SlideShare una empresa de Scribd logo
1 de 7
Paul C Redford. App ref No: 0932329 PhD Research Proposal 18/06/09
Perception and Decision-Making in Young Tennis Players:
Assessment to Development
A recurrent theme in expertise research, stemming from De Groot’s (1965) seminal
work in chess, is that the key characteristics which differentiate experts from novices are the
amount and organisation of knowledge, rather than computational abilities; indeed, recent
neuroimaging work led by Brunel University researchers confirms that experts store domain-
specific visual information in their long-term memory for rapid and efficient retrieval
(Campitelli, Gobet, Head, Buckley, & Parker, 2007). Moreover, those attaining expert status
in chess (i.e., Grandmaster) at the youngest ages began their training at 6½ years of age, on
average (Gobet & Campitelli, 2007).
Expert tennis players are better able to predict shot direction than are novices, even
when presented with limited visual information (e.g., Abernethy, Zawi, & Jackson, 2008;
Huys, Caňal-Bruland, Hagemann, Beek, Smeeton, & Williams, 2009); such experts also
possess greater awareness of the information on which their judgments are based (Jackson &
Mogan, 2007). This ability is crucial to elite tennis performance (Williams, Davids, &
Williams, 1999). Due to varying situational demands, tennis players are required to rapidly
shift attention towards an opponent’s preliminary movements and postural cues. Therefore,
the ability to efficiently and reliably gather, sort, and process perceptual information during
play and respond appropriately under changing situational demands is critical to match
outcome (Caserta & Singer, 2007). Moreover, the use of such explicit knowledge has been
found to improve early development of motor skills in children (Curran & Keele, 1993).
Nonetheless, implicitly-learnt skills (i.e., those learnt without explicit rule formation) are less
likely to break down when the exponent is under pressure (Masters, 1992), thus implicit
perceptual training in sport has become increasingly popular.
Paul C Redford. App ref No: 0932329 PhD Research Proposal 18/06/09
Elite level tennis is played at such speed that temporal pressure often exceeds basic
human information processing constraints (Williams, 2009). For example, elite players often
return serves reaching speeds of 240 km.h1
(men) and 205 km.h1
(women). Despite high
demands, it is often commented that the world’s top players are always in the right place at
the right time. It is generally accepted that the expert’s perceptual advantage results from
enhanced sports-specific cognitive knowledge structures, acquired through years of
deliberate, purposeful practice (Ericsson, Krampe, & Tesch-Romer, 1993; Ericcson, 1996).
As postulated by Ericsson et al. (1993), it requires approximately 10,000 hours practice to
become an expert, which equates to 20 hours per week of quality practice over a 10 year
period. The current challenge is in determining appropriate methods for developing expertise
with maximum efficiency to accelerate this process.
Perceptual ability and consequent decision-making in tennis have successfully been
trained using implicit video-based (Abernethy, Wood, & Parks, 1999; Farrow, Chivers,
Hardingham, & Sachse, 1998; Singer et al., 1994) and explicit instructional (Williams, Ward,
Smeeton, & Allen, 2004) approaches. The development of sport-specific perceptual and
cognitive strategies (e.g. situational awareness, anticipation, and decision making), which
enhance cognitive processing and in turn physical performance, may reduce the time related
pressure of tennis, whilst facilitating perceptual skills acquisition. Despite multiple variables
involved in the outcome of externally paced sports, the primary difference between success
and failure in tennis is often believed to manifest in perceptual capabilities (Caserta & Singer,
2007).
However, studies have typically been confined to young adult participants, despite the
fact that children may be more responsive to such interventions: prioritisation of our sensory
systems undergoes a developmental shift in early years, such that early reliance on tactile-
kinaesthetic information is superseded by visual system predominance at around seven or
Paul C Redford. App ref No: 0932329 PhD Research Proposal 18/06/09
eight years of age (Williams, 1983); this development is mirrored in significant tennis timing
accuracy improvements between the ages of 7 and 10 years (Benguigui & Ripoll, 1998).
The proposed programme of research aims to identify the perceptual and decision-
making characteristics that distinguish stronger from weaker players in a large (N ~ 40) group
of young tennis players aged 7-11 years based at a national HPC, using within- and between-
age group comparisons. This information will be utilised to develop and implement
idiosyncratic perceptual/decision-making training programmes which utilise either implicit or
explicit learning paradigms. It is anticipated that (i) superior attunement to key features of an
opponent’s movements will differentiate young national level tennis players from other age-
matched players, (ii) that a programme targeted at drawing young players’ attention to these
features will improve performance at all levels, and (iii) that implicitly-trained young athletes
will demonstrate superior decision-making under pressure. The unique participant pool offers
the opportunity to obtain cross-sectional and longitudinal/developmental data from a group of
young athletes; something which has hitherto not been done.
Access to young participants has been provisionally granted, pursuant to School
