Developing Child, based at Harvard University. They state: ‘The Harvard Center synthesises selective samples of neuroscientific studies; although linked with Harvard University, a cursory visit to the website reveals a campaigning ethos, rather than a centre for objective science. So it is unsurprising that the Center assembles studies and produces reports which serve an a priori agenda. The egregious titles of some of its reports rather give the game away: ‘Building the Brain's Air Traffic Control System' provides one lurid example. The key players are ubiquitous figures in the early interventionist project; Shonkoff (its chair) is a veteran campaigner.' (Wastell, White and Lorek, 2013, pp 51–52). The Center on the Developing Child was established by academics who are experts in the field of neuroscience. The purpose of the site is to ensure that the findings of a deeply complex science are disseminated clearly and appropriately to practitioners and policy makers. The scientists involved are at the top of their field and their research is published in the most prestigious journals. To describe this site as ‘selective', ‘campaigning', ‘a priori' and ‘lurid' is a travesty; no doubt Harvard will be dealing appropriately with these allegations. 3. ‘The conflation of statistical significance and predictive validity; statistical significance merely means that an observed result is not a chance finding; it does not imply the level of predictive power for practical application.' (p 1054) Ward and Brown (2012) does not conflate statistical significance and predictive validity. Our report summarises evidence taken from meta-analyses and systematic reviews, both of which represent the highest level of evidence, primarily because they involve the use of a rigorous methodology for identifying and synthesising all of the available research findings on a particular topic. Much of the evidence in these reviews is epidemiological in nature and is therefore on the whole observational and thereby correlational. Such correlational evidence does not show causation but it is the best that it is possible to obtain in many areas and forms the basis, for example, of our understanding about the relationship between poverty and poor health. The aim of such evidence is not to predict outcomes but to identify consistent patterns and relationships. Once there is sufficient evidence available from such correlational studies, they are summarised in the type of meta-analyses and systematic reviews on which we drew, which is then used for the purpose of policymaking (see M Marmot, Fair Society, Healthy Lives (The Marmot Review, 2010)). This is in contrast to the White and Wastell (2012) paper ‘Blinded by neuroscience' in which the authors have picked just one of the many systematic reviews on the neuroscience (E McCory, S A De Brito, and E Viding, ‘The link between child abuse and psychopathology: a review of neurobiological and genetic research' (2012) Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 105:151–6), extracted those elements that support their case and ignored others, and in so doing have, in our opinion, misrepresented to the reader the overall findings of that review and the wider evidence. Similarly, they quote the Belsky and de Haan (2011) statement to support their claim that the knowledge base is insufficiently established: ‘the study …
  • 4. © Jordan Publishing 2013 1184 of parenting and brain development is not even yet in its infancy; it would be more appropriate to conclude that it is still in the embryonic stage' (p 409–410). They do not quote the next sentence, which reads: ‘Nevertheless, as we plan to show, there is emerging evidence that experience shapes the developing brain in humans.' (p 410); nor the following: ‘[N]ow that so much progress has been made in the first stage of inquiry, it seems appropriate to conclude that research in this area of inquiry has reached “the end of the beginning”.' p 423 (J Belsky and M de Haan, ‘Annual research review: parenting and children's brain development: the end of the beginning' (2011) Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 52 (4): 409–428). 4. ‘The neuroscientific strand of the argument is a resurrection of the ‘myth of the first three years', the idea that the brain is highly vulnerable to irreversible damage in the early years of life. The science, in fact, says quite the opposite.' (p 1054) The neuroscience evidence that has been undertaken over the past two decades has now been systematically summarised in a range of highly rigorous and closely scrutinised, peer- reviewed scientific papers. This evidence shows beyond doubt that severely suboptimal and abusive parenting that takes place during the first 3 years of life has a disproportionately large impact on the infant/toddler brain. Although much of the evidence about this particular period refers to the ‘global neglect' experienced by infants in orphanages, this research helps us to understand the extensive evidence about the emotional and behavioural development of very young children in the UK who experience neglect and maltreatment, including for example, the persistent fear response, hyper-arousal, dissociation and disorganised attachments that have been widely identified. As far as we are aware, none of this evidence suggests that the damage that so clearly occurs as a result of abuse during this period is irreversible. It does, however, indicate that the longer the exposure to maltreatment, the harder it is to overcome the consequences. It is not, therefore, acceptable to leave children in environments that we now recognise to have this sort of impact on both their physical (brain) and emotional (attachment) wellbeing in the hope that something can be done to reverse it later in their life. Lloyd-Jones alludes at p 1053 to the Munro review, of which White and Wastell were expert members, as supporting their stance; but the Munro report states: ‘Neuroscience also offers lessons on the importance of the early years. A recent paper by the Royal Society (The Royal Society, 2011) on the implications of neuroscience for education policy, highlights that there are changes in the brain taking place throughout life, but the number decreases with age. The worst and deepest brain damage occurs before birth and in the first 18 months of life when the emotional circuits are forming'. (E Munro, The Munro Review of Child Protection: Final Report – A child-centred system (Department for Education, 2011), at p 71). 5. ‘The core argument, that maltreatment is intrinsically bad and can cause lasting damage, is only weakened by this apparent misrepresentation of neuroscience. Invocation of attachment theory also does not aid the cause – attachment theory is superfluous to the core argument and its reliability and predictive power are questionable.' (p 1054) Like the evidence about the impact of abuse on children's brains, attachment research is now a highly developed body of knowledge, much of which has again been summarised in rigorous systematic reviews. These reviews clearly show that the type of ‘disorganised attachment', which results from abuse during the early years, is strongly associated with later psychopathology (J Green and R Goldwyn, ‘Annotation: attachment disorganisation and psychopathology: new findings in attachment research and their potential implications for developmental psychopathology in childhood', (2002) Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 43 (7): 835–846). This observational evidence is similar to that for a wide range of health outcomes (eg cigarette smoking where we know that not every smoker will develop lung cancer but many will suffer seriously as a result of it) about which we now have very clear policy directives.
