SlideShare una empresa de Scribd logo
1 de 94
REQUIREMENTS
As outlined in the Syllabus on Blackboard:
Article Review (80 points). Article review papers must include
a brief summary of
the study, explanation of the results, and your opinion of the
conclusions. In addition,
the paper should include relevant course material and a critique
of the article’s
statistics when discussing the results and conclusions. It should
be between 2 to 3
pages in length. Prior to completing an article review you
MUST get the article
approved by Dr. Mohler.
Goal: to demonstrate knowledge of statistical concepts
discussed in class and that you
can apply these to research.
REQUIREMENTS
Must use APA format
� Should include a title and references page.
� You only need to reference your article. You DO NOT need
to find alternative sources.
� Any idea that is not your own should be cited within the body
of the paper.
� Size 12 font, Times of New Roman font, double spaced.
� Given the length of your paper being short, you DO NOT
need to include headers, footers, graphs, or an abstract within
the
body of the paper. Rather, I just want to you get comfortable
making a title page, using running heads, citing sources, and
referencing your article APA style.
Resources
�
https://owl.purdue.edu/owl/research_and_citation/apa_style/apa
_formatting_and_style_guide/reference_list_electronic_source
s.html
� http://my.ilstu.edu/~jhkahn/apastats.html
� http://info.csp.edu/globalassets/academic-resources/writing-
center/docs/apa-sample-paper-6th-edition.pdf.
� Helpful Video:
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=9pbUoNa5tyY
� No Plagiarism! This will result in an automatic score of 0 for
the assignment.
� 2-3 pages in length (NOT including your title and reference
pages)
� If it is less than 2 pages, it will be reflected in your grade
https://owl.purdue.edu/owl/research_and_citation/apa_style/apa
_formatting_and_style_guide/reference_list_electronic_sources.
html
http://my.ilstu.edu/~jhkahn/apastats.html
http://info.csp.edu/globalassets/academic-resources/writing-
center/docs/apa-sample-paper-6th-edition.pdf
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=9pbUoNa5tyY
TYPES OF ARTICLES TO CHOOSE
Can be on any topic of interest!
Needs to be a peer reviewed/academic journal article.
� Library journal (online accessible)
� Search Psychology or PsychINFO databases in UALR Library
� Search “Psychology” related journal articles & limit results
to be peer reviewed journals
� Google Scholar
Look for a study, NOT a literature review or a news article
An appropriate article will typically have the following
categories:
❖Literature review
❖Rationale for the current study/Hypotheses
❖Methods/Participants
❖Statistical Analyses
❖Results
❖Discussion
THOUGHTS TO CONSIDER:
This is a time to apply and demonstrate your critical thinking
skills!
DO NOT WRITE ABOUT
� If you agree/disagree with the outcome of the study.
� A long summary of the article and results (brief only)
Rather, write about:
� Concepts discussed in class
THINGS TO POTENTIALLY WRITE ABOUT:
� Who is part of the sample? Can the conclusions be
generalized to the entire population? Think population
and sample.
� Did they use adequate sampling (e.g., random sampling vs.
convenience sampling?)
� What are the variables they used? How do they operationally
define them?
� What type of data was used (e.g., nominal, ordinal, interval,
ratio OR continuous, discrete).
� Identify the IV and DV and describe how they used these in
the study.
� Did they display the data adequately? Why or why not?
� What types of graphs did they use?
� Does the data adequately support the conclusion drawn by the
researcher(s)?
� Measures of central tendency used within the article.
� Discuss concepts related to Variance.
� Discuss distributions and probabilities and how they were
used within the article.
� Did they identify a hypotheses and undergo hypotheses
testing?
� Discuss Type I and Type II Error as related to your article.
� Any other concepts discussed in class that may be relevant to
your article!
� It is okay if you cannot write about everything (e.g.,
statistical analyses used or all the concept outlined
above). Write about what you have learned already and what is
relevant to your article.
TIMELINE
3/6/19: Last day to have Dr. Mohler Approve article
� Email me the article you are wanting to use and I will
approve or let you know my
concerns. Let me know if you need assistance. I’d be happy to
assist you in locating
an article before or after class. Please email me to set up a time
to do this. Come
with some topics of interest.
4/15/19 by 7:30pm: Article Review Paper due
� You will upload your paper AND the chosen article you
wrote about
on Blackboard.
� Your paper will NOT be accepted and graded without your
article
attached to Blackboard.
QUESTIONS ABOUT THE ARTICLE REVIEW PAPER?
245
The Journal of Social Psychology, 2005, 145(3), 245–264
Rethinking Procrastination: Positive Effects
of “Active” Procrastination Behavior on
Attitudes and Performance
ANGELA HSIN CHUN CHU
Department of Organizational Psychology
Columbia University
New York, New York
JIN NAM CHOI
Faculty of Management
McGill University
Montreal, Quebec, Canada
ABSTRACT. Researchers and practitioners have long regarded
procrastination as a self-
handicapping and dysfunctional behavior. In the present study,
the authors proposed that
not all procrastination behaviors either are harmful or lead to
negative consequences.
Specifically, the authors differentiated two types of
procrastinators: passive procrastinators
versus active procrastinators. Passive procrastinators are
procrastinators in the traditional
sense. They are paralyzed by their indecision to act and fail to
complete tasks on time. In
contrast, active procrastinators are a “positive” type of
procrastinator. They prefer to work
under pressure, and they make deliberate decisions to
procrastinate. The present results
showed that although active procrastinators procrastinate to the
same degree as passive pro-
crastinators, they are more similar to nonprocrastinators than to
passive procrastinators in
terms of purposive use of time, control of time, self-efficacy
belief, coping styles, and out-
comes including academic performance. The present findings
offer a more sophisticated
understanding of procrastination behavior and indicate a need to
reevaluate its implications
for outcomes of individuals.
Key words: coping strategies, procrastination, self-efficacy,
time management
INVESTIGATORS HAVE DEFINED PROCRASTINATION as
the lack or
absence of self-regulated performance and the behavioral
tendency to postpone
what is necessary to reach a goal (Ellis & Knaus, 1977; Knaus,
2000). The grow-
ing body of literature has demonstrated that procrastination is
not just a problem
of time management. It is a complex process that involves
affective, cognitive,
and behavioral components (Fee & Tangney, 2000). Blunt and
Pychyl (1998) and
Harriott and Ferrari (1996) have found procrastination to be a
prevalent phe-
nomenon in the general population, chronically affecting a
substantial portion of
adults as well as university students. Procrastination may have
particularly seri-
ous consequences for university students, whose lives are
characterized by fre-
quent deadlines. For example, Tice and Baumeister (1997)
reported that univer-
sity students who rated high on procrastination not only
received low grades but
also reported a high level of stress along with poor self-rated
health.
Most of the existing literature on procrastination has contrasted
procrasti-
nators with nonprocrastinators. Procrastination has been
considered a self-
handicapping behavior that leads to wasted time, poor
performance, and
increased stress. Investigators such as Ferrari (2001) have often
depicted pro-
crastinators as lazy or self-indulgent individuals who are unable
to self-regu-
late. In contrast, nonprocrastination has been associated with
high efficiency,
productivity, and superior performance, and nonprocrastinators
are often
described as organized and highly motivated individuals (e.g.,
Bond & Feath-
er, 1988; Ellis & Knaus, 1977).