Research Ethics Committee approval. The first stage of the research will comprise semi-
structured interviews with UK national level tennis coaches from a range of HPCs, to identify
features of the visual environment to which they believe young players aged 7-11 years
should be attuned; and the perceptual and DM abilities which differentiate national- from
lower-level players. The information derived will form the basis of cross-sectional large
(~50) group perceptual/DM testing, in which I will use regression analyses to derive the
strongest predictors of successful performance; these elements will consequently be
incorporated into video- and computer-based training programmes, for which data will be
collected longitudinally (c. 1 year). Training will comprise of still and video footage, with
and without feedback (see, Maxwell, Masters, & Eves, 2003). Utilising (a) temporal
Paul C Redford. App ref No: 0932329 PhD Research Proposal 18/06/09
occlusion and neutralisation (see, Huys et al. 2009), (b) point light and natural view footage
(see, Williams et al., 2006), (c) highlighting of key areas (e.g., active limb), and (d) post
retention interval testing. Finally, I shall investigate the dependent measures involved in
expert tennis players’ perception, utilising (i) response accuracy and time (e.g., E-Prime;
reaction mats), and (ii) EMG, to differentiate role of initial response component versus
movement efficiency/inefficiency.
It is anticipated that (i) superior attunement to key features of an opponent’s
movements will differentiate young national level tennis players from other age-matched
players; (ii) that a programme targeted at drawing young players’ attention to these features
will improve performance at all levels; and (iii) that implicitly-trained players will
demonstrate superior DM under pressure.
In conclusion, my research proposal presents the opportunity to obtain high-quality
cross-sectional and longitudinal/developmental data from a group of very young but high-
level tennis players, an age group which is presently under-researched.
Paul C Redford. App ref No: 0932329 PhD Research Proposal 18/06/09
References
Abernethy, B., Wood, J. M., & Parks, S. (1999). Can the anticipatory skills of experts be
learned by novices? Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 70, 313-318.
Abernethy, B., Zawi, K., & Jackson, R. C. (2008). Expertise and attunement to kinematic
constraints. Perception advance online publication.
Benguigui N., & Ripoll H. (1998). Effects of tennis practice on the coincidence timing
accuracy of adults and children. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 69, 217-
223.
Bilalić, M., McLeod, P., & Gobet, F. (2007). Does chess need intelligence? — A study with
young chess players. Intelligence, 35, 457–470.
Campitelli, G., Gobet, F., Head, K., Buckley, M., & Parker, A. (2007). Brain localisation of
memory chunks in chessplayers. International Journal of Neuroscience, 117, 1641-
1659.
Caserta, R. J., & Singer, R. N. (2007). The Effectiveness of Situational Awareness Learning
in Response to Video Tennis Match Situations. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology,
19, 125-141.
Curran, T., & Keele, S. W. (1993). Attentional and nonattentional forms of sequence
learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 19,
189-202.
De Groot, A. (1965). Thought and choice in chess. The Hague: Mouton.
Ericcson, K. A. (1996). The road to excellence. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in
the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100 , 363-406.
Paul C Redford. App ref No: 0932329 PhD Research Proposal 18/06/09
Farrow, D., Chivers, P., Hardingham, C., & Sachse, S. (1998). The effect of video-based
perceptual training on the tennis return of serve. International Journal of Sport
Psychology, 29, 231–242.
Gobet. F., & Campitelli, G. (2007). The role of domain-specific practice, handedness, and
starting age in chess. Developmental Psychology, 43, 159-172.
Huys, R., Canal-Bruland, R., Hagemann, N., Beek, P.J., Smeeton, N.J. & Williams, A.M.
(2009) Global information pickup underpins anticipation of tennis shot direction.
Journal of Motor Behavior, 41, (2). pp. 158-170.
Jackson R. C., & Mogan, P. (2007). Advance visual information awareness and anticipation
skill. Journal of Motor Behavior, 39, 341-351.
Masters, R.S.W. (1992). Knowledge, knerves and know-how: The role of explicit versus
implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. British
Journal of Psychology, 83, 343-358.
Maxwell, J. P., Masters, R. S. W. & Eves, F. F. (2003) The role of working memory in motor
learning and performance. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 376-402.
Nideffer, R. M. (1976). Test of attentional and interpersonal style. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 34, 394-404.
Singer, R. N., Caraugh, J. H., Chen, D., Steinberg, G. M., Frehlich, S. G., & Wang, L. (1994).
Training mental quickness in beginning/intermediate tennis players. The Sport
Psychologist, 8, 305-318.
Williams, A. M. (2009). Anticipation and skill in racket sports. In A. Lees, D. Cabello, & G.
Torres, Science and Racket Sports IV (pp. 145-153). Routledge.
Williams, A. M., Davids, K., & Williams, J. G. (1999). Visual Perception and Action in
Sport. London: E. & F.N Spon.
Paul C Redford. App ref No: 0932329 PhD Research Proposal 18/06/09
Williams, A.M., Ward, P., Smeeton, N.J. & Allen, D. (2004). Developing anticipation skills
in tennis using on-court instruction: Perception versus perception and action. Journal of
Applied Sport Psychology, 16, 350-360.
Williams, H.G. (1983). Perceptual and Motor Development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Más contenido relacionado

