SlideShare una empresa de Scribd logo
1 de 5
Descargar para leer sin conexión
Cur cognition: Do stray dogs have qualitatively different kinds of canine ...   http://www.scientificamerican.com/blog/post.cfm?id=cur-cognition-do-st...




          Bering in Mind - July 16, 2010

          Cur cognition: Do stray dogs have qualitatively different kinds of canine minds?
          By Jesse Bering
                                                                   In previous
                                                                   posts, I’ve
                                                                   discussed my
                                                                   fascination with
                                                                   dogs, such as
                                                                   this recent
                                                                   controversial
                                                                   piece mentioning
                                                                   those
                                                                   good-natured pit
                                                                   bulls whose
                                                                   unearned
                                                                   reputations often
                                                                   precede them
                                                                   because of a few
                                                                   maladjusted,
                                                                   vicious outliers.
                                                                   Yet I’ve never seen anything quite like the canines of Sofia, Bulgaria,
                                                                   from where I’ve just returned after a week of teaching at a cognitive
                                                                   science summer school and from listening to a surfeit of long-forgotten,
                                                                   uplifting '80s pop music, which the weary and unshaven Bulgarian taxi
                                                                   drivers seem to adore to no end. Some recent work by University of
                                                                   Florida psychologist Monique Udell and her colleagues suggests that
                                                                   it’s not just my imagination that stray dogs are special—rather, strays
                                                                   in general may be vastly more different from our pet dogs than we
                                                                   assumed, particularly in their social cognitive functioning.


          Now, the stray dog situation in Sofia is notoriously problematic. You know you’ve got a problem when a pack of strays
          breaks into the deer exhibit at your local zoo and “ruthlessly dismembers” almost the entire collection, as happened earlier
          this year. And given the general sentiment that an organized roundup and euthanasia is out of the question for moral
          reasons it’s also a very prickly issue among the people living there. (Stray cats are a problem too, but they appear to be
          kept in check by a lot of very hungry dogs.) Stray dogs are about common as squirrels there, and from what I saw, most
          of Sofia’s human population also has about as much interest in strays as Americans in the suburbs have in the squirrels
          living in their backyards. A June, 2010 estimate placed the figures at around 9,500 dogs running loose in the Sofia
          confines. So when you’ve got that many animals—even man’s best friend—in a relatively small, concentrated city (not to
          mention one with its own human homeless problems in the form of the ostracized Romanian Roma, or “gypsies”), mass
          desensitization is sadly inevitable. But this human-dog indifference is even more striking because it appears to be mutual.

          I watched incredulously as the Sofia strays ambled casually down the sidewalks like proper Bulgarian citizens. They
          stepped aside politely for human pedestrians before continuing on their way, stopped patiently to look both ways before
          loping across frantically organized, crowded freeways, mingled with one another at storefronts and had their mangy coats
          tousled by the whooshing tires of passing commercial trucks while in the midday heat they slept quietly in tree-shaded
          gutters mere inches from the road. Most of these animals are multigenerational strays, which means that they are the
          offspring of strays who were the offspring of strays and so on, and on, for many breeding generations. Natural selection
          must work quickly indeed under such conditions: these are the descendents of the craftiest ancestral dogs of yesteryear
          Sofia, those who survived puppyhood without being crushed by some juggernaut and who managed to live long enough to
          pass on their wily natures to their offspring. Too much reliance on humans or interest in human behavior may well be



1 of 5                                                                                                                               8/10/2010 5:42 PM
Cur cognition: Do stray dogs have qualitatively different kinds of canine ...   http://www.scientificamerican.com/blog/post.cfm?id=cur-cognition-do-st...


          maladaptive to these dogs’ overall genetic interests within this selective context, given the situation. I love my dogs,
          Gulliver and Uma, and they’re pretty smart as far as dogs go. But they wouldn’t last two minutes on the streets of Sofia.

          The Sofia strays may be mongrels, and indeed many looked to me more like dingoes or coyotes than they did domestic
          dogs, but technically they’re every bit as much members of Canis lupus familiaris as your own pampered Lhasa apso or
          Chocolate Labrador. But perhaps genes alone do not a dog make. In an intriguing study published in March of this year in
          the journal Animal Behavior , comparative psychologists Monique Udell, Nicole Dorey and Clive Wynn demonstrated that
          stray dogs reason about human social behavior (and in particular, our intentions) in a fundamentally different way from pet
          dogs.

          The importance of Udell’s work can only be understood in relation to previous experimental findings demonstrating that
          domestic dogs (but not other canine species) comprehend referential meaning and cooperative intent in human behavior.
          That is to say, in controlled laboratory research, domestic dogs can do things such as understand that a human
          experimenter is intentionally trying to share his or her knowledge about the location of a hidden food reward by pointing
          deliberately at one of two containers. Inferring that an indexical point is symbolically about a referential target—something
          “out there”—instead of just an erect index finger sticking out from a closed fist requires more cognitive sophistication than
          it may seem at first blush. This is because comprehending such a gesture demands human-like social cognition, namely a
          precursor to “theory of mind,” which enables us to reason about the abstract, unobservable psychological states driving
          overt behavior. My cat, Tommy, is great, but when I tried to point out something in the corner of the room to him just now
          while sitting in my chair, he stood up on his hind legs to smell my fingertip.

          For human beings, pointing is so much a part of our nature as to seem unremarkable. But if you think about it, it’s not
          even self-evident why our species points most commonly with its index finger rather than, say, its thumb or pinkie finger.
          According to a 1994 Journal of Comparative of Psychology article by University of Louisiana at Lafayette psychologist
          Daniel Povinelli and Louisiana State University anesthesiologist Richard Davis, there is an important anatomical difference
          between the relaxed postures of the human hand and that of our closet living relative, the chimpanzee. Go ahead, let your
          wrist go limp and look at your hand from the side, or if you’re too insecure in your own sexuality, just picture Adam’s limp
          wrist at the moment of creation in Michelangelo’s masterpiece on the Sistine Chapel’s ceiling. See how even in this
          relaxed state the index finger is slightly extended? By contrast, when chimps do this (hence the anesthesiologist
          coauthor), their index finger falls naturally in line with their other fingers. Povinelli and Davis reason that this subtle
          evolutionary change in the morphology of our hands, which occurred after humans and chimpanzees last shared a
          common ancestor five million to seven million years ago, is at least partially responsible for the fact that human pointing
          with the index finger is so culturally ubiquitous today.