  • 5. 1185 6. ‘The repeated claim that emotional abuse is more serious than either physical or sexual abuse seemed perverse to the professors: where was the evidence for that, indeed for the existence of such abuse as a well-defined category? The authors seemed unconcerned about the elevation of such a category to take precedence over other forms of abuse. Another “slippery but potent” concept was Shonkoff's tautological notion of “toxic stress”.' (p 1054) The extensive evidence concerning the existence and consequences of emotional abuse is summarised by Barlow and Schrader McMillan ( Safeguarding Children from Emotional Maltreatment(Jessica Kingsley Publishers, 2010), at pp 20–36). Some researchers consider this to be the most damaging of all forms of maltreatment in early childhood because the perpetrator is almost always the primary caregiver and their behaviour represents a direct negation of the child's need for safety, love, belonging and self-esteem (see D Ivaniec, ‘An overview of emotional maltreatment and failure to thrive' (1997) Child Abuse Review, 6: 370–388). We did not claim that it is more serious than other forms of abuse throughout childhood, and we did not recommend that emotional abuse be given precedence over other forms of abuse. And as to Shonkoff's supposedly tautological notion of ‘toxic stress', the supporting evidence was published in the highly prestigious Journal of the American Medical Association (JP Shonkoff, WT Boyce, BS McEwen, ‘Neuroscience, molecular biology and the childhood roots of health disparities. Building a new framework for health promotion and disease prevention', (2009) JAMA 301(21): 2252–2259), which we think speaks for itself. CONCLUSION Wastell and White's papers are not academic analyses of research data. They claim that Ward and Brown (2012) is ‘designed to prop up a moral mission' (Wastell and White, 2013, p 10): the mission being the current government agenda on adoption and the care system which they apparently view as an attack on the vulnerable and the poor. We too have challenged government policies that focus on adoption and other forms of care without also strengthening support for families and addressing the factors that render abuse and therefore separation more likely (see H Ward, ‘Again the most vulnerable suffer' and ‘Taking children into care can only be a sticking-plaster solution' The Guardian, 7 September 2012 and 21 November 2012 respectively:http://www.guardian.co.uk/society/2012/sep/07/again-the-most-vulnerable- suffer; http://www.guardian.co.uk/society/2012/nov/21/taking-children-care-sticking-plaster- solution). Like our critics we are well aware that current neurobiological research could be misrepresented to inform policies that may damage families (for instance by feeding into the political discourse about ‘strivers and skivers'). However, it is not appropriate to attempt to discredit research because it could be misused in this way. Lloyd-Jones states at p 1055 that: ‘The permanent removal of a child from his birth family is one of the most draconian actions of the state with life-long consequences. Surely we as a society have a responsibility to our most vulnerable children to ensure that such momentous decisions are made in a timely manner on the basis of the best available evidence'. The decision of the Family Justice Board to commission the training that is underpinned by our rigorous overview of the evidence should enable this to happen. In the meantime it is, in our opinion, incumbent upon White and Wastell to produce a scholarly and evidence-based critique of the neuroscientific and related research, which would involve them systematically reviewing the evidence. They also need to provide an alternative explanation for those questions that the neuroscientific research helps us understand, such as why some children may find it difficult to control anger and frustration and why some who are placed for adoption may fail to develop secure attachments with loving parents. Recent research provides conclusive evidence of the damaging consequences to children's life chances of delayed decisions in cases where significant harm is an issue (see C Davies and H Ward, Safeguarding Children Across Services (Jessica Kingsley Publishers, 2012)). It should be of concern to us all that ….
  • 6. 1186 the significance of these findings is being undermined by arguments that we think fail to engage with the evidence in this field. The potential consequence is that more cases will go to the Court of Appeal and the timeframe for the resolution of children's cases will be further extended, to the detriment of their wellbeing. © Jordan Publishing 2013