Similar a PerceptionandDecisionMakinginYoungTennisPlayersPhDResearchProposalPaulCRedford

Dissertation Paper
Dissertation PaperDissertation Paper
Dissertation PaperJames McCabe
 
Skilled perception Duijst & Duijst
Skilled perception Duijst & DuijstSkilled perception Duijst & Duijst
Skilled perception Duijst & DuijstEdwin Duijst
 
Berghorn finall capstonepresentation (1)
Berghorn finall capstonepresentation (1)Berghorn finall capstonepresentation (1)
Berghorn finall capstonepresentation (1)berghorn
 
Perfiles físicos de jugadores adolescentes irlandeses de rugby XV
Perfiles físicos de jugadores adolescentes irlandeses de rugby XVPerfiles físicos de jugadores adolescentes irlandeses de rugby XV
Perfiles físicos de jugadores adolescentes irlandeses de rugby XVEscuelaNacionalEntrenadoresFER
 
SP3022 Dissertation Complete
SP3022 Dissertation CompleteSP3022 Dissertation Complete
SP3022 Dissertation CompleteGreg Palin
 
F:\Cd2009\Projectphoto\Projectfootball\Project Hasib
F:\Cd2009\Projectphoto\Projectfootball\Project HasibF:\Cd2009\Projectphoto\Projectfootball\Project Hasib
F:\Cd2009\Projectphoto\Projectfootball\Project Hasibbksp
 
Project Hasib
Project  HasibProject  Hasib
Project Hasibbksp
 
Mental Imagery and Its Effect on Learning Skills
Mental Imagery and Its Effect on Learning SkillsMental Imagery and Its Effect on Learning Skills
Mental Imagery and Its Effect on Learning SkillsChristine Chin
 