          The argument goes something like this. When young infants begin reaching for objects just out of their range, adults are
          most likely to respond to those reaching attempts and to retrieve the item for the baby when the latter’s index finger is
          more prominently extended. That is to say, initially, the adult mistakenly reads into the child’s reaching attempt as a
          communicative gesture on the part of the child. Over time, this dynamic between the child and adult serves to further “pull
          out” the index finger because the child implicitly learns the behavioral association, so that it slowly becomes a genuine
          pointing gesture. There are several varieties of pointing, and this is important for understanding why dogs’ comprehension
          of the gesture matters. Typically, before 18 to 24 months of age, pointing is used to manipulate others’ behaviors only,
          just like we’ve seen in the description above. If a baby drops her toy on the ground and points to it while looking at you,
          she’s basically saying, “Well, what are you waiting for, give it to me!” In developmental science jargon, this is called
          imperative pointing because it’s more or less a demand. But as children’s brains continue to develop, and once they’re
          able to conceptualize others as conversational partners who have minds that hold information, pointing becomes
          declarative . The two-year-old now uses pointing to say, “Hey, look over there!” because she recognizes that you’re not
          aware of whatever it is (the neighbor’s cat in the bushes, the exorbitantly large man eating a snow cone at the park) and
          wants to share this fascinating information with you. In fact, difficulties with comprehension and production of declarative
          pointing are often used as diagnostic indicators of autism in young children, a disorder characterized by social cognitive
          impairments.

          So this background is important for understanding why the question of whether or not dogs possess the capacity to
          reason correctly about human pointing is interesting. Several years ago, Duke University psychologist Brian Hare and his
          colleagues reported some striking evidence showing that domestic dogs performed above chance on a variety of human-
          guided selection tasks—including studies in which human experimenters pointed to different objects in the room. At the
          time, these data were interpreted as showing that dogs have human-like social cognition allowing them to understand
          cooperative intent in humans. In fact, whereas tame wolves fail to score above chance in such studies, domestic dogs
          even outperform chimpanzees on similar pointing tests, suggesting that we may have more in common psychologically
          with dogs than with species for which we’re taxonomically (much) more closely related. This prompted Hare to argue that



2 of 5                                                                                                                               8/10/2010 5:42 PM
Cur cognition: Do stray dogs have qualitatively different kinds of canine ...   http://www.scientificamerican.com/blog/post.cfm?id=cur-cognition-do-st...


          the co-evolution of humans and the domestic dog had created in the latter a genuine ability to reason about human mental
          states. Anybody who has ever owned or interacted with a dog can probably think up ample anecdotes here, but if not,
          just think of Lassie’s proverbial message-sharing abilities.

          Udell and her group in Florida, however, say that these impressive social cognitive abilities in dogs may not represent the
          “default” canine cognitive system. In their review of this literature on dog social cognition, the authors point out that:

                  The currently available data suggest that populations of dogs differing in [breeding] and in environmental and
                  lifetime pressures might display different behavioral responses to the actions of humans. Despite this fact,
                  the great majority of subjects in studies of the origins of domestic dogs’ human-compatible social cognition
                  have been pet dogs living in human homes, with human-oriented working dogs representing the remainder of
                  the subject pool.


          In other words, Udell and her coauthors’ contention is similar to arguments made by many researchers studying human
          psychological evolution—that our ability to make claims about “human nature” are seriously limited by the fact that the
          data upon which such claims are made are derived almost entirely from middleclass American undergraduate students
          between 18-22 years of age and recruited from a psychology department subject pool. She’s basically arguing that
          existing social cognition research on Canis lupus familiaris has largely neglected large demographic swells of the species
          and therefore does not necessarily paint an entirely accurate portrait of this species’ natural (default) psychological
          stance. Bulgaria notwithstanding, consider that within the U.S. alone six to eight million dogs can be found living in shelters
          each year, which is roughly 11 percent of the entire American domestic dog population.

          Thus, to remedy this theoretical oversight and to address the issue of whether dogs simply inherit the cognitive capacity
          to think about human behaviors in intentional terms without any developmental experiences in interacting socially with
          human beings—that is to say, whether or not it’s something “innate” to dog psychology resulting from dogs’ brain evolution
          coinciding significantly with our own—Udell went to an animal shelter and administered two pointing comprehension
          studies with a range of dogs that were collected from the streets and classified as strays. Obviously, if these strays
          performed as well on such tasks as the pet dogs from earlier studies, the innateness claim would be justified. But if not,
          then developmental experiences with humans—and probably very particular types of experience at that—were
          contributing to their performing above chance in those prior studies.

          After a number of training trials to ensure that the subjects understood the basic task demands, the experimenter pointed
          to one of two cans and then gave the dog the opportunity to respond to this helpful gesture. A “correct” response to the
          pointing gesture was operationalized as the dog touching or coming within 10cm of the indicated can, and for doing so the
          animal received a coveted food reinforcer such as a jerky treat. To prevent the dog from simply using its nose to sniff out
          the reward, both cans were empty—rather, the experimenter simply dropped the food reward on top of the can whenever
          the subject chose correctly (i.e., selected the pointed-to can).

          The most significant findings from Udell’s studies were these. Although the strays performed above chance when the
          experimenter was kneeling on the floor and the tip of the experimenter’s finger was rigidly held 10 cm from the target can,
          unlike the domestic dogs in prior studies these strays failed to respond correctly to the pointing gesture when such an
          obvious physical cue was removed. On pointing trials in which the experimenter’s finger was 50 cm from the closest edge
          of the target container at full extension and then her arm was retracted back to a neutral position before the subject was
          allowed to make a choice, the strays’ performance fell to chance levels. This distinction is critical for the debate over
          whether domestic dogs have some semblance of theory of mind, because in the first instance at least, dogs may be using
          a simple behavioral heuristic such as “pick-the-box-closest-to-the-hand” that does not require human-like social cognition
          in which they are inferring cooperative intent.