Although both the practical literature and the academic
literature have asso-
ciated negative connotations to procrastination, investigators
have found that pro-
crastination can induce some short-term benefits. For example,
Tice and
Baumeister (1997) reported that compared with
nonprocrastinators, procrastina-
tors experience less stress and have better physical health when
deadlines are far
off. In this sense, procrastination can be seen as a strategy that
they use to regu-
late negative emotions, thereby making the individual feel
better, at least tem-
porarily (Baumeister, Heatherton, & Tice, 1994). Moreover, in
principle, whether
a person does a task far ahead of a deadline or only slightly
ahead of it does not
necessarily affect the quality of the work (Tice & Baumeister).
Therefore, prac-
tically speaking, procrastination does not necessarily have a
negative impact on
the effectiveness of the task performance.
In a similar vein, Knaus (2000) argued that not all delays lead
to negative out-
comes. For example, delays resulting from time that was spent
planning and gath-
ering vital preparatory information can be beneficial (Knaus).
Many people claim
that even when they start to work at the last minute, they can
still finish on time
and that they tend to work better and faster or generate more
creative ideas under
time pressure. This line of thought on procrastination suggests
that there might be
more than one kind of procrastinator and that in some cases
procrastination behav-
ior might lead to positive outcomes. In the present study, we
identify two types of
procrastinators and examine whether they have distinct
characteristics in terms of
the use and perception of time, their self-efficacy beliefs, their
motivational ori-
entation, their stress-coping strategies, and their personal
outcomes.
246 The Journal of Social Psychology
Address correspondence to Jin Nam Choi, McGill University,
Faculty of Management,
1001 Sherbrooke Street West, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3A
1G5; [email protected]
mcgill.ca (e-mail).
Passive and Active Procrastination
Addressing the possibility that not all procrastination behaviors
have nega-
tive effects, we conceptually distinguished two different types
of procrastinators:
passive versus active procrastinators. Passive procrastinators
are procrastinators
in the traditional sense. Cognitively, passive procrastinators do
not intend to pro-
crastinate, but they often end up postponing tasks because of
their inability to
make decisions quickly and to thereby act on them quickly.
Active procrastina-
tors, in contrast, are capable of acting on their decisions in a
timely manner. How-
ever, they suspend their actions deliberately and focus their
attention on other
important tasks at hand. Therefore, passive procrastinators
differ from active pro-
crastinators on cognitive, affective, and behavioral dimensions.
Affectively, when a deadline approaches, passive
procrastinators feel pres-
sured and become pessimistic in their outlook, especially about
their ability to
achieve satisfactory results (Ferrari, Parker, & Ware, 1992).
Their thoughts of
self-doubt and inadequacy increase the chance of failure and
induce feelings of
guilt and depression (Steel, Brothen, & Wambach, 2001). Active
procrastinators,
on the other hand, like to work under pressure. When faced with
last-minute
tasks, they feel challenged and motivated, and that feeling
immunizes them
against the kind of suffering common in passive procrastinators.
Different cog-
nitive pathways and affective responses interact to produce
different behavioral
patterns: Active procrastinators are persistent and able to
complete tasks at the
last minute. Passive procrastinators, on the other hand, are more
likely to give up
and fail to complete tasks.
On the basis of the above circumstances, we proposed that
active pro-
crastination is a multifaceted phenomenon that includes
cognitive (decision
to procrastinate), affective (preference for time pressure), and
behavioral
(task completion by the deadline) components as well as the
physical results
and satisfaction with them. Because of these fundamental
differences (cog-
nitive, affective, and behavioral), we expected active and
passive procrasti-
nators to have distinct psychological characteristics and to
achieve different
outcomes.
Time Use and Perception
One of the most common recommendations for effective
management of
time is to properly structure one’s use of time in combination
with a clear sense
of purpose. For example, Dipboye and Phillips (1990) showed
that university stu-
dents who had more sense of purpose and structure in their use
of time reported
greater psychological well-being and more efficient habits of
study. Also,
Vodanovich and Seib (1997) illustrated that procrastinators tend
to have weak, if
any, structure in their time use. Therefore, we expected
nonprocrastinators, who
tend to plan their activities on a day-to-day basis, to have time
use that is more
Chu & Choi 247
structured and to perceive their use of time to be more
purposive. Traditional (pas-
sive) procrastinators, who incorporate less structure in their
time use, may drift
aimlessly from one activity to another (Bond & Feather, 1988).
Active procrasti-
nators, in contrast, are expected to be different from passive
procrastinators in
having more time structure and a better sense of purpose in their
time use because
they are able to make deliberate decisions regarding their time
use on the basis
of urgency or priority. In this sense, active procrastinators are
similar to nonpro-
crastinators in terms of engagement in time-structuring
behaviors and purposive
use of time.
Despite the fact that we can measure time, our perception of
time is a truly
subjective experience (Macan, 1994). Lay (1990) found passive
procrastinators
to underestimate the overall time that was required to complete
tasks. Conse-
quently, they often failed to complete tasks on time, triggering
the perception of
reduced control of time (Lay & Schouwenburg, 1993). In
contrast, nonprocrasti-
nators, who constantly engage in planning and organizing, tend
to have more real-
istic perceptions of time and to perceive more control over their
time (Macan).
Active procrastinators may be similar to nonprocrastinators in
that they take
charge of their time and try to maximize the efficiency of their
time use. Conse-
quently, they would develop an awareness of their time use and
a perception of
time control.
Hypothesis 1: Both nonprocrastinators and active
procrastinators will report more
time structure, more purposive use of time, and greater
perception of control of time
than will passive procrastinators. Nonprocrastinators and active
procrastinators are
not different on these dimensions.
Self-Efficacy
The association between self-efficacy and procrastination was
first introduced
by Bandura (1986). Self-efficacy refers to the belief that one
can reliably perform the
tasks that are required for successful goal achievement
(Bandura, 1977). Bandura
(1986) hypothesized that when adequate levels of ability and
motivation exist, self-
efficacy belief will affect a person’s task initiation and
persistence. Several investi-
gators have examined the relationship between self-efficacy and
procrastination. For
example, Ferrari et al. (1992) and Tuckman (1991) have found
an inverse relation-
ship between self-efficacy belief and academic procrastination
among college stu-
dents. In line with the belief that self-efficacy plays a role in
task initiation and task
persistence, in the present study we expected that
nonprocrastinators will have a
stronger self-efficacy belief than passive procrastinators. As
with nonprocrastinators,
we expected active procrastinators to have a stronger self-
efficacy belief than do pas-
sive procrastinators. Active procrastinators postpone tasks and
direct their attention
toward more urgent issues because they feel that they have
control over their time
use (Hypothesis 1) and are confident in their ability to finish
tasks on time.
