SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 17
Download to read offline
Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37                 www.brill.nl/soan




Can Attitudes About Animal Neglect Be Differentiated
        From Attitudes About Animal Abuse?


                                          Bill C. Henry
     Associate Professor, Department of Psychology, Metropolitan State College of Denver,
                           PO Box 173362, Denver, CO 80217, USA
                                     henrybi@mscd.edu
                          Sent 18 July 2008, Accepted 25 September 2008



Abstract
The past decade has seen an increase in interest relating to the correlates and determinants of
attitudes about nonhuman animals, especially attitudes about the use or abuse of animals. How-
ever, little research has explicitly addressed individual differences in attitudes about the neglect
of animals. The current study employs a factor-analytic approach to explore (a) whether attitudes
about animal neglect can be reliably differentiated from attitudes about animal abuse and
(b) whether the relationship between attitudes about animal neglect and animal abuse differs as
a function of gender. Results indicated that attitudes about abuse and neglect can be reliably
differentiated among both men and women. However, the structure of these attitudes appears to
differ substantially by sex. This paper discusses theoretical and practical implications of these
results.

Keywords
abuse, animals, attitudes, factor structure, neglect



Introduction
Recent decades have seen an increase in interest relating to the correlates and
determinants of attitudes about nonhuman animals (Knight, Vrij, Cherry-
man, & Nunkoosing, 2004; Pagani, Robustelli & Ascione, 2007; Signal &
Taylor, 2006). Of particular interest within this area of study are factors relat-
ing to attitudes about cruelty toward animals. A variety of studies have exam-
ined how various factors influence people’s judgments about cruelty. For
example, Allen et al. (2002) presented participants with a scenario describing
an act of cruelty; within the scenario, the type of animal victimized was varied.
In addition, the mood of the participant was manipulated. Results indicated
that participants in a positive mood-state recommended harsher punishments

© Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2009                               DOI: 10.1163/156853009X393747
22                B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37

for the perpetrator of the abuse. Participants also recommended harsher pun-
ishment when the animal-victim was perceived as being more similar to
humans (primate as compared to reptile), Similarly, Hills and Lalich (1998)
found that participants rated perpetrators of animal maltreatment as more
cruel if the perpetrators were depicted as being aware—rather than unaware—
of an animal’s suffering. Sims, Chin, and Yordon (2007) reported that women
recommended harsher punishments for acts of animal abuse than did men
and that recommended punishments were harsher when the victim was a
puppy compared to when the victim was a chicken.
   While a significant body of research has examined attitudes and beliefs
about cruelty, very little research has addressed attitudes about a related topic:
the neglect of animals. One study that did address the difference in attitudes
about cruelty versus neglect was Sims et al. (2007). As described above, Sims
et al. presented participants with a scenario describing acts of maltreatment
against either a puppy or a chicken. In addition, the nature of the maltreat-
ment was varied. One set of scenarios described maltreatment as a presumably
neglectful behavior (the animal was described as being “thin and visibly under-
weight”); another set of scenarios described the maltreatment as an overt act
of cruelty (the animal was described as being “beaten and severely injured”).
However, no differences were found in recommendations for punishment as a
function of the nature of the maltreatment. It appeared that in the Sims et al.
study, participants were not differentiating between starving an animal and
beating an animal.
   In contrast, Henry (2006) found that participants did in fact differentiate
between acts of cruelty and acts that could be interpreted as neglectful. Henry
conducted a factor analysis of the Attitudes Toward the Treatment of Animals
Scale (ATTAS), a 23-item scale assessing participants’ responses to a variety of
types of treatments of animals. The factor analysis revealed three factors emerg-
ing from the 23 items of the scale. One factor was labeled “Utilitarian” and
reflected differences in peoples’ attitudes about the utilitarian use of animals
(using animals for food and clothing). A second factor was labeled “Cruelty”
and reflected differences in peoples’ attitudes about the intentional malicious
abuse of animals (intentionally killing or injuring animals). A third factor was
labeled “Caregiving” and reflected differences in peoples’ attitudes about the
appropriate care of animals (providing food, water, medical care).
   Although Henry (2006) labeled this last factor “Caregiving,” an inspection
of the items loading on that factor indicates that a better label might be
“Neglect.” In general, the items assessed attitudes about failures to provide
minimal levels of care to animals. Thus, within the sample examined by Henry,
it appeared that a distinction was being drawn between overt acts of cruelty
and behaviors that were considered neglectful.
B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37           23

   It should be noted that these two factors (cruelty and neglect), while dis-
tinct, were significantly correlated. Henry (2006) reported that the correlation
between the Cruelty and Caregiving/Neglect factor was 0.60. In addition, it
should be noted that Henry did not conduct the factor analysis of the ATTAS
separately by sex. Thus, potential differences in the factor-structure of atti-
tudes among men and women were obscured.
   The studies reviewed thus far raise a number of intriguing questions. First,
do cruelty and neglect of animals really constitute distinct factors? And if so,
why? What are the bases upon which people would draw a distinction between
acts of cruelty and acts of neglect? Second, given the substantial body of
research indicating that women are more empathetic toward animals than are
men (Herzog, 2007), do women and men construe the relation between acts
of cruelty and acts of neglect differently? The current study sought to use a
factor-analytic approach to explore the relation between attitudes about ani-
mal cruelty and animal neglect separately by sex.


Methods

Participants
Participants in this study were 683 students enrolled in sections of Introduc-
tion to Psychology. Research participation was a requirement of the course.
Of the participants, 333 (48.8%) were men; 350 (51.2%) were women. The
mean age of participants was 22.64 years (SD = 6.25) with a range of 17 to 57
years. Seventy % of participants identified themselves as White; 13%, His-
panic; 15%, Black; 10%, Asian; 4%, American Indian/Alaska Native; 2%,
Pacific Islander; and 4%, Other. These percentages sum to more than 100%
because participants could identify themselves as members of more than one
race/ethnicity group.

Materials
Attitudes toward the treatment of animals survey (ATTAS). Participants com-
pleted the ATTAS as part of a broader study examining human-animal inter-
actions. The ATTAS is a 23-item attitude scale used to assess attitudes
regarding the treatment of animals (Henry, 2004a, 2004b, 2006). Participants
were asked to indicate the extent to which they would be bothered by thinking
about a particular type of treatment of an animal. Each item was phrased,
“How much would it bother you to think about. . . .” Items assessed a variety
of types of treatment of animals—such as (a) failing to provide adequate food,
shelter, or medical care; (b) using animals in medical research; (c) encouraging
24                 B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37

animals to fight; and (d) killing or hurting an animal for no apparent reason.
Items assessed attitudes pertaining to the following:
     1. companion animals (pet dogs, cats, and rabbits);
     2. domestic stock animals (horses, cows, and pigs); and
     3. animals in the wild (deer, rabbit, and squirrel).
Participants responded to each item on a 5-point scale ranging from 1-5:
“None at all” to “A lot.” Thus, higher scores reflected relatively more discom-
fort with the type of treatment specified.
   As described above, a previous factor analysis of the ATTAS (Henry, 2006)
revealed that the items on the survey divided into three subscales:
     1. Cruelty (eight items);
     2. Utilitarian (eight items); and
     3. Caregiving/Neglect (seven items).
Because the purpose of this study was to explore the relation between attitudes
about cruelty and attitudes about neglect, only the 15 items relating to cruelty
and neglect were analyzed here. Those 15 items are listed in Table 1. The eight
items on the Cruelty subscale assessed individual differences in attitudes
regarding the intentional harming of animals for no apparent reason. High
scores on these items reflected discomfort with such acts of pointless harm to
animals. The seven items on the Caregiving/Neglect subscale assessed indi-
vidual differences in attitudes regarding a person’s responsibilities for ensuring
the safety and well being of an animal. High scores on these items reflected
discomfort with the failure to meet the basic needs of an animal.
   On the Cruelty items, women (Mean = 4.48, SD = 0.60) scored higher
than did men (Mean = 3.93, SD = 0.77), t (681) = -10.32, p < .01.) Likewise,
on the Caregiving/Neglect items, women (Mean = 4.42, SD = 0.53) scored
higher than did men (Mean = 3.83, SD = 0.73), t (681) = -12.27, p < .01.) Age
was not correlated with scores on either the Cruelty items (r = 0.07, ns) or the
Caregiving/neglect items (r = 0.03, ns)


Results

Bivariate Correlations
As mentioned in the Introduction, one rationale for undertaking these analy-
ses was that previous research had revealed strong correlations between scores
on the Cruelty and Caregiving/Neglect subscales of the ATTAS. The same was
B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37            25

  Table 1. Items Loading on the Cruelty and Neglect Subscales of the
                       ATTAS (Henry, 2006)
Cruelty
 1 How much would it bother you to think about someone intentionally
killing a domestic stock animal (e.g., horse, cow, pig, etc.) other than for food
or to help the animal because it was hurt, old, or sick?
 2 How much would it bother you to think about someone intentionally
killing a wild animal (e.g., deer, rabbit, squirrel, etc.) other than for food,
while hunting, or to help the animal because it was hurt or sick?
 3 How much would it bother you to think about someone intentionally
killing a companion animal (e.g., pet dog, cat, rabbit, etc.) other than to help
the animal because it was hurt, old or sick?
  8 How much would it bother you to think about someone intentionally
hurting a domestic stock animal (e.g., horse, cow, pig, etc.) other than for
training, branding, etc?
 9 How much would it bother you to think about someone intentionally
hurting a wild animal (e.g., deer, rabbit, squirrel, etc.)?
10 How much would it bother you to think about someone intentionally
hurting a companion animal (e.g., pet dog, cat, rabbit, etc.) other than for
training?
13 How much would it bother you to think about someone using dogs or
cats in research that results in serious injury, illness, or death of the animal?
23 How much would it bother you to think about someone intentionally
encouraging or causing animals to fight one another (e.g., dog fighting, cock
fighting, etc.)?
Caregiving/Neglect
  7 How much would it bother you to think about someone intentionally
killing (i.e., euthanizing) a companion animal or domestic stock animal
because the owner is unable to care for it (e.g., the person is moving out of
state and cannot take the animal to their new home.)?
11 How much would it bother you to think about someone having sexual
contact with an animal?
26                 B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37

Table 1. (cont.)
16 How much would it bother you to think about someone failing to provide
medical care for a companion animal that is clearly injured or ill?
17 How much would it bother you to think about someone failing to pro-
vide domestic stock animals or companion animals with food or water for
24 hours?
18 How much would it bother you to think about someone leaving domestic
stock animals outside without shelter for 24 hours?
19 How much would it bother you to think about someone leaving compan-
ion animals outside without shelter for 24 hours?
20 How much would it bother you to think about someone leaving a com-
panion animal in a locked car with the windows cracked with an outside
temperature of 70° for one hour?

true of scores within this sample. Among men, the correlation between the
two subscales was r = 0.62 (p < .01); among women, the correlation was
r = 0.51 (p < .01). To further examine the reasons for the substantial overlap
between the Cruelty and Caregiving/Neglect items, principle components fac-
tor-analyses of the 15 items (separately by sex) were conducted.