          In a follow-up study, Udell found some evidence that strays who were afforded additional experience with humans in an
          exposure condition, which involved play, petting and free exploration of the target area, showed better performance on
          the pointing task, but still they were less impressive in their comprehension of human pointing than the pet dogs in
          previous studies. Although the authors acknowledge the limitations of this work (it cannot directly address the evolution of
          domestic dog social behavior), Udell and her colleagues argue that these findings reveal how:

                  Evidence of differences in the behavior of populations within a subspecies of animals is important when
                  attempting to draw comparative conclusions between species and subspecies of animals …

                  [and so] comparisons between domestic dog populations should consider different forms of response in the
                  presence of different human stimuli instead of searching for evidence of a universal inborn capacity to



3 of 5                                                                                                                               8/10/2010 5:42 PM
Cur cognition: Do stray dogs have qualitatively different kinds of canine ...   http://www.scientificamerican.com/blog/post.cfm?id=cur-cognition-do-st...


                  respond to human gestures.


          The debate reminds me of some work I was involved with back in graduate school, in which there was some evidence
          that great apes that had been raised by human beings as though they were human beings actually demonstrated species
          atypical human-like social cognition (namely, a theory of mind). The core question in all of this is what is the ‘default’ type
          of social cognition of a dog, of a chimp … of a human being?

          In any event, now that I’m back to the home-sweet-hominess of Belfast and its ongoing riots, I can’t help but to think back
          to all of those motley, feral dogs of Sofia. I was ambivalent to learn about the Bulgarian government’s plans to (finally)
          construct a series of dog kennels to house strays by the end of 2011. On the one hand, getting these animals off the
          streets is clearly a good thing in many ways, both for their own wellbeing and that of, for example, the zoo animals. And
          there’s also, of course, the safety and interest of people—there have been several highly publicized attacks by wild dog
          packs, including fatalities—on children and the elderly in Sofia. On the other hand, most of these dogs are very clever,
          harmless, disinterested in human beings and surviving in some very complex and novel ecological conditions. Surely these
          stray dogs, as well as those eking out a living in other major cities in which strays are a notorious problem, present
          extraordinary scientific opportunities.

          And from an animal wellbeing perspective, removing these stray dogs from these environmentally rich, if difficult, industrial
          surroundings and isolating them to physically restricted concrete-and-steel living conditions strikes me as a rather sad
          prospect, too. I say this having worked at the Broward County Animal Shelter in South Florida the summer after I
          graduated from high school, where I saw scenes of dogs in distress that haunt me to this day. (Seriously, what kind of
          person would discard their happy pair of 12-year-old, sweet-faced golden retrievers because their new girlfriend doesn’t
          “like dogs”?) In fact, embarrassingly, I was fired from that job for spending too much time petting, playing with and
          otherwise comforting animals that were clearly suffering. True, I should probably have been doing something more
          productive and useful like cleaning cages or unloading food bags, but I couldn’t help it: the sight of a suffering dog makes
          me human—oddly, peculiarly, probably more so than the sight of another suffering human being. But I’ll save that curiosity
          for another post.

          I found some solace from my poor employee behavior from this 2006 Physiology & Behavior study by Crista Coppola
          (coauthored by the autistic animal behavior scientist Temple Grandin). The authors point out that when dogs are housed in
          an animal shelter, they usually experience a severe form of psychological stress caused by exposure to novel or
          threatening surroundings, separation from attachment objects, unpredictability of external events, lack or loss of control
          over the environment, and so on. This stress activates their hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and pumps out
          explosive levels of cortisol, which is the major hormonal indicator of response to stress. Coppola and her colleagues found
          that, regardless of breed, age of dog and sex, those shelter dogs that received a pleasant “human interaction session” on
          Day 2 of their incarceration had significantly lower cortisol levels on Day 9—that is to say, the benefits of this simple pet
          and play session were found a week later, even without any subsequent interaction with human beings during the
          intervening days. Honestly, this finding brings tears to my eyes: simple human affection is that long-lasting and important
          for dogs. And Coppola and her coauthors rightly lament that it’s a pity that the vast majority of dog shelters have not
          instituted routine human interaction sessions with their new, stressed-out arrivals.

          But the dogs in Coppolo’s study were largely owner-surrendered ex-pets, those who’d learned to be dependent on the
          kindness of human beings in their lives. Whether these cortisol-relieving effects of human interaction would be replicated
          in, say, those stray dogs of Sofia is unclear. It’s possible that the effects would be reversed, given their developmental
          histories on the streets.

          And so it is that those stray dogs in Bulgaria broke my heart and intrigued me by their adaptedness to their unusual
          surroundings. Like the Bulgarians, I was left without any easy answers to the problem. It’s a real mess indeed, with
          people bringing puppies home, failing to neuter them and so on. But, for science’s sake, and particularly for our
          understanding of the coevolution of human and canine social cognition, I’d personally love to see someone do a research
          documentary on the feral dogs of Sofia before they’re off the streets and locked up for good behind those new kennel
          walls.



          In this column presented by Scientific American Mind magazine, research psychologist Jesse Bering of Queen's
          University Belfast ponders some of the more obscure aspects of everyday human behavior. Sign up for the RSS feed,
          visit www.JesseBering.com, friend Dr. Bering on Facebook or follow @JesseBering on Twitter and never miss an
          installment again. For articles published prior to September 29, 2009, click here: older Bering in Mind columns. Jesse's



4 of 5                                                                                                                               8/10/2010 5:42 PM
Cur cognition: Do stray dogs have qualitatively different kinds of canine ...   http://www.scientificamerican.com/blog/post.cfm?id=cur-cognition-do-st...


          first book, The Belief Instinct (Norton) [The God Instinct (Nicholas Brealey) in the U.K.], will be published early
          February, 2011.