Archives of Sports Medicine and Physiotherapy
Archives of Sports Medicine and PhysiotherapyArchives of Sports Medicine and Physiotherapy
Archives of Sports Medicine and Physiotherapypeertechzpublication
 
AR-sir-julius-queddeng.docx
AR-sir-julius-queddeng.docxAR-sir-julius-queddeng.docx
AR-sir-julius-queddeng.docxDENNISRAMIREZ39
 
Executive functions predict the success of top-soccer players
Executive functions predict the success of top-soccer playersExecutive functions predict the success of top-soccer players
Executive functions predict the success of top-soccer playersSportNetworking
 
THE INFLUENCE OF BALL-TOUCHES NUMBER ON PHYSICAL AND PHYSIOLOGICAL DEMANDS OF...
THE INFLUENCE OF BALL-TOUCHES NUMBER ON PHYSICAL AND PHYSIOLOGICAL DEMANDS OF...THE INFLUENCE OF BALL-TOUCHES NUMBER ON PHYSICAL AND PHYSIOLOGICAL DEMANDS OF...
THE INFLUENCE OF BALL-TOUCHES NUMBER ON PHYSICAL AND PHYSIOLOGICAL DEMANDS OF...Ángel Aceña Rodríguez
 
The importance of a sport specific stimulus for training agility
The importance of a sport specific stimulus for training agilityThe importance of a sport specific stimulus for training agility
The importance of a sport specific stimulus for training agilityFernando Farias
 
Motor Behavior Final Project
Motor Behavior Final ProjectMotor Behavior Final Project
Motor Behavior Final ProjectRobGoldPE
 
(2013) Comparison of tactical behaviour efficiency between U-12 and U-13 yout...
(2013) Comparison of tactical behaviour efficiency between U-12 and U-13 yout...(2013) Comparison of tactical behaviour efficiency between U-12 and U-13 yout...
(2013) Comparison of tactical behaviour efficiency between U-12 and U-13 yout...Rodrigo Santos
 
Brendan_updated thesis final 130912
Brendan_updated thesis final 130912Brendan_updated thesis final 130912
Brendan_updated thesis final 130912Brendan Henderson
 

Similar a PerceptionandDecisionMakinginYoungTennisPlayersPhDResearchProposalPaulCRedford (20)

Dissertation Paper
Dissertation PaperDissertation Paper
Dissertation Paper
 
Skilled perception Duijst & Duijst
Skilled perception Duijst & DuijstSkilled perception Duijst & Duijst
Skilled perception Duijst & Duijst
 
Berghorn finall capstonepresentation (1)
Berghorn finall capstonepresentation (1)Berghorn finall capstonepresentation (1)
Berghorn finall capstonepresentation (1)
 
Demandas físicas de jugadores de rugby league
Demandas físicas de jugadores de rugby leagueDemandas físicas de jugadores de rugby league
Demandas físicas de jugadores de rugby league
 
Perfiles físicos de jugadores adolescentes irlandeses de rugby XV
Perfiles físicos de jugadores adolescentes irlandeses de rugby XVPerfiles físicos de jugadores adolescentes irlandeses de rugby XV
Perfiles físicos de jugadores adolescentes irlandeses de rugby XV
 
International Journal of Sports Science & Medicine
International Journal of Sports Science & MedicineInternational Journal of Sports Science & Medicine
International Journal of Sports Science & Medicine
 
SP3022 Dissertation Complete
SP3022 Dissertation CompleteSP3022 Dissertation Complete
SP3022 Dissertation Complete
 
Acta Gymnica
Acta GymnicaActa Gymnica
Acta Gymnica
 
F:\Cd2009\Projectphoto\Projectfootball\Project Hasib
F:\Cd2009\Projectphoto\Projectfootball\Project HasibF:\Cd2009\Projectphoto\Projectfootball\Project Hasib
F:\Cd2009\Projectphoto\Projectfootball\Project Hasib
 
Project Hasib
Project  HasibProject  Hasib
Project Hasib
 
Mental Imagery and Its Effect on Learning Skills
Mental Imagery and Its Effect on Learning SkillsMental Imagery and Its Effect on Learning Skills
Mental Imagery and Its Effect on Learning Skills
 