Factor Structure of Neglect and Cruelty Items Among Men
The 15 cruelty and neglect items were entered into a principle components
factor analysis with varimax rotation. Factors with eigenvalues greater that
1.00 were selected. Three factors emerged, accounting for 58.8% of the vari-
ability in responses. Factor loadings for the items are presented in Table 2. In
Table 2, factor loadings of .30 or greater are in bold. The items loading on
each of the three factors are presented graphically in Figure 1.
   A number of things are worth noting in this analysis. First, consistent with
the findings of Henry (2006), items relating to neglect tended to load on a
separate factor more than did items relating to cruelty. Three neglect items in
particular (“leaving domestic stock animals outside without shelter,” “leaving
companion animals outside without shelter,” and “leaving a companion ani-
mal in a locked car”) had very strong loadings on Factor II and weak loadings
on Factors I and III.
   Likewise, three cruelty items in particular (“intentionally killing a domestic
stock animal,” “intentionally killing a wild animal,” and “intentionally killing
B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37             27

                   Table 2. Three Factor Solution for Men
                                                                      I   II    III
  1 Intentionally killing a domestic stock animal (e.g.,          .28     .09   .85
horse, cow, pig, etc.) other than for food or to help the
animal because it was hurt, old, or sick?
  2 Intentionally killing a wild animal (e.g., deer, rabbit,      .27     .10   .84
squirrel, etc.) other than for food, while hunting, or to
help the animal because it was hurt or sick?
  3 Intentionally killing a companion animal (e.g., pet           .12     .15   .80
dog, cat, rabbit,etc.) other than to help the animal
because it was hurt, old or sick?
  7 Intentionally killing (i.e., euthanizing) a companion         .64     .08   .16
animal or domestic stock animal because the owner is
unable to care for it (e.g., the person is moving out of
state and cannot take the animal to their new home.)?
  8 Intentionally hurting a domestic stock animal (e.g.,          .76     .14   .23
horse, cow, pig, etc.) other than for training, branding, etc?
  9 Intentionally hurting a wild animal (e.g., deer, rab-         .77     .09   .22
bit, squirrel, etc.)?
10 Intentionally hurting a companion animal (e.g., pet            .77     .17   .14
dog, cat, rabbit, etc.) other than for training?
11 Having sexual contact with an animal?                          .25     .17   .25
13 Using dogs or cats in research that results in serious         .52     .40   .15
injury, illness, or death of the animal?
16 Failing to provide medical care for a companion ani-           .59     .48   .13
mal that is clearly injured or ill?
17 Failing to provide domestic stock animals or com-              .44     .61   .15
panion animals with food or water for 24 hours?
18 Leaving domestic stock animals outside without                 .07     .89   .13
shelter for 24 hours?
19 Leaving companion animals outside without shelter              .08     .86   .09
for 24 hours?
20 Leaving a companion animal in a locked car with                .27     .59   .09
the windows cracked with an outside temperature of 70°
for one hour?
23 Intentionally encouraging or causing animals to fight           .52     .30   .25
one another (e.g., dog fighting, cock fighting, etc.)?
28                       B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37

Factor III
Killing stock
Killing wild animal
Killing companion
        animal




                          Factor I
                          Euthanizing companion
                                     animal
                          Hurting stock
                          Hurting wild animal
                          Hurting companion
                                     animal                        Factor II
                                                  Research
                                                  Medical care
                                                  Food and water
                                                  Fighting
                                                                     Leaving stock outside
                                                                     Leaving companion animals outside
                                                                     Leaving companion animal in locked car


                      Figure 1. Items loading on Factors I, II, and III (men)

a companion animal”) had very strong loadings on Factor III and weak load-
ings on Factors I and II. Finally, four items (“euthanizing a companion animal
or stock animal,” “intentionally hurting a domestic stock animal,” “intention-
ally hurting a wild animal,” and “intentionally hurting a companion animal”)
had strong loadings on Factor I and weak loadings on Factors II and III.
   Thus, in this analysis, men appeared to differentiate between different types
of treatments of animals on the basis of the severity of harm suffered by the
animals. Factor III appears to capture those items in which the animal suffers
death as a result of an intentional, malicious act. With the exception of the
item relating to euthanizing an animal, Factor I appears to capture those items
in which the animal is hurt but not killed. It is possible that men were likely
to judge euthanasia less harshly because, even though it involves the death of
the animal, it lacks the malicious nature of the items loading on Factor III.
Factor II appears to capture those items in which there is the possibility of
harm to the animal due to the neglect of the animal but in which the harm is
not immediate.
   It is worth noting the four items that loaded both on the “harm” factor
(Factor I) and the neglect (or “possible harm”) factor (Factor II). Three of these
items are “failing to provide medical care for a companion animal,” “failing to
provide domestic stock animals or companion animals with food or water,”
B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37           29

and “encouraging or causing animals to fight one another”; all represent situ-
ations in which suffering on the animal’s part, though not overtly specified, is
a likely outcome,. The fourth item (“using dogs or cats in research that results
in serious injury, illness, or death”), on the other hand, does specify that the
animal will suffer. It might be that individuals held mixed views on the use of
animals in research—concerned about the suffering of the animal but, at the
same time, making allowance for the fact that the suffering might be in some
way “justified” by the benefits that might result from the research.
   The results of the three-factor solution suggest that men differentiate
between acts of cruelty and acts of neglect. However, the men in this sample
also differentiated between behaviors that result in the death of an animal
and behaviors that result in injury to an animal. In order to examine how
the “death” and “injury” items grouped together relative to the neglect (or
“possible harm”) items, a second principle components factor analysis was
undertaken. In this analysis, items were forced onto only two factors. Results
are presented in Table 3. As seen in Table 3, all seven of the “death” and
“injury” items had loadings greater than .50 on Factor I, with much weaker
loadings on Factor II. In contrast, the three neglect items all had loadings
greater than .60 on Factor II. These results indicate that the “injury” items
were more strongly related to the “death” items than they were to the neglect
items.

                   Table 3. Two Factor Solution for Men
                                                                       I     II
  1 Intentionally killing a domestic stock animal (e.g., horse,       .81   .04
cow, pig, etc.) other than for food or to help the animal because
it was hurt, old, or sick?
  2 Intentionally killing a wild animal (e.g., deer, rabbit, squir-   .79   .05
rel, etc.) other than for food, while hunting, or to help the ani-
mal because it was hurt or sick?
  3 Intentionally killing a companion animal (e.g., pet dog, cat,     .66   .06
rabbit,etc.) other than to help the animal because it was hurt,
old or sick?
  7 Intentionally killing (i.e., euthanizing) a companion animal      .54   .24
or domestic stock animal because the owner is unable to care for
it (e.g., the person is moving out of state and cannot take the
animal to their new home.)?
30                 B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37

Table 3. (cont.)
                                                                        I      II
  8 Intentionally hurting a domestic stock animal (e.g., horse,        .66    .32
cow, pig, etc.) other than for training, branding, etc?
  9 Intentionally hurting a wild animal (e.g., deer, rabbit, squir-    .67    .29
rel, etc.)?
10 Intentionally hurting a companion animal (e.g., pet dog,            .60    .37
cat, rabbit, etc.) other than for training?
11 Having sexual contact with an animal?                               .33    .20
13 Using dogs or cats in research that results in serious injury,      .42    .51
illness, or death of the animal?
16 Failing to provide medical care for a companion animal that         .44    .61
is clearly injured or ill?
17 Failing to provide domestic stock animals or companion              .34    .69
animals with food or water for 24 hours?
18 Leaving domestic stock animals outside without shelter for          .05    .84
24 hours?
19 Leaving companion animals outside without shelter for               .04    .81
24 hours?
20 Leaving a companion animal in a locked car with the win-            .19    .62
dows cracked with an outside temperature of 70° for one hour?
23 Intentionally encouraging or causing animals to fight one            .50    .40
another (e.g., dog fighting, cock fighting, etc.)?

Finally, as a further check on the relation between the “death,” “injury,” and
“neglect” items, scales were constructed using factors loadings from the three-
factor solution. The four injury items that had loaded clearly on Factor I were
averaged into an injury scale; the three neglect items that had loaded clearly
on Factor II were averaged into a neglect scale; and the three items that had
loaded clearly on Factor III were averaged into a death scale. The bivariate
correlations between these scales are presented in Table 4. Consistent with the
two-factor solution described above, the injury scale was more strongly
correlated with the death scale (r = 0.49) than it was with the neglect scale
(r = 0.36). In addition, the death scale was only moderately correlated with the
neglect scale (r = 0.28.) Recall that the original correlation between the cruelty
and neglect subscales was r = 0.62.
B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37             31

          Table 4. Bivariate Correlations Among Subscales for Men
                                       Death                                 Injury
Injury                                  .49
Neglect                                 .28                                   .36

Factor Structure of Neglect and Cruelty Items among Women
The 15 cruelty and neglect items were entered into a principle components
factor analysis with varimax rotation. Factors with eigenvalues greater that
1.00 were selected. Three factors emerged, accounting for 56.4% of the vari-
ability in responses. Factor loadings for the items are presented in Table 5. In
Table 5, factor loadings of .30 or greater are in bold. The items loading on
each of the three factors are presented graphically in Figure 2.

                 Table 5. Three Factor Solution for Women
                                                                I      II      III
Intentionally killing a domestic stock animal (e.g.,          .87     −.01    −.01
horse, cow, pig, etc.) other than for food or to help
the animal because it was hurt, old, or sick?
Intentionally killing a wild animal (e.g., deer, rab-         .87     .13     −.03
bit, squirrel, etc.) other than for food, while hunt-
ing, or to help the animal because it was hurt or
sick?
Intentionally killing a companion animal (e.g., pet           .67     .07      .12
dog, cat, rabbit, etc.) other than to help the animal
because it was hurt, old or sick?
Intentionally killing (i.e., euthanizing) a compan-           .46     .12      .28
ion animal or domestic stock animal because the
owner is unable to care for it (e.g., the person is
moving out of state and cannot take the animal to
their new home.)?
Intentionally hurting a domestic stock animal (e.g.,          .64     .14      .37
horse, cow, pig, etc.) other than for training, brand-
ing, etc?
Intentionally hurting a wild animal (e.g., deer, rab-         .60     .22      .28
bit, squirrel, etc.)?
32                          B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37

Table 5. (cont.)
                                                                                         I            II          III
Intentionally hurting a companion animal (e.g., pet                                     .56           .23          .53
dog, cat, rabbit, etc.) other than for training?
Having sexual contact with an animal?                                                −.01           −.06           .78
Using dogs or cats in research that results in serious                                .37            .26           .44
injury, illness, or death of the animal?
Failing to provide medical care for a companion                                         .19           .53          .51
animal that is clearly injured or ill?
Failing to provide domestic stock animals or com-                                       .18           .64          .37
panion animals with food or water for 24 hours?
Leaving domestic stock animals outside without                                          .10           .83        −.04
shelter for 24 hours?
Leaving companion animals outside without shelter                                       .13           .83          .21
for 24 hours?
Leaving a companion animal in a locked car with                                         .05           .62        −.01
the windows cracked with an outside temperature
of 70° for one hour?
Intentionally encouraging or causing animals to                                         .38           .25          .51
fight one another (e.g., dog fighting, cock fighting,
etc.)?