          Image ©iStockphoto.com/TSnowImages
          Further Reading
          Mother's Pregnancy Weight Linked to Child's Obesity
          Speaking in Tones: Music and Language Partner in the Brain
          Skeleton Key: Bone Cells May Play a Part in Regulating the Body's Metabolism
          How Does a Heat Wave Affect the Human Body?

          DNA Drugs Come of Age
          How Does Sewage Treatment Work?
          Live Long and Proper: Genetic Factors Associated with Increased Longevity Identified
          California Proposes New Regulations on Chemicals in Consumer Products




5 of 5                                                                                                                               8/10/2010 5:42 PM

Más contenido relacionado

Destacado

Sylvesterkaart
SylvesterkaartSylvesterkaart
SylvesterkaartGilles K.
 
Donaciones de libros 2008
Donaciones de libros 2008Donaciones de libros 2008
Donaciones de libros 2008biblioteca6de13
 
где заработать ит специалисту в н.новгороде.
где заработать ит специалисту в н.новгороде. где заработать ит специалисту в н.новгороде.
где заработать ит специалисту в н.новгороде. Elena Kotina
 
Skydiving 07-2006
Skydiving 07-2006Skydiving 07-2006
Skydiving 07-2006TracieGrady
 
Lois Lerner IRS email scandal: the parade of clowns
Lois Lerner IRS email scandal: the parade of clownsLois Lerner IRS email scandal: the parade of clowns
Lois Lerner IRS email scandal: the parade of clownsKent Summers
 
Scharmika Youngblood
Scharmika YoungbloodScharmika Youngblood
Scharmika Youngbloodsyoungblood
 
Kelleman Associates
Kelleman  AssociatesKelleman  Associates
Kelleman Associatespkellerman
 
Modos De Produccion 2
Modos De Produccion 2Modos De Produccion 2
Modos De Produccion 2HERMENEGILDO
 
Współczesne przeglądarki - ZamCamp#3
Współczesne przeglądarki - ZamCamp#3Współczesne przeglądarki - ZamCamp#3
Współczesne przeglądarki - ZamCamp#3zamcamp
 

Destacado (17)

Sylvesterkaart
SylvesterkaartSylvesterkaart
Sylvesterkaart
 
Moments
MomentsMoments
Moments
 
UQUDENT Arabic Annual Report 2011
UQUDENT Arabic Annual Report 2011UQUDENT Arabic Annual Report 2011
UQUDENT Arabic Annual Report 2011
 
Topical
TopicalTopical
Topical
 
Donaciones de libros 2008
Donaciones de libros 2008Donaciones de libros 2008
Donaciones de libros 2008
 
где заработать ит специалисту в н.новгороде.
где заработать ит специалисту в н.новгороде. где заработать ит специалисту в н.новгороде.
где заработать ит специалисту в н.новгороде.
 
Skydiving 07-2006
Skydiving 07-2006Skydiving 07-2006
Skydiving 07-2006
 
Lois Lerner IRS email scandal: the parade of clowns
Lois Lerner IRS email scandal: the parade of clownsLois Lerner IRS email scandal: the parade of clowns
Lois Lerner IRS email scandal: the parade of clowns
 
Scharmika Youngblood
Scharmika YoungbloodScharmika Youngblood
Scharmika Youngblood
 
Buena Publicidad
Buena PublicidadBuena Publicidad
Buena Publicidad
 
Mitocondria
MitocondriaMitocondria
Mitocondria
 
linux
linuxlinux
linux
 
Kelleman Associates
Kelleman  AssociatesKelleman  Associates
Kelleman Associates
 
NetworkerMsb
NetworkerMsbNetworkerMsb
NetworkerMsb
 
Modos De Produccion 2
Modos De Produccion 2Modos De Produccion 2
Modos De Produccion 2
 
Współczesne przeglądarki - ZamCamp#3
Współczesne przeglądarki - ZamCamp#3Współczesne przeglądarki - ZamCamp#3
Współczesne przeglądarki - ZamCamp#3
 
Steel
SteelSteel
Steel
 

Similar a Cur cognition

Example Of Argumentative Essay
Example Of Argumentative EssayExample Of Argumentative Essay
Example Of Argumentative EssayKenya Lucas
 
Animals are way smarter than we think
Animals are way smarter than we thinkAnimals are way smarter than we think
Animals are way smarter than we thinkSergei VanBellinghen
 
Romeo And Juliet Fate Essay. Romeo and Juliet Essay - GCSE English - Marked b...
Romeo And Juliet Fate Essay. Romeo and Juliet Essay - GCSE English - Marked b...Romeo And Juliet Fate Essay. Romeo and Juliet Essay - GCSE English - Marked b...
Romeo And Juliet Fate Essay. Romeo and Juliet Essay - GCSE English - Marked b...Angel Newell
 
Get Help For Writing All Essay Types On EssayGat
Get Help For Writing All Essay Types On EssayGatGet Help For Writing All Essay Types On EssayGat
Get Help For Writing All Essay Types On EssayGatMichelle Brown
 
speaking class about dogs - aula de conversação
speaking class about dogs - aula de conversaçãospeaking class about dogs - aula de conversação
speaking class about dogs - aula de conversaçãoamandavaleriogs
 
Discourse Community Essay Example. Discourse community essay outline - writef...
Discourse Community Essay Example. Discourse community essay outline - writef...Discourse Community Essay Example. Discourse community essay outline - writef...
Discourse Community Essay Example. Discourse community essay outline - writef...Mimi Williams
 
The Secret Lives of Animals.pdf
The Secret Lives of Animals.pdfThe Secret Lives of Animals.pdf
The Secret Lives of Animals.pdfToniEakes1
 

Similar a Cur cognition (13)

The Age Of Empathy
The Age Of EmpathyThe Age Of Empathy
The Age Of Empathy
 
dogs history
dogs history dogs history
dogs history
 
Example Of Argumentative Essay
Example Of Argumentative EssayExample Of Argumentative Essay
Example Of Argumentative Essay
 
Animals are way smarter than we think
Animals are way smarter than we thinkAnimals are way smarter than we think
Animals are way smarter than we think
 
Romeo And Juliet Fate Essay. Romeo and Juliet Essay - GCSE English - Marked b...
Romeo And Juliet Fate Essay. Romeo and Juliet Essay - GCSE English - Marked b...Romeo And Juliet Fate Essay. Romeo and Juliet Essay - GCSE English - Marked b...
Romeo And Juliet Fate Essay. Romeo and Juliet Essay - GCSE English - Marked b...
 