248 The Journal of Social Psychology
Hypothesis 2: Both nonprocrastinators and active
procrastinators will report stronger
self-efficacy beliefs than will passive procrastinators.
Nonprocrastinators and active
procrastinators are not different on this dimension.
Motivational Orientation
Motivation is a force that drives a person to engage in a
particular activity.
Investigators have identified two types of motivation: intrinsic
and extrinsic.
According to Deci and Ryan (1985), intrinsic motivation refers
to motivation that
results from an internal drive. Extrinsic motivation, in contrast,
refers to motiva-
tion that results from either positive or negative external
contingencies. Brown-
low and Reasinger (2000) found that both intrinsic motivation
and extrinsic moti-
vation were negatively related to academic procrastination.
Conti (2000)
suggested that although extrinsic motivation was necessary to
prevent task delay,
participants with intrinsic motivation devoted more time to their
projects than did
participants with extrinsic motivation.
On the basis of these findings (Brownlow & Reasinger, 2000;
Conti, 2000),
we hypothesized that nonprocrastinators are motivated by both
extrinsic motiva-
tion and intrinsic motivation because nonprocrastinators tend to
not delay task
performance and to perhaps spend more time on the task. In
contrast, because of
their frequent task delay and task incompletion, passive
procrastinators seem to
have low intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation. Finally,
we speculated that
active procrastinators have relatively high levels of extrinsic
motivation but low
levels of intrinsic motivation. Active procrastinators are able to
finish tasks at the
last minute, but they do not devote more time than necessary to
each task. Their
orientation toward work and life may be to achieve as much as
possible in the
least possible time, manifesting a value that is more closely
aligned with extrin-
sic motivation than with intrinsic motivation.
Hypothesis 3a: Both nonprocrastinators and active
procrastinators will report a high-
er level of extrinsic motivation than that which passive
procrastinators will report.
Nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators are not different
on this dimension.
Hypothesis 3b: Nonprocrastinators will report a higher level of
intrinsic motivation
than those that active procrastinators and passive
procrastinators will report.
Stress-Coping Strategy
When dealing with stress, individuals try to remove the threat
from the stres-
sor or to reduce the discomfort caused by the stressor (Latack &
Havlovic, 1992).
The three most frequently mentioned coping strategies include
task-oriented strate-
gies, emotion-oriented strategies, and avoidance-oriented
strategies (Carver,
Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989; Endler & Parker, 1990, 1994;
Kosic, 2004). Task-ori-
ented coping strategies reduce stress by focusing on immediate
problems. Emotion-
Chu & Choi 249
oriented coping strategies involve diminishing the emotional
distress that is induced
by the stressors. And avoidance-oriented coping strategies
involve ignoring a prob-
lem or distracting oneself from it. Although most stressors elicit
a mixture of cop-
ing strategies, task-oriented strategies predominate when
individuals feel that they
can do something constructive in regard to the situation, and
emotion- and avoid-
ance-oriented strategies emerge when people feel that they do
not have much con-
trol over the stressors (Folkman & Lazarus, 1980). Therefore, a
person’s self-effi-
cacy level partially determines which coping strategy he or she
will use.
Expanding this line of thought, we expected that
nonprocrastinators will use
task-oriented coping strategies to alleviate stress by considering
realistic alterna-
tives in response to problems they face. Nonprocrastinators may
have a higher
level of self-efficacy belief (Hypothesis 2) and thus believe that
they have some
control over the stressors and are capable of improving their
situation. As a result,
instead of engaging in irrelevant tasks or self-handicapping
behaviors, they will
choose to work on solving the problems at hand. Similarly,
active procrastinators
will use task-oriented coping, because their high level of self-
efficacy belief
makes them feel competent to overcome stressful circumstances.
In contrast, we expected passive procrastinators to use either
emotion- or
avoidance-oriented coping strategies when encountering
stressful events. Ferrari
and Tice (2000) found that procrastinators were more concerned
about their self-
image than were nonprocrastinators, offering another
explanation for the coping
strategy that was used. Low self-efficacy belief and concern for
poor self-image
together make passive procrastinators feel powerless and cause
them to actively
avoid negative situations. As a result, they may cry, complain,
or whine to express
their frustration and fears; they may engage in trivial, irrelevant
tasks to distract
themselves; or they may ignore tasks completely.
Hypothesis 4: In stressful situations, nonprocrastinators and
active procrastinators
will use task-oriented coping strategies, and passive
procrastinators will use either
emotion- or avoidance-oriented coping strategies.
Personal Outcomes
Investigators have reported that procrastination leads to low
academic
achievement, elevated levels of anxiety, stress, feelings of
hopelessness, depres-
sion, and poor physical health; the opposite patterns have been
observed among
nonprocrastinators (Beswick, Rothblum, & Mann, 1988; Bond &
Feather, 1988;
Dipboye & Phillips, 1990; Ellis & Knaus, 1977; Owens &
Newbegin, 1997; Tice
& Baumeister, 1997). In the present study, we examined four
outcome variables:
stress, depression, life satisfaction, and performance level as
indicated by grade
point average (GPA). Because previous studies have revealed
opposite outcome
patterns between passive procrastination and nonprocrastination
with the four
outcome variables, we expected to obtain similar results in the
present study.
250 The Journal of Social Psychology
On the basis of our conceptualization of active procrastinators,
we expected
them to achieve positive outcomes rather than negative
outcomes. As discussed
earlier in the present article, active procrastinators are more
likely to engage in
behaviors of time management, perceive their use of time to be
more purposive,
and feel more control over their use of time than do passive
procrastinators. Based
on the existing findings of the positive association between
patterns of time use
or perception and individual well-being (Bond & Feather, 1988;
Macan, 1994;
Macan, Shahani, Dipboye, & Phillips, 1990), constructive time-
related percep-
tion and behavior among active procrastinators will reduce their
stress and
depression, inducing increased life satisfaction and academic
performance.
The positive outcomes of active procrastinators may also derive
from their
self-efficacy belief, motivation, and stress-coping strategies that
we hypothesized
in the preceding sections. Compared with passive
procrastinators, active procras-
tinators have greater confidence in their own abilities in
overcoming obstacles. As
task-oriented individuals, active procrastinators direct their
attention and actions
toward pressing problems, offering themselves a greater chance
of solving them
in a satisfactory manner. Active procrastinators are extrinsically
motivated and
attempt to maximize their time utility, possibly leading to more
successful task
completion. Because successful task completion among
nonprocrastinators
reduces stress and feelings of depression and increases life
satisfaction and aca-
demic performance (e.g., Dipboye & Phillips, 1990; Tice &
Baumeister, 1997),
in the present study we expected the same positive outcomes
among active pro-
crastinators.
Hypothesis 5: Both nonprocrastinators and active
procrastinators will report a lower
level of stress and depression, greater life satisfaction, and a
higher GPA than pas-
sive procrastinators. Nonprocrastinators and active
procrastinators are not different
on these dimensions.