Factor I
Killing stock
Killing wild animal
Killing companion
           animal
Euthanizing companion
           animal                                   Factor III
Hurting wild animal
Hurting stock
                                    Hurting companion
                                           animal
                                    Research
                                    Fighting
                                                                 Sexual contact   Factor II
Medical care
                                                                 Food and water
Leaving stock outside
                                                                                        Leaving companion animals outside
Leaving companion animal in locked car


                        Figure 2. Items loading on Factors I, II, and III (women)
B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37            33

As was the case with men, among women three items relating to neglect (“leav-
ing domestic stock animals outside without shelter,” “leaving companion
animals outside without shelter,” and “leaving a companion animal in a
locked car”) emerged with strong loadings on Factor II and weak loadings on
Factors I and III. However, inconsistent with the findings for men, women
appeared to show less differentiation when evaluating behaviors relating to the
death or injury of an animal. Of the seven items that loaded on either the
“death” or “injury” factor for men, five had strong loadings on Factor I for
women; the other two had the highest loadings on Factor I with secondary
loadings on Factor III. Thus, women were not differentiating between injury
and death to the same extent that men were.
   In order to further examine the relation between the cruelty and neglect
items among women, scales were constructed using factor loadings from
the three-factor solution. The seven death/injury items that had loaded on
Factors I or III were averaged into a “cruelty” scale; and the three neglect items
that had loaded clearly on Factor II were averaged into a neglect scale. The
bivariate correlations between these scales was r = 0.29, p < .01. Recall that the
original correlation between the cruelty and neglect subscales was r = 0.51.


Discussion
While a substantial body of research has been directed at understanding the
determinants of attitudes about animals in general and animal abuse in par-
ticular, little research has focused on individual differences in attitudes about
animal neglect. The current study suggests that attitudes about abuse and
neglect can be reliably differentiated among both men and women. However,
the structure of these attitudes appears to differ substantially by sex.
   Among men, the items that were originally viewed as being related to cru-
elty (those items relating to the intentional killing or injuring of animals)
divided into two factors. It appears that the primary determinant of this dis-
tinction is the amount of harm suffered by the animal: Items related to the
killing of animals loaded on one factor, and items related to the injury of ani-
mals loaded on a separate factor. Notice that the type of animal involved did
not appear to influence the structure of these factors. Items related to compan-
ion animals, domestic stock animals, and wild animals were represented on
both factors.
   Among men, the greatest area of overlap across factors was found on those
items that reflected likely, but not definitive, harm to animals. These items,
such as using animals in research or causing animals to fight, loaded both on
the “injury” and the neglect factors. In contrast, items that reflected possible
34                B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37

harm—such as leaving an animal in a locked car on a warm day—loaded only
on the neglect factor. Thus, men appeared to be differentiating between these
various behaviors on the basis of the perceived degree of harm suffered by the
animal.
   The factor structure for women differed somewhat from that for men.
Although women differentiated between behaviors that represented explicit
harm to animals from those that represented possible harm to animals, they
did not differentiate between death and injury. Further, those items that
bridged the gap between “injury” and neglect among men were more clearly
linked to the cruelty factor among women. Previous research (Herzog, 2007)
has indicated that women are more empathetic toward animals than are men.
The current research suggests that, not only do men and women differ with
regard to their absolute level of empathy, but also that men and women differ
with regard to the structure of their attitudes. Women appear to have a broader
scope of what constitutes cruelty than do men.
   The reason for this sex difference is not immediately apparent and warrants
further exploration. One possibility is that the greater differentiation on the
part of men reflects the relatively lower levels of empathy observed among
men compared to women. Lower levels of empathy may result in men judging
acts of violence differently, depending on the impact on the victim. In con-
trast, among women, any act of violence—regardless of the impact on the
victim—may be viewed as essentially the same. This interpretation is consis-
tent with the results of Pakaslahti and Keltikangas-Jarvinen (1997). These
authors reported that—among a sample of adolescents—when presented with
descriptors of aggressive behaviors, boys were more likely than girls to legiti-
mize the aggression by focusing on factors that might “excuse” the aggression.
In contrast, regardless of possible extenuating or justifying circumstances, girls
were more likely to view acts of aggression as wrong. Similarly, among chil-
dren, Perry, Perry, and Rasmussen (1986) demonstrated that girls perceived
aggression as being more harmful to victims than did boys and anticipated
greater punishment in response to aggression than did boys. Taken together,
these findings suggest that women are likely to have stronger and broader
moral strictures against aggression than do men.
   These structural differences might be important to the extent that these
attitudes are related to behavior. Thus, it may be that, depending upon the
extent of harm they believe an animal will suffer as a consequence, some men
may be willing to engage in certain types of behaviors. This hypothesis implies
that cruelty-and neglect-related attitudes are indeed predictors of overt behav-
ior. An extensive psychological literature has arisen around the question of
attitude-behavior consistency (Crano & Prislin, 2006). For example, research
B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37            35

has indicated that attitude-behavior consistency is greatest when those atti-
tudes are relatively strong (Cooke & Sheeran, 2004). Attitudinal characteris-
tics such as accessibility, how easily the attitude is brought to mind (Fazio &
Williams, 1986) and temporal stability (deCosta, Dibonaventura & Chap-
man, 2005) have been shown to predict attitude-behavior consistency. Fur-
ther, research has indicated that attitude-behavior consistency is strongest
when the attitudes are perceived to conform to group norms (Smith, Hogg,
Martin & Terry, 2007). Taken together, these findings suggest potentially
fruitful avenues for future research within the human-nonhuman animal
interaction field. Based on broader social-psychological research, it would be
expected that attitudes toward animals would predict behavior toward animals
when the attitudes in question were strongly held, of longer duration, and
perceived as generally supported within the peer group.
   A practical implication of this study is that researchers who are investigating
issues relating to animal cruelty need to consider the types of items included
in attitudinal surveys. Surveys that are heavily oriented toward items involving
the death of an animal may be answered differently by men from surveys ori-
ented toward items involving the injury of animals. Further, researchers inves-
tigating animal neglect must recognize that men appear to view behaviors
associated with probable harm to animals differently from how they view
behaviors associated with possible harm to animals. A researcher who wants to
clearly distinguish between cruelty attitudes and neglect attitudes would per-
haps want to focus on death-related behaviors to assess cruelty and on possi-
ble-harm items to assess neglect.
   It should be noted that although this study examined the structure of atti-
tudes, it did not address the issue of factors that influence the development of
those attitudes. Further research could assess correlates of individual differ-
ences in neglect attitudes. In general, attitudes about animals have been shown
to be related to factors such as history of pet ownership (Pagani et al., 2007);
demographic characteristics (Signal & Taylor, 2006); and attributions regard-
ing animal mind (Knight et al., 2004). However, the extent to which these
factors influence individual definitions of cruelty and neglect remains unclear.
Based on information derived from the current study, it appears that level of
harm (and likelihood of harm) is a significant determinant of the structure of
these attitudes.
   Other factors, such as the intentionality of the behavior, also deserve to
be addressed. Recall that Hills and Lalich (1998) found that a perpetrator’s
level of awareness of suffering influenced perceptions of that person: Those
who were described as being aware of the animal’s suffering were judged
more harshly than those who were described as being ignorant of the animal’s
36                  B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37

suffering. This factor may shed light on the findings of Sims et al. (2007). Sims
et al. presented participants with a scenario describing acts of maltreatment
against either a puppy or a chicken. In one set of scenarios, the maltreatment
was described as a presumably neglectful behavior (the animal was described
as being “thin and visibly underweight”); in another set of scenarios, the mal-
treatment was described as an overt act of cruelty (the animal was described as
being “beaten and severely injured”). However, no differences were found in
recommendations for punishment as a function of the nature of the maltreat-
ment. It is possible that neglect behaviors (failing to provide adequate food for
an animal) may be viewed differently, depending on the motivations of the
caregiver (an owner who is unable to provide adequate food because of eco-
nomic reasons versus an owner who intentionally withholds food from an
animal). The impact that the perceived intentionality of an act has on how
that act is construed is a topic that deserves further attention.
   It should be noted that this study is based on factor-analyses of a limited set
of items drawn from a single survey. The nature of factor analysis as a statistical
procedure is that the factors are determined by the items included. Future
research utilizing a broader range of cruelty and neglect items would be worth-
while for understanding the relations between these sets of attitudes.


Conclusion
In summary, the current study demonstrates that cruelty and neglect attitudes
can be distinguished among both men and women. However, the structure of
the cruelty attitudes differs as a function of sex, with men holding a more dif-
ferentiated view of the types of behaviors constituting cruelty than do women.
The relatively high correlations previously observed between cruelty and
neglect attitudes are likely due to those areas of overlap involving behaviors
that are associated with probable, but not explicit, harm to animals. When
those items are removed from the cruelty scales, the correlations between cru-
elty and neglect attitudes drop substantially. Future research should explore
the nature of the sex differences in attitude structure, as well as factors influ-
encing the scope of behaviors that are defined as cruelty versus neglect across
sexes.