Archives of Sports Medicine and Physiotherapy
Archives of Sports Medicine and PhysiotherapyArchives of Sports Medicine and Physiotherapy
Archives of Sports Medicine and Physiotherapy
 
AR-sir-julius-queddeng.docx
AR-sir-julius-queddeng.docxAR-sir-julius-queddeng.docx
AR-sir-julius-queddeng.docx
 
Executive functions predict the success of top-soccer players
Executive functions predict the success of top-soccer playersExecutive functions predict the success of top-soccer players
Executive functions predict the success of top-soccer players
 
THE INFLUENCE OF BALL-TOUCHES NUMBER ON PHYSICAL AND PHYSIOLOGICAL DEMANDS OF...
THE INFLUENCE OF BALL-TOUCHES NUMBER ON PHYSICAL AND PHYSIOLOGICAL DEMANDS OF...THE INFLUENCE OF BALL-TOUCHES NUMBER ON PHYSICAL AND PHYSIOLOGICAL DEMANDS OF...
THE INFLUENCE OF BALL-TOUCHES NUMBER ON PHYSICAL AND PHYSIOLOGICAL DEMANDS OF...
 
The importance of a sport specific stimulus for training agility
The importance of a sport specific stimulus for training agilityThe importance of a sport specific stimulus for training agility
The importance of a sport specific stimulus for training agility
 
Motor Behavior Final Project
Motor Behavior Final ProjectMotor Behavior Final Project
Motor Behavior Final Project
 
Soccer Fitness
Soccer FitnessSoccer Fitness
Soccer Fitness
 
(2013) Comparison of tactical behaviour efficiency between U-12 and U-13 yout...
(2013) Comparison of tactical behaviour efficiency between U-12 and U-13 yout...(2013) Comparison of tactical behaviour efficiency between U-12 and U-13 yout...
(2013) Comparison of tactical behaviour efficiency between U-12 and U-13 yout...
 
Brendan_updated thesis final 130912
Brendan_updated thesis final 130912Brendan_updated thesis final 130912
Brendan_updated thesis final 130912
 