Method
Participants and Procedure
Participants were 230 undergraduate students (166 women
[72.2%], 64 men
[27.8%]) from three Canadian universities. The mean age of the
sample was
21.49 years (SD = 2.23 years). The distribution of participants’
racial background
was as follows: 53.7% Asian, 33.3% White, 6.5% Hispanic,
1.5% African Amer-
ican, and 5.0% other. Regarding first language, 47.8% of the
participants report-
ed English, 32% reported Chinese, 7.9% reported French, and
12.3% reported
other. The majority (96.5%) of participants were full-time
students, and the aver-
age year at university was 2.67 years. We invited participants to
take part in the
study by filling out a questionnaire entitled “Survey of
University Students’ Time
Use.” Participation was entirely voluntary, and participants
completed the ques-
tionnaire at their own convenience.
Chu & Choi 251
Measures
To measure the study variables, we used multi-item scales with
acceptable
internal consistencies. Whenever possible, we adopted existing
measures with
demonstrated validity and reliability. A 7-point Likert-type
scale (1 = not at all true,
7 = very true) was used as the response format for all items.
Academic procrastination. To measure the degree of
procrastination, we adopted
six items (α = .82) from two existing measures of
procrastination (L. Mann’s
[1982] Decisional Procrastinational Scale, as cited in Ferrari,
Johnson, & McCown,
1995; H. C. Schouwenburg’s [1995] “Academic Procrastination:
Theoretical
Notions, Measurement, and Research,” as cited in Ferrari et al.;
e.g., “I tend to
leave things until the last minute”).
Active procrastination. Based on our conceptualization, we
developed a 12-item
scale (α = .67) to distinguish active procrastinators from passive
procrastinators.
The scale was designed to measure four defining characteristics
of active procras-
tinators: (a) preference for pressure (e.g., “I tend to work better
under pressure”),
(b) intentional procrastination (e.g., “I intentionally put off
work to maximize my
motivation”), (c) ability to meet deadlines (e.g., “Since I often
start working on
things at the last moment, I have trouble finishing assigned
tasks most of the time”
[reverse coded]), and (d) outcome satisfaction (e.g., “I feel that
putting work off
until the last minute does not do me any good” [reverse coded]).
We tested the fac-
tor structure of the 12 items measuring active procrastination
using principal-com-
ponent extraction with varimax rotation. This exploratory
analysis generated four
factors that confirm the hypothesized factor structure, with high
factor loadings on
the corresponding factors (all greater than .50) and low cross-
loadings (all less than
.24). (Complete results of factor analysis and items are
available from Jin Nam Choi
on request.) We used a composite measure of these four
subscales to assess the over-
all level of the tendency of individuals toward active
procrastination.
Patterns of time use. Adopting items from the Time Structure
Questionnaire
(Bond & Feather, 1988), we assessed two aspects of time use.
One was the struc-
ture of time use (3 items, α = .70, e.g., “I have a daily routine,
which I follow”),
and the other was purposive use of time (3 items, α = .73, e.g.,
“I often feel that
my life is aimless, with no definite purpose”).
Perception of time control. To measure participants’ perceptions
of control of their
time, we used two items (α = .66) developed by Macan et al.
(1990; e.g., “I feel
in control of my time” and “I can use my time the way I want to
use it”).
Self-efficacy belief. The self-efficacy belief scale included four
items (α = .76)
drawn from the Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale (Schwarzer &
Jerusalem,
252 The Journal of Social Psychology
1995) to measure individuals’ beliefs about their ability to
successfully per-
form desired tasks (e.g., “I believe that I can solve most
problems if I invest
enough effort”).
Motivational orientation. The motivation scale contained seven
items from Shia’s
(1998) Academic Motivation Questionnaire. We measured both
extrinsic motiva-
tion (3 items, α = .70, e.g., “Getting a good grade in my classes
is the most sat-
isfying thing for me right now”) and intrinsic motivation (4
items, α = .75, e.g.,
“I like courses that arouse my curiosity, even if they are
difficult”) for school
activities.
Stress-coping strategy. To assess how participants respond to
stressful situations,
we adopted items from the Proactive Coping Inventory
(Greenglass, Schwarzer,
& Taubert, 1999): (a) task-oriented coping behavior (3 items, α
= .71, e.g., “I just
focus on the task to get what I want”), (b) emotion-oriented
coping behavior (3
items, α = .82, e.g., “I get out and talk to others to deal with
stress”), and (c)
avoidance-oriented coping behavior (3 items, α = .67, e.g., “I
try to turn my atten-
tion away from the problem”).
Stress. We used a four-item scale (α = .77) to measure the level
of stress that indi-
viduals had experienced in the previous month (MacArthur &
MacArthur, 2001;
e.g., “In the past one month, how often did you feel difficulties
were piling up so
high that you could not overcome them?”). Each statement was
rated on a 7-point
Likert-type scale (1 = never, 7 = very often).
Depression. We used a four-item scale (α = .73) to measure the
level of depres-
sion (Sheikh & Yesavage, 1982; e.g., “I often feel downhearted
and blue”).
Life satisfaction. This scale consisted of four items (α = .82)
that assess stu-
dents’ general satisfaction with life (e.g., “In general, I am
satisfied with my
life”).
Academic performance. For a measure of academic
performance, participants
reported their GPA.
Results
Table 1 presents means, standard deviations, and correlation
coefficients
among the scales used in the present study. The correlation
between the academ-
ic procrastination scale and the active procrastination scale was
.03 (p > .60), indi-
cating that these two scales were independent and that they
measured nonover-
lapping conceptual domains. Virtually all correlations involving
academic
procrastination and active procrastination were in the
hypothesized directions.
Chu & Choi 253
254 The Journal of Social Psychology
T
A
B
L
E
1
.
M
ea
n
s,
S
ta
n
d
ar
d
D
ev
ia
ti
on
s,
an
d
I
n
te
rc
or
re
la
ti
on
s
A
m
on
g
V
ar
ia
b
le
s
V
ar
ia
bl
e
M
S
D
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
1.
A
ca
de
m
ic
pr
oc
ra
st
in
at
io
n
4.
51
1.
22
—
2.
A
ct
iv
e
pr
oc
ra
st
in
at
io
n
4.
36
0.
79
.0
3
—
3.
T
im
e
st
ru
ct
ur
e
4.
04
1.
32
–.
27
**
*
–.
10
—
4.
P
ur
po
si
ve
u
se
of
t
im
e
4.
72
1.
47
–.
27
**
*
.2
4*
**
.1
1
—
5.
P
er
ce
iv
ed
t
im
e
co
nt
ro
l
4.
27
1.
43
–.
40
**
*
.2
0*
*
.1
9*
*
.3
6*
**
—
6.
S
el
f-
ef
fi
ca
cy
5.
10
1.
04
–.
13
*
.3
4*
**
.0
5
.3
2*
**
.4
7*
**
—
7.
E
xt
ri
ns
ic
m
ot
iv
at
io
n
4.
13
1.
44
.0
1
–.
20
**
.1
9*
*
–.
16
*
–.
19
**
–.
27
**
*
—
8.
In
tr
in
si
c
m
ot
iv
at
io
n
4.
98
1.
15
–.
06
.1
0
.2
0*
*
.1
5*
.0
7
.2
5*
**
.0
4
9.