References
Allen, M., Hunstone, M., Waerstad, J., Foy, E., Hobbins, T., Wikner, B., & Wirrel, J. (2002).
   Human-to-animal similarity and participant mood influence punishment recommendations
   for animal abusers. Society &Animals, 10, 267-284.
B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37                        37

Cooke, R., & Sheeran, P. (2004). Moderation of cognition-intention and cognition-behavior
   relations: A meta-analysis of properties of variables from the theory of planned behavior.
   British Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 159-186.
Crano, W., & Prislin, R. (2006). Attitudes and persuasion. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 345-
   374.
DeCosta Dibonaventura, M., & Chapman, G. (2005). Moderators of the intention-behavior
   relationship in influenza vaccinations: Intention stability and unforeseen barriers. Psychology
   & Health, 20, 761-774.
Fazio, R., & Williams, C. (1986). Attitude accessibility as a moderator of the attitude-perception
   and attitude-behavior relations: An investigation of the 1984 presidential election. Journal of
   Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 505-514.
Hills, A., & Lalich, N. (1998). Judgments of cruelty towards animals. Sex differences and effect
   of awareness of suffering. Anthrozoös, 11, 142-147.
Henry, B. (2004a). The relation between animal cruelty, delinquency, and attitudes toward the
   treatment of animals. Society & Animals, 12, 185-207.
—— (2004b). Exposure to animal abuse and group context: Two factors affecting participation
   in animal abuse. Anthrozoös, 17, 290-305.
—— (2006). Empathy, home environment, and attitudes toward animals in relation to animal
   abuse. Anthrozoös, 19 (1), 17-34.
Herzog, H. A. (2007). Gender differences in human-animal interactions: A review. Anthrozoös,
   20 (1), 7-21.
Knight, S., Vrij, A., Cherryman, J., & Nunkoosing, K. (2004). Attitudes towards animal use and
   belief in animal mind. Anthrozoös, 17 (1), 43-62.
Pagani, C., Robustelli, F., & Ascione, F. (2007). Italian youths’ attitudes toward, and concern
   for, animals. Anthrozoös, 20, 275-293.
Pakaslahti, L., & Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L. (1997). The relationship between moral approval of
   aggression, aggressive problem-solving strategies, and aggressive behavior in 14-year-old ado-
   lescents. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 12, 905-924.
Perry, D., Perry, L., & Rasmussen, P. (1986). Cognitive social learning mediators of aggression.
   Child Development, 57, 700-711.
Signal, T. D., & Taylor, N. (2006). Attitudes to animals: Demographics within a community
   sample. Society & Animals, 14 (2), 147-157.
Sims, V., Chin, M., & Yordon, R. (2007). Don’t be cruel: Assessing beliefs about punishments
   for crimes against animals. Anthrozoös, 20, 251-259.
Smith, J., Hogg, M., Martin, R., & Terry, D. (2007). Uncertainty and the influence of group
   norms in the attitude-behaviour relationship. British Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 769-
   792.

More Related Content

What's hot

literature review - besire paralik
literature review - besire paralikliterature review - besire paralik
literature review - besire paralikBesire Paralik
 
Homosexuality, 1800's-2015
Homosexuality, 1800's-2015Homosexuality, 1800's-2015
Homosexuality, 1800's-2015Jennifer Fiato
 
Correlates of criminal behavior among female prisoners 1
Correlates of criminal behavior among female prisoners 1Correlates of criminal behavior among female prisoners 1
Correlates of criminal behavior among female prisoners 1ashtinadkins
 
Significant Life Experiences publication
Significant Life Experiences publicationSignificant Life Experiences publication
Significant Life Experiences publicationDonovon Ceaser
 
Altruism and evolution
Altruism and evolutionAltruism and evolution
Altruism and evolutionPRITHISH NATH
 
Transphobia in Today's Society: Implicit Attitudes and Personal Beliefs
Transphobia in Today's Society: Implicit Attitudes and Personal BeliefsTransphobia in Today's Society: Implicit Attitudes and Personal Beliefs
Transphobia in Today's Society: Implicit Attitudes and Personal BeliefsStephanie Azzarello
 
Honors_Thesis_Poster_Presentation1
Honors_Thesis_Poster_Presentation1Honors_Thesis_Poster_Presentation1
Honors_Thesis_Poster_Presentation1Marie Elaine Ortega
 
Is altruism a myth
Is altruism a mythIs altruism a myth
Is altruism a mythGerd Naydock
 
Erin Faith Page Homophobia - Final
Erin Faith Page Homophobia - FinalErin Faith Page Homophobia - Final
Erin Faith Page Homophobia - FinalErin Faith Page
 
Graduate Research Poster_Extraverion_EWhite_ACavazos
Graduate Research Poster_Extraverion_EWhite_ACavazosGraduate Research Poster_Extraverion_EWhite_ACavazos
Graduate Research Poster_Extraverion_EWhite_ACavazosEarledreka White, LPA
 
Altruism theories
Altruism theoriesAltruism theories
Altruism theoriesabonica
 
Behavior in Social Settings - Inverted and Upright IATs & Interracial Interac...
Behavior in Social Settings - Inverted and Upright IATs & Interracial Interac...Behavior in Social Settings - Inverted and Upright IATs & Interracial Interac...
Behavior in Social Settings - Inverted and Upright IATs & Interracial Interac...James Lee
 

What's hot (16)

literature review - besire paralik
literature review - besire paralikliterature review - besire paralik
literature review - besire paralik
 
Homosexuality, 1800's-2015
Homosexuality, 1800's-2015Homosexuality, 1800's-2015
Homosexuality, 1800's-2015
 
Correlates of criminal behavior among female prisoners 1
Correlates of criminal behavior among female prisoners 1Correlates of criminal behavior among female prisoners 1
Correlates of criminal behavior among female prisoners 1
 
Significant Life Experiences publication
Significant Life Experiences publicationSignificant Life Experiences publication
Significant Life Experiences publication
 
Altruism and evolution
Altruism and evolutionAltruism and evolution
Altruism and evolution
 
BSL Research Report
BSL Research ReportBSL Research Report
BSL Research Report
 
Transphobia in Today's Society: Implicit Attitudes and Personal Beliefs
Transphobia in Today's Society: Implicit Attitudes and Personal BeliefsTransphobia in Today's Society: Implicit Attitudes and Personal Beliefs
Transphobia in Today's Society: Implicit Attitudes and Personal Beliefs
 
Estimated Deaths Attributable to Social Factors in the United States
Estimated Deaths Attributable to Social Factors in the United StatesEstimated Deaths Attributable to Social Factors in the United States
Estimated Deaths Attributable to Social Factors in the United States
 
Honors_Thesis_Poster_Presentation1
Honors_Thesis_Poster_Presentation1Honors_Thesis_Poster_Presentation1
Honors_Thesis_Poster_Presentation1
 
ALTRUISM
ALTRUISMALTRUISM
ALTRUISM
 
Amanda Tom Final ID Paper
Amanda Tom Final ID PaperAmanda Tom Final ID Paper
Amanda Tom Final ID Paper
 
Is altruism a myth
Is altruism a mythIs altruism a myth
Is altruism a myth
 
Erin Faith Page Homophobia - Final
Erin Faith Page Homophobia - FinalErin Faith Page Homophobia - Final
Erin Faith Page Homophobia - Final
 
Graduate Research Poster_Extraverion_EWhite_ACavazos
Graduate Research Poster_Extraverion_EWhite_ACavazosGraduate Research Poster_Extraverion_EWhite_ACavazos
Graduate Research Poster_Extraverion_EWhite_ACavazos
 
Altruism theories
Altruism theoriesAltruism theories
Altruism theories
 
Behavior in Social Settings - Inverted and Upright IATs & Interracial Interac...
Behavior in Social Settings - Inverted and Upright IATs & Interracial Interac...Behavior in Social Settings - Inverted and Upright IATs & Interracial Interac...
Behavior in Social Settings - Inverted and Upright IATs & Interracial Interac...
 

Similar to Can attitudes about animal neglect be diff erentiated

Name Panther ID Case Study 1 Media Globaliza.docx
Name Panther ID Case Study 1 Media Globaliza.docxName Panther ID Case Study 1 Media Globaliza.docx
Name Panther ID Case Study 1 Media Globaliza.docxroushhsiu
 
Attitudes toward animals
Attitudes toward animalsAttitudes toward animals
Attitudes toward animalsFábio Coltro
 
Wangui mid-SURE poster final
Wangui mid-SURE poster finalWangui mid-SURE poster final
Wangui mid-SURE poster finalWangui Hymes
 
Animal Cruelty
Animal CrueltyAnimal Cruelty
Animal CrueltyTony Lisko
 
Altruism Theories
Altruism TheoriesAltruism Theories
Altruism Theoriesabonica
 
Au Psy M7 A2 Kalla S.Doc.Docx
Au Psy M7 A2 Kalla S.Doc.DocxAu Psy M7 A2 Kalla S.Doc.Docx
Au Psy M7 A2 Kalla S.Doc.Docxsheilakalla
 
Animal Abuse And Youth Violence
Animal Abuse And Youth ViolenceAnimal Abuse And Youth Violence
Animal Abuse And Youth ViolenceLisa Graves
 
Applyingbesttreatmentsbyusingagregressionequationtotargetviolenceproneyouthar...
Applyingbesttreatmentsbyusingagregressionequationtotargetviolenceproneyouthar...Applyingbesttreatmentsbyusingagregressionequationtotargetviolenceproneyouthar...
Applyingbesttreatmentsbyusingagregressionequationtotargetviolenceproneyouthar...Robert Zagar
 
current trends in criminal justice system
current trends in criminal justice systemcurrent trends in criminal justice system
current trends in criminal justice systemPATRICK MAELO
 
Corporal Punishmentand Aggression Reseach
Corporal Punishmentand Aggression ReseachCorporal Punishmentand Aggression Reseach
Corporal Punishmentand Aggression Reseachashleyep1984
 
Animal Abuse In Childhood And Later Support For Interpersonal Violence In Fam...
Animal Abuse In Childhood And Later Support For Interpersonal Violence In Fam...Animal Abuse In Childhood And Later Support For Interpersonal Violence In Fam...
Animal Abuse In Childhood And Later Support For Interpersonal Violence In Fam...Darian Pruitt
 
Excerpt from the Research Article2 In the Midwest Longitudin.pdf
Excerpt from the Research Article2 In the Midwest Longitudin.pdfExcerpt from the Research Article2 In the Midwest Longitudin.pdf
Excerpt from the Research Article2 In the Midwest Longitudin.pdfAASTHASTYLETRADITION
 
PULLING BACK THE CURTAIN ON HERITABILITYSTUDIES BIOSOCIAL C.docx
PULLING BACK THE CURTAIN ON HERITABILITYSTUDIES BIOSOCIAL C.docxPULLING BACK THE CURTAIN ON HERITABILITYSTUDIES BIOSOCIAL C.docx
PULLING BACK THE CURTAIN ON HERITABILITYSTUDIES BIOSOCIAL C.docxamrit47
 
Final-Research-Report
Final-Research-ReportFinal-Research-Report
Final-Research-ReportLucas Liddell
 
Morality Relativism & the Concerns it RaisesI want to g.docx
Morality Relativism & the Concerns it RaisesI want to g.docxMorality Relativism & the Concerns it RaisesI want to g.docx
Morality Relativism & the Concerns it RaisesI want to g.docxroushhsiu
 