Dogs feel your pain
Dogs feel your painDogs feel your pain
Dogs feel your pain
 
Get Help For Writing All Essay Types On EssayGat
Get Help For Writing All Essay Types On EssayGatGet Help For Writing All Essay Types On EssayGat
Get Help For Writing All Essay Types On EssayGat
 
speaking class about dogs - aula de conversação
speaking class about dogs - aula de conversaçãospeaking class about dogs - aula de conversação
speaking class about dogs - aula de conversação
 
GENERAL HISTORY OF DOG
GENERAL HISTORY OF DOGGENERAL HISTORY OF DOG
GENERAL HISTORY OF DOG
 
Dog's history
Dog's historyDog's history
Dog's history
 
Discourse Community Essay Example. Discourse community essay outline - writef...
Discourse Community Essay Example. Discourse community essay outline - writef...Discourse Community Essay Example. Discourse community essay outline - writef...
Discourse Community Essay Example. Discourse community essay outline - writef...
 
GENERAL HISTORY OF DOGS
GENERAL HISTORY OF DOGSGENERAL HISTORY OF DOGS
GENERAL HISTORY OF DOGS
 
The Secret Lives of Animals.pdf
The Secret Lives of Animals.pdfThe Secret Lives of Animals.pdf
The Secret Lives of Animals.pdf
 

Más de Raamesh Gowri Raghavan

Fortifications of China, Japan and Korea
Fortifications of China, Japan and KoreaFortifications of China, Japan and Korea
Fortifications of China, Japan and KoreaRaamesh Gowri Raghavan
 
Trankebar and the Danish East India Co (DOK)
Trankebar and the Danish East India Co (DOK)Trankebar and the Danish East India Co (DOK)
Trankebar and the Danish East India Co (DOK)Raamesh Gowri Raghavan
 
Ancient scripts, their decipherment and palaeography of India, 29 January 2017
Ancient scripts, their decipherment and palaeography of India, 29 January 2017Ancient scripts, their decipherment and palaeography of India, 29 January 2017
Ancient scripts, their decipherment and palaeography of India, 29 January 2017Raamesh Gowri Raghavan
 
India Post: Themes and Biases in Commemorative Stamps Issued From 1994 To 2016
India Post: Themes and Biases in Commemorative Stamps Issued From 1994 To 2016India Post: Themes and Biases in Commemorative Stamps Issued From 1994 To 2016
India Post: Themes and Biases in Commemorative Stamps Issued From 1994 To 2016Raamesh Gowri Raghavan
 
Will the Indian Ocean ever truly be India’s Ocean? (Prospects for the future ...
Will the Indian Ocean ever truly be India’s Ocean? (Prospects for the future ...Will the Indian Ocean ever truly be India’s Ocean? (Prospects for the future ...
Will the Indian Ocean ever truly be India’s Ocean? (Prospects for the future ...Raamesh Gowri Raghavan
 
Preliminary Explorations at the Cave Complex at Kothaligad, Taluka Karjat, Di...
Preliminary Explorations at the Cave Complex at Kothaligad, Taluka Karjat, Di...Preliminary Explorations at the Cave Complex at Kothaligad, Taluka Karjat, Di...
Preliminary Explorations at the Cave Complex at Kothaligad, Taluka Karjat, Di...Raamesh Gowri Raghavan
 
Grantha Script: Writing System and Evolutionary History
Grantha Script: Writing System and Evolutionary HistoryGrantha Script: Writing System and Evolutionary History
Grantha Script: Writing System and Evolutionary HistoryRaamesh Gowri Raghavan
 

Más de Raamesh Gowri Raghavan (20)

Fortifications of China, Japan and Korea
Fortifications of China, Japan and KoreaFortifications of China, Japan and Korea
Fortifications of China, Japan and Korea
 
Trankebar and the Danish East India Co (DOK)
Trankebar and the Danish East India Co (DOK)Trankebar and the Danish East India Co (DOK)
Trankebar and the Danish East India Co (DOK)
 
Ancient scripts, their decipherment and palaeography of India, 29 January 2017
Ancient scripts, their decipherment and palaeography of India, 29 January 2017Ancient scripts, their decipherment and palaeography of India, 29 January 2017
Ancient scripts, their decipherment and palaeography of India, 29 January 2017
 
India Post: Themes and Biases in Commemorative Stamps Issued From 1994 To 2016
India Post: Themes and Biases in Commemorative Stamps Issued From 1994 To 2016India Post: Themes and Biases in Commemorative Stamps Issued From 1994 To 2016
India Post: Themes and Biases in Commemorative Stamps Issued From 1994 To 2016
 
Will the Indian Ocean ever truly be India’s Ocean? (Prospects for the future ...
Will the Indian Ocean ever truly be India’s Ocean? (Prospects for the future ...Will the Indian Ocean ever truly be India’s Ocean? (Prospects for the future ...
Will the Indian Ocean ever truly be India’s Ocean? (Prospects for the future ...
 
Preliminary Explorations at the Cave Complex at Kothaligad, Taluka Karjat, Di...
Preliminary Explorations at the Cave Complex at Kothaligad, Taluka Karjat, Di...Preliminary Explorations at the Cave Complex at Kothaligad, Taluka Karjat, Di...
Preliminary Explorations at the Cave Complex at Kothaligad, Taluka Karjat, Di...
 