T
as
k
co
pi
ng
4.
96
0.
99
–.
05
.2
7*
**
.0
7
.2
8*
**
.1
7*
.4
3*
**
–.
03
10
.
E
m
ot
io
n
co
pi
ng
4.
34
1.
59
.1
6*
–.
05
–.
05
–.
14
*
–.
12
–.
10
.0
9
11
.
A
vo
id
an
ce
co
pi
ng
3.
19
1.
30
.2
3*
*
–.
22
**
.0
2
–.
45
**
*
–.
24
**
*
–.
35
**
*
.2
0*
*
12
.
S
tr
es
s
4.
17
1.
26
.2
5*
**
–.
31
**
*
.0
0
–.
39
**
*
–.
38
**
*
–.
44
**
*
.2
4*
**
13
.
D
ep
re
ss
io
n
2.
93
1.
18
.1
8*
*
–.
27
**
*
–.
04
–.
56
**
*
–.
33
**
*
–.
44
**
*
.1
8*
*
14
.
L
if
e
sa
ti
sf
ac
ti
on
4.
97
1.
16
–.
10
.1
9*
*
.1
8*
*
.4
6*
**
.3
5*
**
.4
4*
**
–.
06
15
.
G
PA
3.
25
0.
40
–.
20
**
.2
3*
*
.1
1
.2
4*
*
.1
4*
.3
0*
**
–.
02
(t
a
b
le
c
o
n
ti
n
u
es
)
Chu & Choi 255
T
A
B
L
E
1
.
C
on
ti
n
u
ed
V
ar
ia
bl
e
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
8.
In
tr
in
si
c
m
ot
iv
at
io
n
—
9.
T
as
k
co
pi
ng
.3
0*
**
—
10
.
E
m
ot
io
n
co
pi
ng
.0
1
.0
3
—
11
.
A
vo
id
an
ce
co
pi
ng
–.
12
–.
30
**
*
.2
3*
**
—
12
.
S
tr
es
s
–.
03
–.
27
**
*
.1
9*
*
.3
1*
**
—
13
.
D
ep
re
ss
io
n
–.
08
–.
26
**
*
.0
2
.3
7*
**
.5
5*
**
—
14
.
L
if
e
sa
ti
sf
ac
ti
on
.1
3*
.3
0*
**
.0
8
–.
24
**
*
–.
39
**
*
–.
71
**
*
—
15
.
G
PA
.2
7*
**
.2
2*
*
.0
2
–.
23
**
–.
18
*
.2
2*
*
–.
23
**
—
N
o
te
.
G
PA
=
g
ra
de
p
oi
nt
a
ve
ra
ge
.
*p
<
.
05
.
**
p
<
.
01
.
**
*p
<
.
00
1.
Comparing the Three Groups Using One-Way Analysis of
Variance (ANOVA)
The present hypotheses involved a series of comparisons among
three dis-
tinct groups of people: nonprocrastinators, passive
procrastinators, and active
procrastinators. To test the hypotheses, we created three equal-
sized subgroups
from the entire sample in a two-step process. First,
nonprocrastinators were sep-
arated from procrastinators. We categorized participants who
scored less than 4
(which was the midpoint of a 7-point scale) on the academic
procrastination scale
as nonprocrastinators and categorized those who scored greater
than 4 as pro-
crastinators. In our sample of 230 participants, 77 were
categorized as nonpro-
crastinators, and 153 were categorized as procrastinators.
Admittedly, using a
scale midpoint to distinguish between procrastinators and
nonprocrastinators
might be somewhat arbitrary for the purpose of identifying
procrastinators (cf.
Brownlow & Reasinger, 2000). However, we used this
procedure intentionally
here to compare nonprocrastinators with different types of
procrastinators with
increased statistical power, rather than comparing “chronic”
procrastinators with
“pathological” procrastinators.
In the second step, we further divided the 153 procrastinators
into two
groups: passive procrastinators and active procrastinators. To
create two groups
of comparable sizes, we chose 4.33 as the cutoff point on the
active procrastina-
tion scale. Participants who scored less than 4.33 were
categorized as passive pro-
crastinators (n = 74), and participants who scored greater than
4.33 were catego-
rized as active procrastinators (n = 79).
Table 2 presents the results of a one-way ANOVA, in which we
compared
the means of the study variables among the three groups. The
first row in Table
2 shows the comparison of the level of procrastination across
the three groups of
individuals as identified earlier in the present article. Paired t
tests indicate that
passive and active procrastinators reported significantly more
academic procras-
tination than did nonprocrastinators (p < .001), whereas they
were not different
from each other, mean difference = .10, t(151) = .84. Because
we compared three
groups, applying the Bonferroni procedure (Rosenthal &
Rosnow, 1991, pp.
329–332), we used an adjusted alpha level (.5 ÷ 3 = .017) as the
criterion for sta-
tistical significance of t tests. In addition, for each inferential
test, effect estimates
in Cohen’s d were provided (Rosenthal & Rosnow, pp. 301–
317).
Time use and perception. The results of the ANOVA shown in
Table 2 indicated
that, as we hypothesized, there were significant mean
differences in patterns of
time use and time perception among the three groups.
Nonprocrastinators and
active procrastinators showed higher levels of purposive use of
time and time con-
trol than did passive procrastinators (F = 7.34, p < .01, and F =
19.50, p < .001,
respectively; for all F tests hereafter, df = 2, 227, effect size
estimates in Cohen’s
d ranging between 0.41 and 1.06, all significant at p < .001).
Nonprocrastinators
reported greater perceived time control than did active
procrastinators (d = .56,
256 The Journal of Social Psychology
Chu & Choi 257
T
A
B
L
E
2
.
R
es
u
lt
s
of
O
n
e-
W
ay
A
N
O
V
A
:
C
om
p
ar
is
on
s
of
M
ea
n
s
of
N
on
p
ro
cr
as
ti
n
at
or
s,
P
as
si
ve
P
ro
cr
as
ti
n
at
or
s,
an
d
A
ct
iv
e
P
ro
cr
as
ti
n
at
or
s
N
on
pr
oc
ra
st
in
at
or
s
P
as
si
ve
p
ro
cr
as
ti
na
to
rs
A
ct
iv
e
pr
oc
ra
st
in
at
or
s
V
ar
ia
bl
e
N
=
7
7
N
=
7
4
N
=
7
9
F
(2
,
22
7)
p
A
ca
de
m
ic
p
ro
cr
as
ti
na
ti
on
3.
12
5.
16
5.
26
21
3.
86
<
.
00
1
T
im
e
st
ru
ct
ur
e
4.
46
4.
16
3.
52
11
.3
3
<
.
00
1
P
ur
po
si
ve
u
se
o
f
ti
m
e
5.
08
4.
21
4.
84
7.
34
.0
01
P
er
ce
iv
ed
t
im
e
co
nt
ro
l
4.
95
3.
60
4.
22
19
.5
0
<
.
00
1
S
el
f-
ef
fi
ca
cy
5.
24
4.
66
5.
38
11
.1
1
<
.
00
1
E
xt
ri
ns
ic
m
ot
iv
at
io
n
4.