Shifting landscape of lgbt org research
Shifting landscape of lgbt org researchShifting landscape of lgbt org research
Shifting landscape of lgbt org researchArushi Verma
 
final thesis revision
final thesis revisionfinal thesis revision
final thesis revisionKat Wortham
 

Similar to Can attitudes about animal neglect be diff erentiated (20)

Name Panther ID Case Study 1 Media Globaliza.docx
Name Panther ID Case Study 1 Media Globaliza.docxName Panther ID Case Study 1 Media Globaliza.docx
Name Panther ID Case Study 1 Media Globaliza.docx
 
Attitudes toward animals
Attitudes toward animalsAttitudes toward animals
Attitudes toward animals
 
Wangui mid-SURE poster final
Wangui mid-SURE poster finalWangui mid-SURE poster final
Wangui mid-SURE poster final
 
Animal Cruelty
Animal CrueltyAnimal Cruelty
Animal Cruelty
 
Altruism Theories
Altruism TheoriesAltruism Theories
Altruism Theories
 
Au Psy M7 A2 Kalla S.Doc.Docx
Au Psy M7 A2 Kalla S.Doc.DocxAu Psy M7 A2 Kalla S.Doc.Docx
Au Psy M7 A2 Kalla S.Doc.Docx
 
Animal Abuse And Youth Violence
Animal Abuse And Youth ViolenceAnimal Abuse And Youth Violence
Animal Abuse And Youth Violence
 
Applyingbesttreatmentsbyusingagregressionequationtotargetviolenceproneyouthar...
Applyingbesttreatmentsbyusingagregressionequationtotargetviolenceproneyouthar...Applyingbesttreatmentsbyusingagregressionequationtotargetviolenceproneyouthar...
Applyingbesttreatmentsbyusingagregressionequationtotargetviolenceproneyouthar...
 
current trends in criminal justice system
current trends in criminal justice systemcurrent trends in criminal justice system
current trends in criminal justice system
 
Corporal Punishmentand Aggression Reseach
Corporal Punishmentand Aggression ReseachCorporal Punishmentand Aggression Reseach
Corporal Punishmentand Aggression Reseach
 
Spss
SpssSpss
Spss
 
Spss
SpssSpss
Spss
 
Spss
SpssSpss
Spss
 
Animal Abuse In Childhood And Later Support For Interpersonal Violence In Fam...
Animal Abuse In Childhood And Later Support For Interpersonal Violence In Fam...Animal Abuse In Childhood And Later Support For Interpersonal Violence In Fam...
Animal Abuse In Childhood And Later Support For Interpersonal Violence In Fam...
 
Excerpt from the Research Article2 In the Midwest Longitudin.pdf
Excerpt from the Research Article2 In the Midwest Longitudin.pdfExcerpt from the Research Article2 In the Midwest Longitudin.pdf
Excerpt from the Research Article2 In the Midwest Longitudin.pdf
 
PULLING BACK THE CURTAIN ON HERITABILITYSTUDIES BIOSOCIAL C.docx
PULLING BACK THE CURTAIN ON HERITABILITYSTUDIES BIOSOCIAL C.docxPULLING BACK THE CURTAIN ON HERITABILITYSTUDIES BIOSOCIAL C.docx
PULLING BACK THE CURTAIN ON HERITABILITYSTUDIES BIOSOCIAL C.docx
 
Final-Research-Report
Final-Research-ReportFinal-Research-Report
Final-Research-Report
 
Morality Relativism & the Concerns it RaisesI want to g.docx
Morality Relativism & the Concerns it RaisesI want to g.docxMorality Relativism & the Concerns it RaisesI want to g.docx
Morality Relativism & the Concerns it RaisesI want to g.docx
 
Shifting landscape of lgbt org research
Shifting landscape of lgbt org researchShifting landscape of lgbt org research
Shifting landscape of lgbt org research
 
final thesis revision
final thesis revisionfinal thesis revision
final thesis revision
 

More from Fábio Coltro

Monkey business social and cultural geography 2010
Monkey business   social and cultural geography 2010Monkey business   social and cultural geography 2010
Monkey business social and cultural geography 2010Fábio Coltro
 
Marxism and the moral status of animals ted benton
Marxism and the moral status of animals   ted bentonMarxism and the moral status of animals   ted benton
Marxism and the moral status of animals ted bentonFábio Coltro
 
Marketing farm animal welfare emma roe
Marketing farm animal welfare   emma roeMarketing farm animal welfare   emma roe
Marketing farm animal welfare emma roeFábio Coltro
 
Law library acquisitions list for march 2007
Law library acquisitions list for march 2007Law library acquisitions list for march 2007
Law library acquisitions list for march 2007Fábio Coltro
 
Kangaroos the non-issue
Kangaroos   the non-issueKangaroos   the non-issue
Kangaroos the non-issueFábio Coltro
 
Human discorses animal gepgraphies
Human discorses animal gepgraphiesHuman discorses animal gepgraphies
Human discorses animal gepgraphiesFábio Coltro
 
Geography, value paradigms and environmental justice lynn
Geography, value paradigms and environmental justice   lynnGeography, value paradigms and environmental justice   lynn
Geography, value paradigms and environmental justice lynnFábio Coltro
 
Geoethicsnews112 junebasic
Geoethicsnews112 junebasicGeoethicsnews112 junebasic
Geoethicsnews112 junebasicFábio Coltro
 
Frankenstein syndrome bernard rollin
Frankenstein syndrome   bernard rollinFrankenstein syndrome   bernard rollin
Frankenstein syndrome bernard rollinFábio Coltro
 
European approaches to ensure good animal welfare emma roe
European approaches to ensure good animal welfare   emma roeEuropean approaches to ensure good animal welfare   emma roe
European approaches to ensure good animal welfare emma roeFábio Coltro
 
Especismo e a percepção dos animais coltro
Especismo e a percepção dos animais   coltroEspecismo e a percepção dos animais   coltro
Especismo e a percepção dos animais coltroFábio Coltro
 
Dr. s. mary p. benbow cv
Dr. s. mary p. benbow   cvDr. s. mary p. benbow   cv
Dr. s. mary p. benbow cvFábio Coltro
 
Companhias multiespécies nas naturezaculturas uma conversa entre donna hara...
Companhias multiespécies nas naturezaculturas   uma conversa entre donna hara...Companhias multiespécies nas naturezaculturas   uma conversa entre donna hara...
Companhias multiespécies nas naturezaculturas uma conversa entre donna hara...Fábio Coltro
 
Associate editor's introduction bringing animals into social scientific resea...
Associate editor's introduction bringing animals into social scientific resea...Associate editor's introduction bringing animals into social scientific resea...
Associate editor's introduction bringing animals into social scientific resea...Fábio Coltro
 
Anthropomorphism to zoomorphism
Anthropomorphism to zoomorphismAnthropomorphism to zoomorphism
Anthropomorphism to zoomorphismFábio Coltro
 

More from Fábio Coltro (20)

Monkey business social and cultural geography 2010
Monkey business   social and cultural geography 2010Monkey business   social and cultural geography 2010
Monkey business social and cultural geography 2010
 
Marxism and the moral status of animals ted benton
Marxism and the moral status of animals   ted bentonMarxism and the moral status of animals   ted benton
Marxism and the moral status of animals ted benton
 
Marketing farm animal welfare emma roe
Marketing farm animal welfare   emma roeMarketing farm animal welfare   emma roe
Marketing farm animal welfare emma roe
 
Law library acquisitions list for march 2007
Law library acquisitions list for march 2007Law library acquisitions list for march 2007
Law library acquisitions list for march 2007
 
Kangaroos the non-issue
Kangaroos   the non-issueKangaroos   the non-issue
Kangaroos the non-issue
 
Human discorses animal gepgraphies
Human discorses animal gepgraphiesHuman discorses animal gepgraphies
Human discorses animal gepgraphies
 
Geography, value paradigms and environmental justice lynn
Geography, value paradigms and environmental justice   lynnGeography, value paradigms and environmental justice   lynn
Geography, value paradigms and environmental justice lynn
 
Geografia e ética
Geografia e éticaGeografia e ética
Geografia e ética
 
Geoethicsnews112 junebasic
Geoethicsnews112 junebasicGeoethicsnews112 junebasic
Geoethicsnews112 junebasic
 
Frankenstein syndrome bernard rollin
Frankenstein syndrome   bernard rollinFrankenstein syndrome   bernard rollin
Frankenstein syndrome bernard rollin
 
European approaches to ensure good animal welfare emma roe
European approaches to ensure good animal welfare   emma roeEuropean approaches to ensure good animal welfare   emma roe
European approaches to ensure good animal welfare emma roe
 
Ethical consumption
Ethical consumptionEthical consumption
Ethical consumption
 
Especismo e a percepção dos animais coltro
Especismo e a percepção dos animais   coltroEspecismo e a percepção dos animais   coltro
Especismo e a percepção dos animais coltro
 
Dr. s. mary p. benbow cv
Dr. s. mary p. benbow   cvDr. s. mary p. benbow   cv
Dr. s. mary p. benbow cv
 
Dog duty
Dog dutyDog duty
Dog duty
 
Companhias multiespécies nas naturezaculturas uma conversa entre donna hara...
Companhias multiespécies nas naturezaculturas   uma conversa entre donna hara...Companhias multiespécies nas naturezaculturas   uma conversa entre donna hara...
Companhias multiespécies nas naturezaculturas uma conversa entre donna hara...
 
Carr _cohen_2009
Carr  _cohen_2009Carr  _cohen_2009
Carr _cohen_2009
 
Associate editor's introduction bringing animals into social scientific resea...
Associate editor's introduction bringing animals into social scientific resea...Associate editor's introduction bringing animals into social scientific resea...
Associate editor's introduction bringing animals into social scientific resea...
 