PerceptionandDecisionMakinginYoungTennisPlayersPhDResearchProposalPaulCRedford

  • 1. Paul C Redford. App ref No: 0932329 PhD Research Proposal 18/06/09 Perception and Decision-Making in Young Tennis Players: Assessment to Development A recurrent theme in expertise research, stemming from De Groot’s (1965) seminal work in chess, is that the key characteristics which differentiate experts from novices are the amount and organisation of knowledge, rather than computational abilities; indeed, recent neuroimaging work led by Brunel University researchers confirms that experts store domain- specific visual information in their long-term memory for rapid and efficient retrieval (Campitelli, Gobet, Head, Buckley, & Parker, 2007). Moreover, those attaining expert status in chess (i.e., Grandmaster) at the youngest ages began their training at 6½ years of age, on average (Gobet & Campitelli, 2007). Expert tennis players are better able to predict shot direction than are novices, even when presented with limited visual information (e.g., Abernethy, Zawi, & Jackson, 2008; Huys, Caňal-Bruland, Hagemann, Beek, Smeeton, & Williams, 2009); such experts also possess greater awareness of the information on which their judgments are based (Jackson & Mogan, 2007). This ability is crucial to elite tennis performance (Williams, Davids, & Williams, 1999). Due to varying situational demands, tennis players are required to rapidly shift attention towards an opponent’s preliminary movements and postural cues. Therefore, the ability to efficiently and reliably gather, sort, and process perceptual information during play and respond appropriately under changing situational demands is critical to match outcome (Caserta & Singer, 2007). Moreover, the use of such explicit knowledge has been found to improve early development of motor skills in children (Curran & Keele, 1993). Nonetheless, implicitly-learnt skills (i.e., those learnt without explicit rule formation) are less likely to break down when the exponent is under pressure (Masters, 1992), thus implicit perceptual training in sport has become increasingly popular.
  • 2. Paul C Redford. App ref No: 0932329 PhD Research Proposal 18/06/09 Elite level tennis is played at such speed that temporal pressure often exceeds basic human information processing constraints (Williams, 2009). For example, elite players often return serves reaching speeds of 240 km.h1 (men) and 205 km.h1 (women). Despite high demands, it is often commented that the world’s top players are always in the right place at the right time. It is generally accepted that the expert’s perceptual advantage results from enhanced sports-specific cognitive knowledge structures, acquired through years of deliberate, purposeful practice (Ericsson, Krampe, & Tesch-Romer, 1993; Ericcson, 1996). As postulated by Ericsson et al. (1993), it requires approximately 10,000 hours practice to become an expert, which equates to 20 hours per week of quality practice over a 10 year period. The current challenge is in determining appropriate methods for developing expertise with maximum efficiency to accelerate this process. Perceptual ability and consequent decision-making in tennis have successfully been trained using implicit video-based (Abernethy, Wood, & Parks, 1999; Farrow, Chivers, Hardingham, & Sachse, 1998; Singer et al., 1994) and explicit instructional (Williams, Ward, Smeeton, & Allen, 2004) approaches. The development of sport-specific perceptual and cognitive strategies (e.g. situational awareness, anticipation, and decision making), which enhance cognitive processing and in turn physical performance, may reduce the time related pressure of tennis, whilst facilitating perceptual skills acquisition. Despite multiple variables involved in the outcome of externally paced sports, the primary difference between success and failure in tennis is often believed to manifest in perceptual capabilities (Caserta & Singer, 2007). However, studies have typically been confined to young adult participants, despite the fact that children may be more responsive to such interventions: prioritisation of our sensory systems undergoes a developmental shift in early years, such that early reliance on tactile- kinaesthetic information is superseded by visual system predominance at around seven or
  • 3. Paul C Redford. App ref No: 0932329 PhD Research Proposal 18/06/09 eight years of age (Williams, 1983); this development is mirrored in significant tennis timing accuracy improvements between the ages of 7 and 10 years (Benguigui & Ripoll, 1998). The proposed programme of research aims to identify the perceptual and decision- making characteristics that distinguish stronger from weaker players in a large (N ~ 40) group of young tennis players aged 7-11 years based at a national HPC, using within- and between- age group comparisons. This information will be utilised to develop and implement idiosyncratic perceptual/decision-making training programmes which utilise either implicit or explicit learning paradigms. It is anticipated that (i) superior attunement to key features of an opponent’s movements will differentiate young national level tennis players from other age- matched players, (ii) that a programme targeted at drawing young players’ attention to these features will improve performance at all levels, and (iii) that implicitly-trained young athletes will demonstrate superior decision-making under pressure. The unique participant pool offers the opportunity to obtain cross-sectional and longitudinal/developmental data from a group of young athletes; something which has hitherto not been done. Access to young participants