Grantha Script: Writing System and Evolutionary History
Grantha Script: Writing System and Evolutionary HistoryGrantha Script: Writing System and Evolutionary History
Grantha Script: Writing System and Evolutionary History
 
The wonderful world of flowers
The wonderful world of flowersThe wonderful world of flowers
The wonderful world of flowers
 
Dogs and people
Dogs and peopleDogs and people
Dogs and people
 
Mathematics of life
Mathematics of lifeMathematics of life
Mathematics of life
 
Mathematics of life
Mathematics of lifeMathematics of life
Mathematics of life
 
Poor rich
Poor richPoor rich
Poor rich
 
Mc donald's calendar_2011
Mc donald's calendar_2011Mc donald's calendar_2011
Mc donald's calendar_2011
 
Flowchart
FlowchartFlowchart
Flowchart
 
Chinesep
ChinesepChinesep
Chinesep
 
Shreeganesh
ShreeganeshShreeganesh
Shreeganesh
 
Think it over
Think it overThink it over
Think it over
 
Daddy's chair
Daddy's chairDaddy's chair
Daddy's chair
 
Daddy's chair
Daddy's chairDaddy's chair
Daddy's chair
 
Galveston treecarving
Galveston treecarvingGalveston treecarving
Galveston treecarving
 

Cur cognition

  • 1. Cur cognition: Do stray dogs have qualitatively different kinds of canine ... http://www.scientificamerican.com/blog/post.cfm?id=cur-cognition-do-st... Bering in Mind - July 16, 2010 Cur cognition: Do stray dogs have qualitatively different kinds of canine minds? By Jesse Bering In previous posts, I’ve discussed my fascination with dogs, such as this recent controversial piece mentioning those good-natured pit bulls whose unearned reputations often precede them because of a few maladjusted, vicious outliers. Yet I’ve never seen anything quite like the canines of Sofia, Bulgaria, from where I’ve just returned after a week of teaching at a cognitive science summer school and from listening to a surfeit of long-forgotten, uplifting '80s pop music, which the weary and unshaven Bulgarian taxi drivers seem to adore to no end. Some recent work by University of Florida psychologist Monique Udell and her colleagues suggests that it’s not just my imagination that stray dogs are special—rather, strays in general may be vastly more different from our pet dogs than we assumed, particularly in their social cognitive functioning. Now, the stray dog situation in Sofia is notoriously problematic. You know you’ve got a problem when a pack of strays breaks into the deer exhibit at your local zoo and “ruthlessly dismembers” almost the entire collection, as happened earlier this year. And given the general sentiment that an organized roundup and euthanasia is out of the question for moral reasons it’s also a very prickly issue among the people living there. (Stray cats are a problem too, but they appear to be kept in check by a lot of very hungry dogs.) Stray dogs are about common as squirrels there, and from what I saw, most of Sofia’s human population also has about as much interest in strays as Americans in the suburbs have in the squirrels living in their backyards. A June, 2010 estimate placed the figures at around 9,500 dogs running loose in the Sofia confines. So when you’ve got that many animals—even man’s best friend—in a relatively small, concentrated city (not to mention one with its own human homeless problems in the form of the ostracized Romanian Roma, or “gypsies”), mass desensitization is sadly inevitable. But this human-dog indifference is even more striking because it appears to be mutual. I watched incredulously as the Sofia strays ambled casually down the sidewalks like proper Bulgarian citizens. They stepped aside politely for human pedestrians before continuing on their way, stopped patiently to look both ways before loping across frantically organized, crowded freeways, mingled with one another at storefronts and had their mangy coats tousled by the whooshing tires of passing commercial trucks while in the midday heat they slept quietly in tree-shaded gutters mere inches from the road. Most of these animals are multigenerational strays, which means that they are the offspring of strays who were the offspring of strays and so on, and on, for many breeding generations. Natural selection must work quickly indeed under such conditions: these are the descendents of the craftiest ancestral dogs of yesteryear Sofia, those who survived puppyhood without being crushed by some juggernaut and who managed to live long enough to pass on their wily natures to their offspring. Too much reliance on humans or interest in human behavior may well be 1 of 5 8/10/2010 5:42 PM
  • 2. Cur cognition: Do stray dogs have qualitatively different kinds of canine ... http://www.scientificamerican.com/blog/post.cfm?id=cur-cognition-do-st... maladaptive to these dogs’ overall genetic interests within this selective context, given the situation. I love my dogs, Gulliver and Uma, and they’re pretty smart as far as dogs go. But they wouldn’t last two minutes on the streets of Sofia. The Sofia strays may be mongrels, and indeed many looked to me more like dingoes or coyotes than they did domestic dogs, but technically they’re every bit as much members of Canis lupus familiaris as your own pampered Lhasa apso or Chocolate Labrador. But perhaps genes alone do not a dog make. In an intriguing study published in March of this year in the journal Animal Behavior , comparative psychologists Monique Udell, Nicole Dorey and Clive Wynn demonstrated that stray dogs reason about human social behavior (and in particular, our intentions) in a fundamentally different way from pet dogs. The importance of Udell’s work can only be understood in relation to previous experimental findings demonstrating that domestic dogs (but not other canine species) comprehend referential meaning and cooperative intent in human behavior. That is to say, in controlled laboratory research, domestic dogs can do things such as understand that a human experimenter is intentionally trying to share his or her knowledge about the location of a hidden food reward by pointing deliberately at one of two containers. Inferring that an indexical point is symbolically about a referential target—something “out there”—instead of just an erect index finger sticking out from a closed fist requires more cognitive sophistication than it may seem at first blush. This is because comprehending such a gesture demands human-like social cognition, namely a precursor to “theory of mind,” which enables us to reason about the abstract, unobservable psychological states driving overt behavior. My cat, Tommy, is great, but when I tried to point out something in the corner of the room to him just now while sitting in my chair, he stood up on his hind legs to smell my fingertip. For human beings, pointing is so much a part of our nature as to seem unremarkable. But if you think about it, it’s not even self-evident why our species points most commonly with its index finger rather than, say, its thumb or pinkie finger. According to a 1994 Journal of Comparative of Psychology article by University of Louisiana at Lafayette psychologist Daniel Povinelli and Louisiana State University anesthesiologist Richard Davis, there is an important anatomical difference between the relaxed postures of the human hand and that of our closet living relative, the chimpanzee. Go ahead, let your wrist go limp and look at your hand from the side, or if you’re too insecure in your own sexuality, just picture Adam’s limp wrist at the moment of creation in Michelangelo’s masterpiece on the Sistine Chapel’s ceiling. See how even in this relaxed state the index finger is slightly extended? By contrast, when chimps