Anthropomorphism to zoomorphism
Anthropomorphism to zoomorphismAnthropomorphism to zoomorphism
Anthropomorphism to zoomorphism
 
Animaurbis
AnimaurbisAnimaurbis
Animaurbis
 

Can attitudes about animal neglect be diff erentiated

  • 1. Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37 www.brill.nl/soan Can Attitudes About Animal Neglect Be Differentiated From Attitudes About Animal Abuse? Bill C. Henry Associate Professor, Department of Psychology, Metropolitan State College of Denver, PO Box 173362, Denver, CO 80217, USA henrybi@mscd.edu Sent 18 July 2008, Accepted 25 September 2008 Abstract The past decade has seen an increase in interest relating to the correlates and determinants of attitudes about nonhuman animals, especially attitudes about the use or abuse of animals. How- ever, little research has explicitly addressed individual differences in attitudes about the neglect of animals. The current study employs a factor-analytic approach to explore (a) whether attitudes about animal neglect can be reliably differentiated from attitudes about animal abuse and (b) whether the relationship between attitudes about animal neglect and animal abuse differs as a function of gender. Results indicated that attitudes about abuse and neglect can be reliably differentiated among both men and women. However, the structure of these attitudes appears to differ substantially by sex. This paper discusses theoretical and practical implications of these results. Keywords abuse, animals, attitudes, factor structure, neglect Introduction Recent decades have seen an increase in interest relating to the correlates and determinants of attitudes about nonhuman animals (Knight, Vrij, Cherry- man, & Nunkoosing, 2004; Pagani, Robustelli & Ascione, 2007; Signal & Taylor, 2006). Of particular interest within this area of study are factors relat- ing to attitudes about cruelty toward animals. A variety of studies have exam- ined how various factors influence people’s judgments about cruelty. For example, Allen et al. (2002) presented participants with a scenario describing an act of cruelty; within the scenario, the type of animal victimized was varied. In addition, the mood of the participant was manipulated. Results indicated that participants in a positive mood-state recommended harsher punishments © Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2009 DOI: 10.1163/156853009X393747
  • 2. 22 B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37 for the perpetrator of the abuse. Participants also recommended harsher pun- ishment when the animal-victim was perceived as being more similar to humans (primate as compared to reptile), Similarly, Hills and Lalich (1998) found that participants rated perpetrators of animal maltreatment as more cruel if the perpetrators were depicted as being aware—rather than unaware— of an animal’s suffering. Sims, Chin, and Yordon (2007) reported that women recommended harsher punishments for acts of animal abuse than did men and that recommended punishments were harsher when the victim was a puppy compared to when the victim was a chicken. While a significant body of research has examined attitudes and beliefs about cruelty, very little research has addressed attitudes about a related topic: the neglect of animals. One study that did address the difference in attitudes about cruelty versus neglect was Sims et al. (2007). As described above, Sims et al. presented participants with a scenario describing acts of maltreatment against either a puppy or a chicken. In addition, the nature of the maltreat- ment was varied. One set of scenarios described maltreatment as a presumably neglectful behavior (the animal was described as being “thin and visibly under- weight”); another set of scenarios described the maltreatment as an overt act of cruelty (the animal was described as being “beaten and severely injured”). However, no differences were found in recommendations for punishment as a function of the nature of the maltreatment. It appeared that in the Sims et al. study, participants were not differentiating between starving an animal and beating an animal. In contrast, Henry (2006) found that participants did in fact differentiate between acts of cruelty and acts that could be interpreted as neglectful. Henry conducted a factor analysis of the Attitudes Toward the Treatment of Animals Scale (ATTAS), a 23-item scale assessing participants’ responses to a variety of types of treatments of animals. The factor analysis revealed three factors emerg- ing from the 23 items of the scale. One factor was labeled “Utilitarian” and reflected differences in peoples’ attitudes about the utilitarian use of animals (using animals for food and clothing). A second factor was labeled “Cruelty” and reflected differences in peoples’ attitudes about the intentional malicious abuse of animals (intentionally killing or injuring animals). A third factor was labeled “Caregiving” and reflected differences in peoples’ attitudes about the appropriate care of animals (providing food, water, medical care). Although Henry (2006) labeled this last factor “Caregiving,” an inspection of the items loading on that factor indicates that a better label might be “Neglect.” In general, the items assessed attitudes about failures to provide minimal levels of care to animals. Thus, within the sample examined by Henry, it appeared that a distinction was being drawn between overt acts of cruelty and behaviors that were considered neglectful.
  • 3. B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37 23 It should be noted that these two factors (cruelty and neglect), while dis- tinct, were significantly correlated. Henry (2006) reported that the correlation between the Cruelty and Caregiving/Neglect factor was 0.60. In addition, it should be noted that Henry did not conduct the factor analysis of the ATTAS separately by sex. Thus, potential differences in the factor-structure of atti- tudes among men and women were obscured. The studies reviewed thus far raise a number of intriguing questions. First, do cruelty and neglect of animals really constitute distinct factors? And if so, why? What are the bases upon which people would draw a distinction between acts of cruelty and acts of neglect? Second, given the substantial body of research indicating that women are more empathetic toward animals than are men (Herzog, 2007), do women and men construe the relation between acts of cruelty and acts of neglect differently? The current study sought to use a factor-analytic approach to explore the relation between attitudes about ani- mal cruelty and animal neglect separately by sex. Methods Participants Participants in this study were 683 students enrolled in sections of Introduc- tion to Psychology. Research participation was a requirement of the course. Of the participants, 333 (48.8%) were men; 350 (51.2%) were women. The mean age of participants was 22.64 years (SD = 6.25) with a range of 17 to 57 years. Seventy % of participants identified themselves as White; 13%, His- panic; 15%, Black; 10%, Asian; 4%, American Indian/Alaska Native; 2%, Pacific Islander; and 4%, Other. These percentages sum to more than 100% because participants could identify themselves as members of more than one race/ethnicity group. Materials Attitudes toward the treatment of animals survey (ATTAS). Participants com- pleted the ATTAS as part of a broader study examining human-animal inter- actions. The ATTAS is a 23-item attitude scale used to assess attitudes regarding the treatment of animals (Henry, 2004a, 2004b, 2006). Participants were asked to indicate the extent to which they would be bothered by thinking about a particular type of treatment of an animal. Each item was phrased, “How much would it bother you to think about. . . .” Items assessed a variety of types of treatment of animals—such as (a) failing to provide adequate food, shelter, or medical care; (b) using animals in medical research; (c) encouraging
  • 4. 24 B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37 animals to fight; and (d) killing or hurting an animal for no apparent reason. Items assessed attitudes pertaining to the following: 1. companion animals (pet dogs, cats, and rabbits); 2. domestic stock animals (horses, cows, and pigs); and 3. animals in the wild (deer, rabbit, and squirrel). Participants responded to each item on a 5-point scale ranging from 1-5: “None at all” to “A lot.” Thus, higher scores reflected relatively more discom- fort with the type of treatment specified. As described above, a previous factor analysis of the ATTAS (Henry, 2006) revealed that the items on the survey divided into three subscales: 1. Cruelty (eight items); 2. Utilitarian (eight items); and 3. Caregiving/Neglect (seven items). Because the purpose of this study was to explore the relation between attitudes about cruelty and attitudes about neglect, only the 15 items relating to cruelty and neglect were analyzed here. Those 15 items are listed in Table 1. The eight items on the Cruelty subscale assessed individual differences in attitudes regarding the intentional harming of animals for no apparent reason. High scores on these items reflected discomfort with such acts of pointless harm to animals. The seven items on the Caregiving/Neglect subscale assessed indi- vidual differences in attitudes regarding a person’s responsibilities for ensuring the safety and well being of an animal. High scores on these items reflected discomfort with the failure to meet the basic needs of an animal. On the Cruelty items, women (Mean = 4.48, SD = 0.60) scored higher than did men (Mean = 3.93, SD = 0.77), t (681) = -10.32, p < .01.) Likewise, on the Caregiving/Neglect items, women (Mean = 4.42, SD = 0.53) scored higher than did men (Mean = 3.83, SD = 0.73), t (681) = -12.27, p < .01.) Age was not correlated with scores on either the Cruelty items (r = 0.07, ns) or the Caregiving/neglect items (r = 0.03, ns) Results Bivariate Correlations As mentioned in the Introduction, one rationale for undertaking these analy- ses was that previous research had revealed strong correlations between scores on the Cruelty and Caregiving/Neglect subscales of the ATTAS. The same was
  • 5. B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37 25 Table 1. Items Loading on the Cruelty and Neglect Subscales of the ATTAS (Henry, 2006) Cruelty 1 How much would it bother you to think about someone intentionally killing a domestic stock animal (e.g., horse, cow, pig, etc.) other than for food or to help the animal because it was hurt, old, or sick? 2 How much would it bother you to think about someone intentionally killing a wild animal (e.g., deer, rabbit, squirrel, etc.) other than for food, while hunting, or to help the animal because it was hurt or sick? 3 How much would it bother you to think about someone intentionally killing a companion animal (e.g., pet dog, cat, rabbit, etc.) other than to help the animal because it was hurt, old or sick? 8 How much would it bother you to think about someone intentionally hurting a domestic stock animal (e.g., horse, cow, pig, etc.) other than for training, branding, etc? 9 How much would it bother you to think about someone intentionally hurting a wild animal (e.g., deer, rabbit, squirrel, etc.)? 10 How much would it bother you to think about someone intentionally hurting a companion animal (e.g., pet dog, cat, rabbit, etc.) other than for training? 13 How much would it bother you to think about someone using dogs or cats in research that results in serious injury, illness, or death of the animal? 23 How much would it bother you to think about someone intentionally encouraging or causing animals to fight one another (e.g., dog fighting, cock fighting, etc.)? Caregiving/Neglect 7 How much would it bother you to think about someone intentionally killing (i.e., euthanizing) a companion animal or domestic stock animal because the owner is unable to care for it (e.g., the person is moving out of state and cannot take the animal to their new home.)? 11 How much would it bother you to think about someone having sexual contact with an animal?
  • 6. 26 B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37 Table 1. (cont.) 16 How much would it bother you to think about someone failing to provide medical care for a companion animal that is clearly injured or ill? 17 How much would it bother you to think about someone failing to pro- vide domestic stock animals or companion animals with food or water for 24 hours? 18 How much would it bother you to think about someone leaving domestic stock animals outside without shelter for 24 hours? 19 How much would it bother you to think about someone leaving compan- ion animals outside without shelter for 24 hours? 20 How much would it bother you to think about someone leaving a com- panion animal in a locked car with the windows cracked with an outside temperature of 70° for one hour? true of scores within this sample. Among men, the correlation between the two subscales was r = 0.62 (p < .01); among women, the correlation was r = 0.51 (p < .01). To further examine the reasons for the substantial overlap between the Cruelty and Caregiving/Neglect items, principle components fac- tor-analyses of the 15 items (separately by sex) were conducted. Factor Structure of Neglect and Cruelty Items Among Men The 15 cruelty and neglect items were entered into a principle components factor analysis with varimax rotation. Factors with eigenvalues greater that 1.00 were selected. Three factors emerged, accounting for 58.8% of the vari- ability in responses. Factor loadings for the items are presented in Table 2. In Table 2, factor loadings of .30 or greater are in bold. The items loading on each of the three factors are presented graphically in Figure 1. A number of things are worth noting in this analysis. First, consistent with the findings of Henry (2006), items relating to neglect tended to load on a separate factor more than did items relating to cruelty. Three neglect items in particular (“leaving domestic stock animals outside without shelter,” “leaving companion animals outside without shelter,” and “leaving a companion ani- mal in a locked car”) had very strong loadings on Factor II and weak loadings on Factors I and III. Likewise, three cruelty items in particular (“intentionally killing a domestic stock animal,” “intentionally killing a wild animal,” and “intentionally killing