has been provisionally granted, pursuant to School Research Ethics Committee approval. The first stage of the research will comprise semi- structured interviews with UK national level tennis coaches from a range of HPCs, to identify features of the visual environment to which they believe young players aged 7-11 years should be attuned; and the perceptual and DM abilities which differentiate national- from lower-level players. The information derived will form the basis of cross-sectional large (~50) group perceptual/DM testing, in which I will use regression analyses to derive the strongest predictors of successful performance; these elements will consequently be incorporated into video- and computer-based training programmes, for which data will be collected longitudinally (c. 1 year). Training will comprise of still and video footage, with and without feedback (see, Maxwell, Masters, & Eves, 2003). Utilising (a) temporal
  • 4. Paul C Redford. App ref No: 0932329 PhD Research Proposal 18/06/09 occlusion and neutralisation (see, Huys et al. 2009), (b) point light and natural view footage (see, Williams et al., 2006), (c) highlighting of key areas (e.g., active limb), and (d) post retention interval testing. Finally, I shall investigate the dependent measures involved in expert tennis players’ perception, utilising (i) response accuracy and time (e.g., E-Prime; reaction mats), and (ii) EMG, to differentiate role of initial response component versus movement efficiency/inefficiency. It is anticipated that (i) superior attunement to key features of an opponent’s movements will differentiate young national level tennis players from other age-matched players; (ii) that a programme targeted at drawing young players’ attention to these features will improve performance at all levels; and (iii) that implicitly-trained players will demonstrate superior DM under pressure. In conclusion, my research proposal presents the opportunity to obtain high-quality cross-sectional and longitudinal/developmental data from a group of very young but high- level tennis players, an age group which is presently under-researched.
  • 5. Paul C Redford. App ref No: 0932329 PhD Research Proposal 18/06/09 References Abernethy, B., Wood, J. M., & Parks, S. (1999). Can the anticipatory skills of experts be learned by novices? Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 70, 313-318. Abernethy, B., Zawi, K., & Jackson, R. C. (2008). Expertise and attunement to kinematic constraints. Perception advance online publication. Benguigui N., & Ripoll H. (1998). Effects of tennis practice on the coincidence timing accuracy of adults and children. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 69, 217- 223. Bilalić, M., McLeod, P., & Gobet, F. (2007). Does chess need intelligence? — A study with young chess players. Intelligence, 35, 457–470. Campitelli, G., Gobet, F., Head, K., Buckley, M., & Parker, A. (2007). Brain localisation of memory chunks in chessplayers. International Journal of Neuroscience, 117, 1641- 1659. Caserta, R. J., & Singer, R. N. (2007). The Effectiveness of Situational Awareness Learning in Response to Video Tennis Match Situations. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 19, 125-141. Curran, T., & Keele, S. W. (1993). Attentional and nonattentional forms of sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 19, 189-202. De Groot, A. (1965). Thought and choice in chess. The Hague: Mouton. Ericcson, K. A. (1996). The road to excellence. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100 , 363-406.
  • 6. Paul C Redford. App ref No: 0932329 PhD Research Proposal 18/06/09 Farrow, D., Chivers, P., Hardingham, C., & Sachse, S. (1998). The effect of video-based perceptual training on the tennis return of serve. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 29, 231–242. Gobet. F., & Campitelli, G. (2007). The role of domain-specific practice, handedness, and starting age in chess. Developmental Psychology, 43, 159-172. Huys, R., Canal-Bruland, R., Hagemann, N., Beek, P.J., Smeeton, N.J. & Williams, A.M. (2009) Global information pickup underpins anticipation of tennis shot direction. Journal of Motor Behavior, 41, (2). pp. 158-170. Jackson R. C., & Mogan, P. (2007). Advance visual information awareness and anticipation skill. Journal of Motor Behavior, 39, 341-351. Masters, R.S.W. (1992). Knowledge, knerves and know-how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 343-358. Maxwell, J. P., Masters, R. S. W. & Eves, F. F. (2003) The role of working memory in motor learning and performance. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 376-402. Nideffer, R. M. (1976). Test of attentional and interpersonal style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 394-404. Singer, R. N., Caraugh, J. H., Chen, D., Steinberg, G. M., Frehlich, S. G., & Wang, L. (1994). Training mental quickness in beginning/intermediate tennis players. The Sport Psychologist, 8, 305-318. Williams, A. M. (2009). Anticipation and skill in racket sports. In A. Lees, D. Cabello, & G. Torres, Science and Racket Sports IV (pp. 145-153). Routledge. Williams, A. M., Davids, K., & Williams, J. G. (1999). Visual Perception and Action in Sport. London: E. & F.N Spon.
  • 7. Paul C Redford. App ref No: 0932329 PhD Research Proposal 18/06/09 Williams, A.M., Ward, P., Smeeton, N.J. & Allen, D. (2004). Developing anticipation skills in tennis using on-court instruction: Perception versus perception and action. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 16, 350-360. Williams, H.G. (1983). Perceptual and Motor Development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.