do this (hence the anesthesiologist coauthor), their index finger falls naturally in line with their other fingers. Povinelli and Davis reason that this subtle evolutionary change in the morphology of our hands, which occurred after humans and chimpanzees last shared a common ancestor five million to seven million years ago, is at least partially responsible for the fact that human pointing with the index finger is so culturally ubiquitous today. The argument goes something like this. When young infants begin reaching for objects just out of their range, adults are most likely to respond to those reaching attempts and to retrieve the item for the baby when the latter’s index finger is more prominently extended. That is to say, initially, the adult mistakenly reads into the child’s reaching attempt as a communicative gesture on the part of the child. Over time, this dynamic between the child and adult serves to further “pull out” the index finger because the child implicitly learns the behavioral association, so that it slowly becomes a genuine pointing gesture. There are several varieties of pointing, and this is important for understanding why dogs’ comprehension of the gesture matters. Typically, before 18 to 24 months of age, pointing is used to manipulate others’ behaviors only, just like we’ve seen in the description above. If a baby drops her toy on the ground and points to it while looking at you, she’s basically saying, “Well, what are you waiting for, give it to me!” In developmental science jargon, this is called imperative pointing because it’s more or less a demand. But as children’s brains continue to develop, and once they’re able to conceptualize others as conversational partners who have minds that hold information, pointing becomes declarative . The two-year-old now uses pointing to say, “Hey, look over there!” because she recognizes that you’re not aware of whatever it is (the neighbor’s cat in the bushes, the exorbitantly large man eating a snow cone at the park) and wants to share this fascinating information with you. In fact, difficulties with comprehension and production of declarative pointing are often used as diagnostic indicators of autism in young children, a disorder characterized by social cognitive impairments. So this background is important for understanding why the question of whether or not dogs possess the capacity to reason correctly about human pointing is interesting. Several years ago, Duke University psychologist Brian Hare and his colleagues reported some striking evidence showing that domestic dogs performed above chance on a variety of human- guided selection tasks—including studies in which human experimenters pointed to different objects in the room. At the time, these data were interpreted as showing that dogs have human-like social cognition allowing them to understand cooperative intent in humans. In fact, whereas tame wolves fail to score above chance in such studies, domestic dogs even outperform chimpanzees on similar pointing tests, suggesting that we may have more in common psychologically with dogs than with species for which we’re taxonomically (much) more closely related. This prompted Hare to argue that 2 of 5 8/10/2010 5:42 PM
  • 3. Cur cognition: Do stray dogs have qualitatively different kinds of canine ... http://www.scientificamerican.com/blog/post.cfm?id=cur-cognition-do-st... the co-evolution of humans and the domestic dog had created in the latter a genuine ability to reason about human mental states. Anybody who has ever owned or interacted with a dog can probably think up ample anecdotes here, but if not, just think of Lassie’s proverbial message-sharing abilities. Udell and her group in Florida, however, say that these impressive social cognitive abilities in dogs may not represent the “default” canine cognitive system. In their review of this literature on dog social cognition, the authors point out that: The currently available data suggest that populations of dogs differing in [breeding] and in environmental and lifetime pressures might display different behavioral responses to the actions of humans. Despite this fact, the great majority of subjects in studies of the origins of domestic dogs’ human-compatible social cognition have been pet dogs living in human homes, with human-oriented working dogs representing the remainder of the subject pool. In other words, Udell and her coauthors’ contention is similar to arguments made by many researchers studying human psychological evolution—that our ability to make claims about “human nature” are seriously limited by the fact that the data upon which such claims are made are derived almost entirely from middleclass American undergraduate students between 18-22 years of age and recruited from a psychology department subject pool. She’s basically arguing that existing social cognition research on Canis lupus familiaris has largely neglected large demographic swells of the species and therefore does not necessarily paint an entirely accurate portrait of this species’ natural (default) psychological stance. Bulgaria notwithstanding, consider that within the U.S. alone six to eight million dogs can be found living in shelters each year, which is roughly 11 percent of the entire American domestic dog population. Thus, to remedy this theoretical oversight and to address the issue of whether dogs simply inherit the cognitive capacity to think about human behaviors in intentional terms without any developmental experiences in interacting socially with human beings—that is to say, whether or not it’s something “innate” to dog psychology resulting from dogs’ brain evolution coinciding significantly with our own—Udell went to an animal shelter and administered two pointing comprehension studies with a range of dogs that were collected from the streets and classified as strays. Obviously, if these strays performed as well on such tasks as the pet dogs from earlier studies, the innateness claim would be justified. But if not, then developmental experiences with humans—and probably very particular types of experience at that—were contributing to their performing above chance in those prior studies. After a number of training trials to ensure that the subjects understood the basic task demands, the experimenter pointed to one of two cans and then gave the dog the opportunity to respond to this helpful gesture. A “correct” response to the pointing gesture was operationalized as the dog touching or coming within 10cm of the indicated can, and for doing so the animal received a coveted food reinforcer such as a jerky treat. To prevent the dog from simply using its nose to sniff out the reward, both cans were empty—rather, the experimenter simply dropped the food reward on top of the can whenever the subject chose correctly (i.e., selected the pointed-to can). The most significant findings from Udell’s studies were these. Although the strays performed above chance when the experimenter was kneeling on the floor and the tip of the experimenter’s finger was rigidly held 10 cm from the target can, unlike the domestic dogs in prior studies these strays failed to respond correctly to the pointing gesture when such an obvious physical cue was removed. On pointing trials in which the experimenter’s finger was 50 cm from the closest edge of the target container at full extension and then her arm was retracted back to a neutral position before the subject was allowed to make a choice, the strays’ performance fell to chance levels. This distinction is critical for the debate over whether domestic dogs have