  • 7. B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37 27 Table 2. Three Factor Solution for Men I II III 1 Intentionally killing a domestic stock animal (e.g., .28 .09 .85 horse, cow, pig, etc.) other than for food or to help the animal because it was hurt, old, or sick? 2 Intentionally killing a wild animal (e.g., deer, rabbit, .27 .10 .84 squirrel, etc.) other than for food, while hunting, or to help the animal because it was hurt or sick? 3 Intentionally killing a companion animal (e.g., pet .12 .15 .80 dog, cat, rabbit,etc.) other than to help the animal because it was hurt, old or sick? 7 Intentionally killing (i.e., euthanizing) a companion .64 .08 .16 animal or domestic stock animal because the owner is unable to care for it (e.g., the person is moving out of state and cannot take the animal to their new home.)? 8 Intentionally hurting a domestic stock animal (e.g., .76 .14 .23 horse, cow, pig, etc.) other than for training, branding, etc? 9 Intentionally hurting a wild animal (e.g., deer, rab- .77 .09 .22 bit, squirrel, etc.)? 10 Intentionally hurting a companion animal (e.g., pet .77 .17 .14 dog, cat, rabbit, etc.) other than for training? 11 Having sexual contact with an animal? .25 .17 .25 13 Using dogs or cats in research that results in serious .52 .40 .15 injury, illness, or death of the animal? 16 Failing to provide medical care for a companion ani- .59 .48 .13 mal that is clearly injured or ill? 17 Failing to provide domestic stock animals or com- .44 .61 .15 panion animals with food or water for 24 hours? 18 Leaving domestic stock animals outside without .07 .89 .13 shelter for 24 hours? 19 Leaving companion animals outside without shelter .08 .86 .09 for 24 hours? 20 Leaving a companion animal in a locked car with .27 .59 .09 the windows cracked with an outside temperature of 70° for one hour? 23 Intentionally encouraging or causing animals to fight .52 .30 .25 one another (e.g., dog fighting, cock fighting, etc.)?
  • 8. 28 B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37 Factor III Killing stock Killing wild animal Killing companion animal Factor I Euthanizing companion animal Hurting stock Hurting wild animal Hurting companion animal Factor II Research Medical care Food and water Fighting Leaving stock outside Leaving companion animals outside Leaving companion animal in locked car Figure 1. Items loading on Factors I, II, and III (men) a companion animal”) had very strong loadings on Factor III and weak load- ings on Factors I and II. Finally, four items (“euthanizing a companion animal or stock animal,” “intentionally hurting a domestic stock animal,” “intention- ally hurting a wild animal,” and “intentionally hurting a companion animal”) had strong loadings on Factor I and weak loadings on Factors II and III. Thus, in this analysis, men appeared to differentiate between different types of treatments of animals on the basis of the severity of harm suffered by the animals. Factor III appears to capture those items in which the animal suffers death as a result of an intentional, malicious act. With the exception of the item relating to euthanizing an animal, Factor I appears to capture those items in which the animal is hurt but not killed. It is possible that men were likely to judge euthanasia less harshly because, even though it involves the death of the animal, it lacks the malicious nature of the items loading on Factor III. Factor II appears to capture those items in which there is the possibility of harm to the animal due to the neglect of the animal but in which the harm is not immediate. It is worth noting the four items that loaded both on the “harm” factor (Factor I) and the neglect (or “possible harm”) factor (Factor II). Three of these items are “failing to provide medical care for a companion animal,” “failing to provide domestic stock animals or companion animals with food or water,”
  • 9. B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37 29 and “encouraging or causing animals to fight one another”; all represent situ- ations in which suffering on the animal’s part, though not overtly specified, is a likely outcome,. The fourth item (“using dogs or cats in research that results in serious injury, illness, or death”), on the other hand, does specify that the animal will suffer. It might be that individuals held mixed views on the use of animals in research—concerned about the suffering of the animal but, at the same time, making allowance for the fact that the suffering might be in some way “justified” by the benefits that might result from the research. The results of the three-factor solution suggest that men differentiate between acts of cruelty and acts of neglect. However, the men in this sample also differentiated between behaviors that result in the death of an animal and behaviors that result in injury to an animal. In order to examine how the “death” and “injury” items grouped together relative to the neglect (or “possible harm”) items, a second principle components factor analysis was undertaken. In this analysis, items were forced onto only two factors. Results are presented in Table 3. As seen in Table 3, all seven of the “death” and “injury” items had loadings greater than .50 on Factor I, with much weaker loadings on Factor II. In contrast, the three neglect items all had loadings greater than .60 on Factor II. These results indicate that the “injury” items were more strongly related to the “death” items than they were to the neglect items. Table 3. Two Factor Solution for Men I II 1 Intentionally killing a domestic stock animal (e.g., horse, .81 .04 cow, pig, etc.) other than for food or to help the animal because it was hurt, old, or sick? 2 Intentionally killing a wild animal (e.g., deer, rabbit, squir- .79 .05 rel, etc.) other than for food, while hunting, or to help the ani- mal because it was hurt or sick? 3 Intentionally killing a companion animal (e.g., pet dog, cat, .66 .06 rabbit,etc.) other than to help the animal because it was hurt, old or sick? 7 Intentionally killing (i.e., euthanizing) a companion animal .54 .24 or domestic stock animal because the owner is unable to care for it (e.g., the person is moving out of state and cannot take the animal to their new home.)?
  • 10. 30 B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37 Table 3. (cont.) I II 8 Intentionally hurting a domestic stock animal (e.g., horse, .66 .32 cow, pig, etc.) other than for training, branding, etc? 9 Intentionally hurting a wild animal (e.g., deer, rabbit, squir- .67 .29 rel, etc.)? 10 Intentionally hurting a companion animal (e.g., pet dog, .60 .37 cat, rabbit, etc.) other than for training? 11 Having sexual contact with an animal? .33 .20 13 Using dogs or cats in research that results in serious injury, .42 .51 illness, or death of the animal? 16 Failing to provide medical care for a companion animal that .44 .61 is clearly injured or ill? 17 Failing to provide domestic stock animals or companion .34 .69 animals with food or water for 24 hours? 18 Leaving domestic stock animals outside without shelter for .05 .84 24 hours? 19 Leaving companion animals outside without shelter for .04 .81 24 hours? 20 Leaving a companion animal in a locked car with the win- .19 .62 dows cracked with an outside temperature of 70° for one hour? 23 Intentionally encouraging or causing animals to fight one .50 .40 another (e.g., dog fighting, cock fighting, etc.)? Finally, as a further check on the relation between the “death,” “injury,” and “neglect” items, scales were constructed using factors loadings from the three- factor solution. The four injury items that had loaded clearly on Factor I were averaged into an injury scale; the three neglect items that had loaded clearly on Factor II were averaged into a neglect scale; and the three items that had loaded clearly on Factor III were averaged into a death scale. The bivariate correlations between these scales are presented in Table 4. Consistent with the two-factor solution described above, the injury scale was more strongly correlated with the death scale (r = 0.49) than it was with the neglect scale (r = 0.36). In addition, the death scale was only moderately correlated with the neglect scale (r = 0.28.) Recall that the original correlation between the cruelty and neglect subscales was r = 0.62.
  • 11. B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37 31 Table 4. Bivariate Correlations Among Subscales for Men Death Injury Injury .49 Neglect .28 .36 Factor Structure of Neglect and Cruelty Items among Women The 15 cruelty and neglect items were entered into a principle components factor analysis with varimax rotation. Factors with eigenvalues greater that 1.00 were selected. Three factors emerged, accounting for 56.4% of the vari- ability in responses. Factor loadings for the items are presented in Table 5. In Table 5, factor loadings of .30 or greater are in bold. The items loading on each of the three factors are presented graphically in Figure 2. Table 5. Three Factor Solution for Women I II III Intentionally killing a domestic stock animal (e.g., .87 −.01 −.01 horse, cow, pig, etc.) other than for food or to help the animal because it was hurt, old, or sick? Intentionally killing a wild animal (e.g., deer, rab- .87 .13 −.03 bit, squirrel, etc.) other than for food, while hunt- ing, or to help the animal because it was hurt or sick? Intentionally killing a companion animal (e.g., pet .67 .07 .12 dog, cat, rabbit, etc.) other than to help the animal because it was hurt, old or sick? Intentionally killing (i.e., euthanizing) a compan- .46 .12 .28 ion animal or domestic stock animal because the owner is unable to care for it (e.g., the person is moving out of state and cannot take the animal to their new home.)? Intentionally hurting a domestic stock animal (e.g., .64 .14 .37 horse, cow, pig, etc.) other than for training, brand- ing, etc? Intentionally hurting a wild animal (e.g., deer, rab- .60 .22 .28 bit, squirrel, etc.)?
  • 12. 32 B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37 Table 5. (cont.) I II III Intentionally hurting a companion animal (e.g., pet .56 .23 .53 dog, cat, rabbit, etc.) other than for training? Having sexual contact with an animal? −.01 −.06 .78 Using dogs or cats in research that results in serious .37 .26 .44 injury, illness, or death of the animal? Failing to provide medical care for a companion .19 .53 .51 animal that is clearly injured or ill? Failing to provide domestic stock animals or com- .18 .64 .37 panion animals with food or water for 24 hours? Leaving domestic stock animals outside without .10 .83 −.04 shelter for 24 hours? Leaving companion animals outside without shelter .13 .83 .21 for 24 hours? Leaving a companion animal in a locked car with .05 .62 −.01 the windows cracked with an outside temperature of 70° for one hour? Intentionally encouraging or causing animals to .38 .25 .51 fight one another (e.g., dog fighting, cock fighting, etc.)? Factor I Killing stock Killing wild animal Killing companion animal Euthanizing companion animal Factor III Hurting wild animal Hurting stock Hurting companion animal Research Fighting Sexual contact Factor II Medical care Food and water Leaving stock outside Leaving companion animals outside Leaving companion animal in locked car Figure 2. Items loading on Factors I, II, and III (women)