23
4.
41
3.
77
4.
09
.0
18
In
tr
in
si
c
m
ot
iv
at
io
n
5.
10
4.
82
5.
02
1.
18
.3
08
T
as
k
co
pi
ng
5.
00
4.
66
5.
22
6.
21
.0
02
E
m
ot
io
n
co
pi
ng
4.
01
4.
54
4.
48
2.
55
.0
81
A
vo
id
an
ce
c
op
in
g
2.
78
3.
85
2.
97
16
.3
1
<
.
00
1
S
tr
es
s
3.
87
4.
79
3.
87
14
.9
4
<
.
00
1
L
if
e
sa
ti
sf
ac
ti
on
5.
04
4.
74
5.
12
2.
22
.1
11
D
ep
re
ss
io
n
2.
84
3.
29
2.
67
5.
78
.0
04
G
PA
3.
39
3.
09
3.
26
9.
79
<
.
00
1
N
o
te
.
G
PA
=
g
ra
de
p
oi
nt
a
ve
ra
ge
.
p < .001), but the difference between the two groups in
purposive use of time was
not significant (d = .17, ns). Surprisingly, however, active
procrastinators report-
ed a lower level of time structure than did the other two groups
(both ds > .49, p
< .01), and the difference between nonprocrastinators and
passive procrastinators
was not significant (d = .25, ns). We will address this
counterintuitive pattern
regarding time structure later in the present article.
Self-efficacy belief. Passive procrastinators had a significantly
lower self-effica-
cy belief than did the other two groups (both ds > .53, p < .001).
No meaningful
difference was found between nonprocrastinators and active
procrastinators (d =
.15, ns). Therefore, Hypothesis 2 was supported.
Motivational orientation. In Hypotheses 3a and 3b, we
suggested that nonpro-
crastinators and active procrastinators would display higher
extrinsic motivation
than would passive procrastinators and that nonprocrastinators
would exhibit high-
er intrinsic motivation than would both types of procrastinators.
A significant dif-
ference was found in extrinsic motivation (F = 4.09, p < .05)
but not in intrinsic
motivation. Interestingly, passive procrastinators exhibited a
higher level of extrin-
sic motivation than did active procrastinators (d = .43, p < .01).
On the basis of the
Bonferroni-adjusted criterion, the other two mean comparisons
were not signifi-
cant (d < .33, p > .04). Therefore, Hypotheses 3a and 3b were
not supported.
Stress-coping strategy. Hypothesis 4 posited that when facing
stressful situations,
nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators will employ task-
oriented coping
strategies, whereas passive procrastinators will be inclined to
use either emotion-
oriented coping strategies or avoidance-oriented coping
strategies. The ANOVA
results in Table 2 supported this hypothesis. We identified
significant differences
among the three groups in task-oriented coping strategy (F =
6.21, p < .01) and
avoidance-oriented coping strategy (F = 16.31, p < .001) but not
in emotion-ori-
ented coping strategy. Active procrastinators reported
significantly greater use of
the task-coping strategy than did passive procrastinators (d =
.55, p < .001),
although the other two mean comparisons were not statistically
significant. Both
nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators reported
significantly less use of
avoidance-coping behavior than did passive procrastinators (ds
= .92 and .68,
respectively, both ps < .001). The results support Hypothesis 4.
Personal outcomes. According to Hypothesis 5,
nonprocrastinators and active
procrastinators will experience less stress and depression and
more life satisfac-
tion and higher GPAs than will passive procrastinators. The
present data largely
confirmed this hypothesis. Among the three groups, significant
differences were
observed in stress (F = 14.94, p < .001), depression (F = 5.78, p
< .01), and GPA
(F = 9.79, p < .001). We found no significant differences
between nonprocrasti-
nators and active procrastinators on any of the four outcome
measures, indicat-
258 The Journal of Social Psychology
ing that these two groups achieved positive outcomes to a
comparable degree (all
ds < .22, ns). In contrast, a series of paired t tests showed that
passive procrasti-
nators were significantly different from both nonprocrastinators
and active pro-
crastinators, reporting greater stress, more feelings of
depression, and lower
GPAs (all ds > .39, ps < .017).
Post Hoc Regression Analysis
We could have directly tested the present hypotheses through a
series of com-
parisons among the three different groups of individuals as
shown in Table 2.
However, because the present article describes the construct of
active procrasti-
nation for the first time and because it is a continuous variable,
it would be ben-
eficial to understand how various attitude and coping variables
that we use in the
present study predict this new construct. To this end, we
conducted regression
analyses that evaluate the prediction of academic
procrastination and active pro-
crastination. As shown in Table 3, we used all variables that
were included in this
study as predictors, except the four outcome measures (stress,
life satisfaction,
depression, and GPA), because it is conceptually unlikely that
these outcomes
determine procrastination behavior.
The results showed that both (a) the traditional procrastination
behavior as
indicated by academic procrastination and (b) active
procrastination behavior
were negatively related to perceived time structure (β = −.19, p
< .01, and β =
−.13, p < .05, respectively). However, only academic
procrastination was associ-
Chu & Choi 259
TABLE 3. Regression Analyses Predicting Academic
Procrastination and
Active Procrastination
Dependent variable Academic procrastination Active
procrastination
Time structure –.19** –.13*
Purposive use of time –.10 .11
Perceived time control –.35*** .05
Self-efficacy .09 .18*
Extrinsic motivation –.04 –.09
Intrinsic motivation .00 .01
Task coping .05 .14*
Emotion coping .08 –.01
Avoidance coping .14* –.03
Adjusted R 2 .22 .14
F(9, 220) 8.06*** 5.10***
Note. Entries are standardized regression coefficients (β).
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
ated with decreased perception of time control and increased
avoidance-coping
behavior (β = −.35, p < .001, and β = .14, p < .05, respectively).
In contrast, active
procrastination was positively related to self-efficacy and task-
coping behavior
(β = .18, p < .05, and β = .14, p < .05, respectively).
Discussion
The purpose of the present study was to empirically test
whether there are
different types of procrastinators. In particular, we attempted to
identify a posi-
tive type of procrastinator, which we termed active
procrastinators. Contrary to
prevailing assumptions about procrastinators, we revealed that a
subset of pro-
crastinators (i.e., active procrastinators) indeed possess
desirable attitudinal and
behavioral characteristics, even though they may engage in the
same level of pro-
crastination as “traditional” negative procrastinators (i.e.,
passive procrastina-
tors). Members of this newly identified group of
procrastinators, active procras-
tinators, prefer to work under pressure and make deliberate
procrastination
decisions. They are more likely to accomplish tasks with
satisfactory outcomes
than passive procrastinators, who are often paralyzed by
indecision regarding
action and haunted by past failure to complete tasks.
The present results indicate the possibility that
nonprocrastinators and active
procrastinators share similar characteristics and are
significantly different from
passive procrastinators. Nonprocrastinators and active
procrastinators both tend
to have higher levels of purposive use of time, time control, and
self-efficacy than
do passive procrastinators. In addition, nonprocrastinators and
active procrasti-
nators are more likely to experience positive outcomes. This
pattern indicates that
while active procrastinators procrastinate to the same degree as
do passive pro-
crastinators, mean difference = −.10, t(151) = −.84, their
personal characteristics
and outcomes are quite more similar to those of
nonprocrastinators.