some semblance of theory of mind, because in the first instance at least, dogs may be using a simple behavioral heuristic such as “pick-the-box-closest-to-the-hand” that does not require human-like social cognition in which they are inferring cooperative intent. In a follow-up study, Udell found some evidence that strays who were afforded additional experience with humans in an exposure condition, which involved play, petting and free exploration of the target area, showed better performance on the pointing task, but still they were less impressive in their comprehension of human pointing than the pet dogs in previous studies. Although the authors acknowledge the limitations of this work (it cannot directly address the evolution of domestic dog social behavior), Udell and her colleagues argue that these findings reveal how: Evidence of differences in the behavior of populations within a subspecies of animals is important when attempting to draw comparative conclusions between species and subspecies of animals … [and so] comparisons between domestic dog populations should consider different forms of response in the presence of different human stimuli instead of searching for evidence of a universal inborn capacity to 3 of 5 8/10/2010 5:42 PM
  • 4. Cur cognition: Do stray dogs have qualitatively different kinds of canine ... http://www.scientificamerican.com/blog/post.cfm?id=cur-cognition-do-st... respond to human gestures. The debate reminds me of some work I was involved with back in graduate school, in which there was some evidence that great apes that had been raised by human beings as though they were human beings actually demonstrated species atypical human-like social cognition (namely, a theory of mind). The core question in all of this is what is the ‘default’ type of social cognition of a dog, of a chimp … of a human being? In any event, now that I’m back to the home-sweet-hominess of Belfast and its ongoing riots, I can’t help but to think back to all of those motley, feral dogs of Sofia. I was ambivalent to learn about the Bulgarian government’s plans to (finally) construct a series of dog kennels to house strays by the end of 2011. On the one hand, getting these animals off the streets is clearly a good thing in many ways, both for their own wellbeing and that of, for example, the zoo animals. And there’s also, of course, the safety and interest of people—there have been several highly publicized attacks by wild dog packs, including fatalities—on children and the elderly in Sofia. On the other hand, most of these dogs are very clever, harmless, disinterested in human beings and surviving in some very complex and novel ecological conditions. Surely these stray dogs, as well as those eking out a living in other major cities in which strays are a notorious problem, present extraordinary scientific opportunities. And from an animal wellbeing perspective, removing these stray dogs from these environmentally rich, if difficult, industrial surroundings and isolating them to physically restricted concrete-and-steel living conditions strikes me as a rather sad prospect, too. I say this having worked at the Broward County Animal Shelter in South Florida the summer after I graduated from high school, where I saw scenes of dogs in distress that haunt me to this day. (Seriously, what kind of person would discard their happy pair of 12-year-old, sweet-faced golden retrievers because their new girlfriend doesn’t “like dogs”?) In fact, embarrassingly, I was fired from that job for spending too much time petting, playing with and otherwise comforting animals that were clearly suffering. True, I should probably have been doing something more productive and useful like cleaning cages or unloading food bags, but I couldn’t help it: the sight of a suffering dog makes me human—oddly, peculiarly, probably more so than the sight of another suffering human being. But I’ll save that curiosity for another post. I found some solace from my poor employee behavior from this 2006 Physiology & Behavior study by Crista Coppola (coauthored by the autistic animal behavior scientist Temple Grandin). The authors point out that when dogs are housed in an animal shelter, they usually experience a severe form of psychological stress caused by exposure to novel or threatening surroundings, separation from attachment objects, unpredictability of external events, lack or loss of control over the environment, and so on. This stress activates their hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and pumps out explosive levels of cortisol, which is the major hormonal indicator of response to stress. Coppola and her colleagues found that, regardless of breed, age of dog and sex, those shelter dogs that received a pleasant “human interaction session” on Day 2 of their incarceration had significantly lower cortisol levels on Day 9—that is to say, the benefits of this simple pet and play session were found a week later, even without any subsequent interaction with human beings during the intervening days. Honestly, this finding brings tears to my eyes: simple human affection is that long-lasting and important for dogs. And Coppola and her coauthors rightly lament that it’s a pity that the vast majority of dog shelters have not instituted routine human interaction sessions with their new, stressed-out arrivals. But the dogs in Coppolo’s study were largely owner-surrendered ex-pets, those who’d learned to be dependent on the kindness of human beings in their lives. Whether these cortisol-relieving effects of human interaction would be replicated in, say, those stray dogs of Sofia is unclear. It’s possible that the effects would be reversed, given their developmental histories on the streets. And so it is that those stray dogs in Bulgaria broke my heart and intrigued me by their adaptedness to their unusual surroundings. Like the Bulgarians, I was left without any easy answers to the problem. It’s a real mess indeed, with people bringing puppies home, failing to neuter them and so on. But, for science’s sake, and particularly for our understanding of the coevolution of human and canine social cognition, I’d personally love to see someone do a research documentary on the feral dogs of Sofia before they’re off the streets and locked up for good behind those new kennel walls. In this column presented by Scientific American Mind magazine, research psychologist Jesse Bering of Queen's University Belfast ponders some of the more obscure aspects of everyday human behavior. Sign up for the RSS feed, visit www.JesseBering.com, friend Dr. Bering on Facebook or follow @JesseBering on Twitter and never miss an installment again. For articles published prior to September 29, 2009, click here: older Bering in Mind columns. Jesse's 4 of 5 8/10/2010 5:42 PM
  • 5. Cur cognition: Do stray dogs have qualitatively different kinds of canine ... http://www.scientificamerican.com/blog/post.cfm?id=cur-cognition-do-st... first book, The Belief Instinct (Norton) [The God Instinct (Nicholas Brealey) in the U.K.], will be published early February, 2011. Image ©iStockphoto.com/TSnowImages Further Reading Mother's Pregnancy Weight Linked to Child's Obesity Speaking in Tones: Music and Language Partner in the Brain Skeleton Key: Bone Cells May Play a Part in Regulating the Body's Metabolism How Does a Heat Wave Affect the Human Body? DNA Drugs Come of Age How Does Sewage Treatment Work? Live Long and Proper: Genetic Factors Associated with Increased Longevity Identified California Proposes New Regulations on Chemicals in Consumer Products 5 of 5 8/10/2010 5:42 PM