  • 13. B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37 33 As was the case with men, among women three items relating to neglect (“leav- ing domestic stock animals outside without shelter,” “leaving companion animals outside without shelter,” and “leaving a companion animal in a locked car”) emerged with strong loadings on Factor II and weak loadings on Factors I and III. However, inconsistent with the findings for men, women appeared to show less differentiation when evaluating behaviors relating to the death or injury of an animal. Of the seven items that loaded on either the “death” or “injury” factor for men, five had strong loadings on Factor I for women; the other two had the highest loadings on Factor I with secondary loadings on Factor III. Thus, women were not differentiating between injury and death to the same extent that men were. In order to further examine the relation between the cruelty and neglect items among women, scales were constructed using factor loadings from the three-factor solution. The seven death/injury items that had loaded on Factors I or III were averaged into a “cruelty” scale; and the three neglect items that had loaded clearly on Factor II were averaged into a neglect scale. The bivariate correlations between these scales was r = 0.29, p < .01. Recall that the original correlation between the cruelty and neglect subscales was r = 0.51. Discussion While a substantial body of research has been directed at understanding the determinants of attitudes about animals in general and animal abuse in par- ticular, little research has focused on individual differences in attitudes about animal neglect. The current study suggests that attitudes about abuse and neglect can be reliably differentiated among both men and women. However, the structure of these attitudes appears to differ substantially by sex. Among men, the items that were originally viewed as being related to cru- elty (those items relating to the intentional killing or injuring of animals) divided into two factors. It appears that the primary determinant of this dis- tinction is the amount of harm suffered by the animal: Items related to the killing of animals loaded on one factor, and items related to the injury of ani- mals loaded on a separate factor. Notice that the type of animal involved did not appear to influence the structure of these factors. Items related to compan- ion animals, domestic stock animals, and wild animals were represented on both factors. Among men, the greatest area of overlap across factors was found on those items that reflected likely, but not definitive, harm to animals. These items, such as using animals in research or causing animals to fight, loaded both on the “injury” and the neglect factors. In contrast, items that reflected possible
  • 14. 34 B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37 harm—such as leaving an animal in a locked car on a warm day—loaded only on the neglect factor. Thus, men appeared to be differentiating between these various behaviors on the basis of the perceived degree of harm suffered by the animal. The factor structure for women differed somewhat from that for men. Although women differentiated between behaviors that represented explicit harm to animals from those that represented possible harm to animals, they did not differentiate between death and injury. Further, those items that bridged the gap between “injury” and neglect among men were more clearly linked to the cruelty factor among women. Previous research (Herzog, 2007) has indicated that women are more empathetic toward animals than are men. The current research suggests that, not only do men and women differ with regard to their absolute level of empathy, but also that men and women differ with regard to the structure of their attitudes. Women appear to have a broader scope of what constitutes cruelty than do men. The reason for this sex difference is not immediately apparent and warrants further exploration. One possibility is that the greater differentiation on the part of men reflects the relatively lower levels of empathy observed among men compared to women. Lower levels of empathy may result in men judging acts of violence differently, depending on the impact on the victim. In con- trast, among women, any act of violence—regardless of the impact on the victim—may be viewed as essentially the same. This interpretation is consis- tent with the results of Pakaslahti and Keltikangas-Jarvinen (1997). These authors reported that—among a sample of adolescents—when presented with descriptors of aggressive behaviors, boys were more likely than girls to legiti- mize the aggression by focusing on factors that might “excuse” the aggression. In contrast, regardless of possible extenuating or justifying circumstances, girls were more likely to view acts of aggression as wrong. Similarly, among chil- dren, Perry, Perry, and Rasmussen (1986) demonstrated that girls perceived aggression as being more harmful to victims than did boys and anticipated greater punishment in response to aggression than did boys. Taken together, these findings suggest that women are likely to have stronger and broader moral strictures against aggression than do men. These structural differences might be important to the extent that these attitudes are related to behavior. Thus, it may be that, depending upon the extent of harm they believe an animal will suffer as a consequence, some men may be willing to engage in certain types of behaviors. This hypothesis implies that cruelty-and neglect-related attitudes are indeed predictors of overt behav- ior. An extensive psychological literature has arisen around the question of attitude-behavior consistency (Crano & Prislin, 2006). For example, research
  • 15. B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37 35 has indicated that attitude-behavior consistency is greatest when those atti- tudes are relatively strong (Cooke & Sheeran, 2004). Attitudinal characteris- tics such as accessibility, how easily the attitude is brought to mind (Fazio & Williams, 1986) and temporal stability (deCosta, Dibonaventura & Chap- man, 2005) have been shown to predict attitude-behavior consistency. Fur- ther, research has indicated that attitude-behavior consistency is strongest when the attitudes are perceived to conform to group norms (Smith, Hogg, Martin & Terry, 2007). Taken together, these findings suggest potentially fruitful avenues for future research within the human-nonhuman animal interaction field. Based on broader social-psychological research, it would be expected that attitudes toward animals would predict behavior toward animals when the attitudes in question were strongly held, of longer duration, and perceived as generally supported within the peer group. A practical implication of this study is that researchers who are investigating issues relating to animal cruelty need to consider the types of items included in attitudinal surveys. Surveys that are heavily oriented toward items involving the death of an animal may be answered differently by men from surveys ori- ented toward items involving the injury of animals. Further, researchers inves- tigating animal neglect must recognize that men appear to view behaviors associated with probable harm to animals differently from how they view behaviors associated with possible harm to animals. A researcher who wants to clearly distinguish between cruelty attitudes and neglect attitudes would per- haps want to focus on death-related behaviors to assess cruelty and on possi- ble-harm items to assess neglect. It should be noted that although this study examined the structure of atti- tudes, it did not address the issue of factors that influence the development of those attitudes. Further research could assess correlates of individual differ- ences in neglect attitudes. In general, attitudes about animals have been shown to be related to factors such as history of pet ownership (Pagani et al., 2007); demographic characteristics (Signal & Taylor, 2006); and attributions regard- ing animal mind (Knight et al., 2004). However, the extent to which these factors influence individual definitions of cruelty and neglect remains unclear. Based on information derived from the current study, it appears that level of harm (and likelihood of harm) is a significant determinant of the structure of these attitudes. Other factors, such as the intentionality of the behavior, also deserve to be addressed. Recall that Hills and Lalich (1998) found that a perpetrator’s level of awareness of suffering influenced perceptions of that person: Those who were described as being aware of the animal’s suffering were judged more harshly than those who were described as being ignorant of the animal’s
  • 16. 36 B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37 suffering. This factor may shed light on the findings of Sims et al. (2007). Sims et al. presented participants with a scenario describing acts of maltreatment against either a puppy or a chicken. In one set of scenarios, the maltreatment was described as a presumably neglectful behavior (the animal was described as being “thin and visibly underweight”); in another set of scenarios, the mal- treatment was described as an overt act of cruelty (the animal was described as being “beaten and severely injured”). However, no differences were found in recommendations for punishment as a function of the nature of the maltreat- ment. It is possible that neglect behaviors (failing to provide adequate food for an animal) may be viewed differently, depending on the motivations of the caregiver (an owner who is unable to provide adequate food because of eco- nomic reasons versus an owner who intentionally withholds food from an animal). The impact that the perceived intentionality of an act has on how that act is construed is a topic that deserves further attention. It should be noted that this study is based on factor-analyses of a limited set of items drawn from a single survey. The nature of factor analysis as a statistical procedure is that the factors are determined by the items included. Future research utilizing a broader range of cruelty and neglect items would be worth- while for understanding the relations between these sets of attitudes. Conclusion In summary, the current study demonstrates that cruelty and neglect attitudes can be distinguished among both men and women. However, the structure of the cruelty attitudes differs as a function of sex, with men holding a more dif- ferentiated view of the types of behaviors constituting cruelty than do women. The relatively high correlations previously observed between cruelty and neglect attitudes are likely due to those areas of overlap involving behaviors that are associated with probable, but not explicit, harm to animals. When those items are removed from the cruelty scales, the correlations between cru- elty and neglect attitudes drop substantially. Future research should explore the nature of the sex differences in attitude structure, as well as factors influ- encing the scope of behaviors that are defined as cruelty versus neglect across sexes. References Allen, M., Hunstone, M., Waerstad, J., Foy, E., Hobbins, T., Wikner, B., & Wirrel, J. (2002). Human-to-animal similarity and participant mood influence punishment recommendations for animal abusers. Society &Animals, 10, 267-284.
  • 17. B. C. Henry / Society and Animals 17 (2009) 21-37 37 Cooke, R., & Sheeran, P. (2004). Moderation of cognition-intention and cognition-behavior relations: A meta-analysis of properties of variables from the theory of planned behavior. British Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 159-186. Crano, W., & Prislin, R. (2006). Attitudes and persuasion. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 345- 374. DeCosta Dibonaventura, M., & Chapman, G. (2005). Moderators of the intention-behavior relationship in influenza vaccinations: Intention stability and unforeseen barriers. Psychology & Health, 20, 761-774. Fazio, R., & Williams, C. (1986). Attitude accessibility as a moderator of the attitude-perception and attitude-behavior relations: An investigation of the 1984 presidential election. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 505-514. Hills, A., & Lalich, N. (1998). Judgments of cruelty towards animals. Sex differences and effect of awareness of suffering. Anthrozoös, 11, 142-147. Henry, B. (2004a). The relation between animal cruelty, delinquency, and attitudes toward the treatment of animals. Society & Animals, 12, 185-207. —— (2004b). Exposure to animal abuse and group context: Two factors affecting participation in animal abuse. Anthrozoös, 17, 290-305. —— (2006). Empathy, home environment, and attitudes toward animals in relation to animal abuse. Anthrozoös, 19 (1), 17-34. Herzog, H. A. (2007). Gender differences in human-animal interactions: A review. Anthrozoös, 20 (1), 7-21. Knight, S., Vrij, A., Cherryman, J., & Nunkoosing, K. (2004). Attitudes towards animal use and belief in animal mind. Anthrozoös, 17 (1), 43-62. Pagani, C., Robustelli, F., & Ascione, F. (2007). Italian youths’ attitudes toward, and concern for, animals. Anthrozoös, 20, 275-293. Pakaslahti, L., & Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L. (1997). The relationship between moral approval of aggression, aggressive problem-solving strategies, and aggressive behavior in 14-year-old ado- lescents. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 12, 905-924. Perry, D., Perry, L., & Rasmussen, P. (1986). Cognitive social learning mediators of aggression. Child Development, 57, 700-711. Signal, T. D., & Taylor, N. (2006). Attitudes to animals: Demographics within a community sample. Society & Animals, 14 (2), 147-157. Sims, V., Chin, M., & Yordon, R. (2007). Don’t be cruel: Assessing beliefs about punishments for crimes against animals. Anthrozoös, 20, 251-259. Smith, J., Hogg, M., Martin, R., & Terry, D. (2007). Uncertainty and the influence of group norms in the attitude-behaviour relationship. British Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 769- 792.