Implications for Future Research
We expected active procrastinators to structure their time to a
high degree,
as do nonprocrastinators. Unexpectedly, however, active
procrastinators scored
the lowest on the time structure scale, and there was no
significant difference
between nonprocrastinators and passive procrastinators. One
possible explana-
tion for this finding is that even though active procrastinators
may plan their activ-
ities in an organized fashion, they do not restrict themselves to
following a pre-
planned schedule or time structure. If something unexpectedly
comes up, they
will switch gears and engage in new tasks that they perceive as
more urgent. In
other words, active procrastinators may have more flexibly
structured time and
are more sensitive to changing demands in their environment.
For this reason,
they will act more spontaneously, resulting in more frequent
temporal changes
than with the other two groups. This interpretation, however, is
only speculation.
260 The Journal of Social Psychology
Future studies are needed to investigate the precise mechanisms
underlying this
unexpected pattern by examining variables such as degree of
awareness of envi-
ronmental demands or willingness to make changes.
In line with Conti’s (2000) study, we hypothesized that both
nonprocrastina-
tors and active procrastinators will have a higher level of
extrinsic motivation than
will passive procrastinators. Interestingly, passive
procrastinators turned out to
have the highest level of extrinsic motivation. In the research
setting of the present
study, we constantly evaluated students’ performance on the
basis of externally
imposed requirements, which often effectively reduce an
individual’s sense of
autonomy and enjoyment in regard to a task (Deci & Ryan,
1985). Therefore, stu-
dents who have extrinsic reasons for studying may feel
controlled and pressured
and find the task to be aversive. To avoid these unpleasant
feelings and percep-
tions of being forced to engage in particular activities, they
might be more likely
to postpone their academic work (Blunt & Pychyl, 1998; Lay,
1990). Passive pro-
crastinators, who have weak self-efficacy beliefs, may find
imposed requirements
unpleasant. For this reason, they may develop task aversion and
postpone acade-
mic work to avoid feelings of incompetence. From this
perspective, the only rea-
son for passive procrastinators to perform tasks may be the
threat of serious neg-
ative consequences for failing to perform.
Potential Limitations
Overall, the present study confirms the profile of
nonprocrastinators and tra-
ditional procrastinators (i.e., passive procrastinators) in terms
of their personal
characteristics and outcomes (see Dipboye & Phillips, 1990;
Ferrari et al., 1992;
Lay & Schouwenburg, 1993; Tice & Baumeister, 1997;
Vodanovich & Seib, 1997).
This consistency with prior findings increases our confidence in
the present results.
Nevertheless, the current findings should be interpreted with
caution because of
several limitations of the present study. Our sample consisted of
university stu-
dents, who may possess different characteristics than other
populations such as
organizational employees. Therefore, the generalizability of the
current results to
populations engaging in other types of tasks is limited. Another
limitation is that
all variables were self-reported at one point in time. This cross-
sectional nature of
the data, which was obtained from a single source, raises the
possibility of insen-
sitivity to temporal changes of variables, ambiguous causal
directions of the
observed relationships, and boosted correlations among
variables (Podsakoff &
Organ, 1986).
Practical Implications
Despite these limitations, the present study offers a new
perspective on pro-
crastination and how people perceive and use their time. In
terms of practical
implications, the current findings present a need to break our
long-standing view
Chu & Choi 261
of procrastination as an unhealthy or unproductive behavior. We
should acknowl-
edge the possibility that some forms of procrastination may
actually enhance the
well-being and performance of individuals. Active
procrastination may be partic-
ularly beneficial, or even necessary, for individuals who work
in highly demand-
ing, unpredictable, and fast changing environments. In this type
of context, active
procrastinators may function more effectively than others by not
restricting them-
selves to previously established plans and thus would be able to
deal spontaneously
with unexpected changes. They may constantly engage in a
process of reprioritiz-
ing each of their tasks and thereby increase their capacity to
more effectively meet
frequently changing situational demands. Therefore, active
procrastinators may
function very effectively in highly uncertain situations with
frequently changing
environmental contingencies and demands. Future studies of
this issue of per-
son–situation fit may offer valuable contributions to managerial
practice.
In the present study, we identified a positive form of
procrastination. Future
studies may expand the present findings with the use of
different samples to repli-
cate the presence of active procrastinators in other populations.
Future investiga-
tors might also use a longitudinal research design to detect
temporal changes in
variables and to clarify the causal directions of the observed
relationships. More-
over, conceptual efforts could be devoted to further clarification
of the active pro-
crastination construct. Once investigators have clearly defined
the construct of
active procrastination, and a valid measurement tool becomes
available, the active
procrastination construct could be compared with other
potentially related per-
sonality constructs, such as those of Type A behavior or high-
achievement ori-
entation. It would also be meaningful to develop more precise
descriptions of
active procrastinators in terms of other personality traits such as
the Big 5 factors
or locus of control.
REFERENCES
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of
behavioral change. Psy-
chological Review, 84, 191–215.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A
social cognitive theory.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., & Tice, D. M. (1994).
Losing control: How and why
people fail at self-regulation. San Diego: Academic Press.
Beswick, G., Rothblum, E., & Mann, L. (1988). Psychological
antecedents to student pro-
crastination. Australian Psychologists, 23, 207–217.
Blunt, A., & Pychyl, T. A. (1998). Volitional action and
inaction in the lives of under-
graduate students: State orientation, procrastination and
proneness to boredom. Per-
sonality Individual Difference, 24, 837–846.
Bond, M. J., & Feather, N. T. (1988). Some correlates of
structure and purpose in the use
of time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 321–
329.
Brownlow, S., & Reasinger, R. D. (2000). Putting off until
tomorrow what is better done
today: Academic procrastination as a function of motivation
toward college work. Jour-
nal of Social Behavior and Personality, 15, 15–34.
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989).
Assessing coping strategies: A
262 The Journal of Social Psychology
theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 56,
267–283.
Conti, R. (2000). Competing demands and complimentary
motives: Procrastination on
intrinsically and extrinsically motivated summer projects.
Journal of Social Behavior
and Personality, 15, 47–59.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and
self-determination in human
behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
Dipboye, R. L., & Phillips, A. P. (1990). College students’ time
management: Correlations
with academic performance and stress. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 82,
760–768.
Ellis, A., & Knaus, W. J. (1977). Overcoming procrastination.
New York: Signet.
Endler, N. S., & Parker, J. D. A. (1990). Multidimensional
assessment of coping: A criti-
cal evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
58, 844–854.
Endler, N. S., & Parker, J. D. A. (1994). Assessment of
multidimensional coping: Task,
emotion, and avoidance strategies. Psychological Assessment, 6,
50–60.
Fee, R. L., & Tangney, J. P. (2000). Procrastination: A means of
avoiding shame or guilt?
Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 15, 167–184.
Ferrari, J. R. (2001). Procrastination as self-regulation failure
of performance: Effects of
cognitive load, self-awareness, and time limits on “working best
under pressure.” Euro-
pean Journal of Personality, 15, 391–406.
Ferrari, J. R., Johnson, J. L., & McCown, W. G. (1995).
Procrastination and task avoid-
ance: Theory, research, and treatment. New York: Plenum Press.
Ferrari, J. R., Parker, J. T., & Ware, C. B. (1992). Academic
procrastination: Personality
correlates with Myers-Briggs types, self-efficacy, and academic
locus of control. Jour-
nal of Social Behavior and Personality, 7, 595–602.
Ferrari, J. R., & Tice, D. M. (2000). Procrastination as a self-
handicap for men and women:
A task-avoidance strategy in a laboratory setting. Journal of
Research in Psychology,
34, 73–83.
Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1980). An analysis of coping in
a middle-aged communi-
ty sample. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 21, 219–239.
Greenglass, E., Schwarzer, R., & Taubert, S. (1999). The
Proactive Coping Inventory
(PCI): A multidimensional research instrument. Retrieved
February 17, 2003, from
http://userpage.fuberlin.de/~health/greenpci.htm
Harriott, J., & Ferrari, J. (1996). Prevalence of procrastination
among samples of adults.
Psychological Reports, 78, 611–616.
Knaus, W. J. (2000). Procrastination, blame, and change.
Journal of Social Behavior and
Personality, 15, 153–166.
Kosic, A. (2004). Acculturation strategies, coping process and
acculturative stress. Scan-
dinavian Journal of Psychology, 45, 269–278.
Latack, J. C., & Havlovic, S. J. (1992). Coping with job stress:
A conceptual evaluation
framework for coping measures. Journal of Organizational
Behavior, 13, 479–508.
Lay, C. H. (1990). Working to schedule on personal projects:
An assessment of person-
object characteristics and trait procrastination. Journal of Social
Behavior and Person-
ality, 5, 91–104.
Lay, C. H., & Schouwenburg, H. C. (1993). Trait
procrastination, time management, and
academic behavior. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality,
8, 647–662.
Macan, T. H. (1994). Time management: Test of a process
model. Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 79, 381–391.
Macan, T. H., Shahani, C., Dipboye, R. L., & Phillips, A. P.
(1990). College students’ time
management: Correlations with academic performance and
stress. Journal of Education
Psychology, 82, 760–768.
Chu & Choi 263
MacArthur, J. D., & MacArthur, C. T. (2001). Perceived Stress
Scale. Retrieved February
18, 2003, from Department of Psychology, University of
California, San Francisco, Web
site:
http://www.macses.ucsf.edu/Research/Psychosocial/notebook/P
SS10.html
Owens, A., & Newbegin, I. (1997). Procrastination in high
school achievement: A causal
structural model. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality,
12, 869–887.
Podsakoff, P. M., & Organ, D. W. (1986). Self-reports in
organizational research: Prob-
lems and prospects. Journal of Management, 12, 531–544.
Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1991). Essentials of behavioral
research: Methods and
data analysis (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1995). Generalized Self-
Efficacy Scale. In J. Weinman,
S. Wright, & M. Johnston, Measures in health psychology: A
user’s portfolio: Causal
and control beliefs (pp. 35–37). Windsor, England: NFER-
Nelson.
Sheikh, J. L., & Yesavage, J. A. (1982). Geriatric Depression
Scale (GDS). Retrieved Feb-
ruary 18, 2003, from Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral
Sciences, Stanford Uni-
versity, Web site:
http://www.stanford.edu/~yesavage/GDS.english.long.html
Shia, R. M. (1998). Assessing academic intrinsic motivation: A
look at student goals and
personal strategy. Unpublished college thesis, Wheeling Jesuit
University, Wheeling,
WV. Retrieved February 17, 2003, from
http://www.cet.edu/research/papers/motiva-
tion/motivation.pdf
Steel, P., Brothen, T., & Wambach, C. (2001). Procrastination
and personality perfor-
mance, and mood. Personality and Individual Differences, 30,
95–106.
Tice, D., & Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Longitudinal study of
procrastination, performance,
stress, and health: The cost and benefits of dawdling.
Psychological Science, 8,
454–458.
Tuckman, B. W. (1991). The development and concurrent
validity of the procrastination
scale. Education and Psychological Measurement, 51, 473–480.
Vodanovich, S. J., & Seib, H. M. (1997). Relationship between
time structure and pro-
crastination. Psychological Reports, 80, 211–215.
Received December 29, 2003
Accepted January 25, 2005
264 The Journal of Social Psychology
REQUIREMENTSAs outlined in the Syllabus on Blackboard .docx

Más contenido relacionado

Similar a REQUIREMENTSAs outlined in the Syllabus on Blackboard .docx

University of Guelph, Learning Commons Library (httpwww.lib.uog.docx
University of Guelph, Learning Commons Library (httpwww.lib.uog.docxUniversity of Guelph, Learning Commons Library (httpwww.lib.uog.docx
University of Guelph, Learning Commons Library (httpwww.lib.uog.docxdickonsondorris
 
Undertaking a literature review
Undertaking a literature reviewUndertaking a literature review
Undertaking a literature reviewJennifer Evans
 
Workshop Slides on Research Proposal and Procedure 190415
Workshop Slides on Research Proposal and Procedure 190415Workshop Slides on Research Proposal and Procedure 190415
Workshop Slides on Research Proposal and Procedure 190415Hiram Ting
 
Writing Your Thesis/Disseration
Writing Your Thesis/DisserationWriting Your Thesis/Disseration
Writing Your Thesis/DisserationDr. Niall McElwee
 
Research in Social Psychology [WLOs 1, 3, 4, 5, 6] [CLOs 1, 2, 3.docx
Research in Social Psychology [WLOs 1, 3, 4, 5, 6] [CLOs 1, 2, 3.docxResearch in Social Psychology [WLOs 1, 3, 4, 5, 6] [CLOs 1, 2, 3.docx
Research in Social Psychology [WLOs 1, 3, 4, 5, 6] [CLOs 1, 2, 3.docxverad6
 
How to-prepare-for-journal-club2016
How to-prepare-for-journal-club2016How to-prepare-for-journal-club2016
How to-prepare-for-journal-club2016Ramin Nazari M.D
 
How To Survive Peer Review
How To Survive Peer ReviewHow To Survive Peer Review
How To Survive Peer Reviewmsaghaei
 
WRITING AN ACTION RESEARCH DISSERTATION PART TWOINTRODU.docx
WRITING AN ACTION RESEARCH DISSERTATION PART TWOINTRODU.docxWRITING AN ACTION RESEARCH DISSERTATION PART TWOINTRODU.docx
WRITING AN ACTION RESEARCH DISSERTATION PART TWOINTRODU.docxambersalomon88660
 
Article Manuscript Structure.pdf
Article Manuscript Structure.pdfArticle Manuscript Structure.pdf
Article Manuscript Structure.pdftamrat11
 
Writing A Health Research Proposal
Writing A Health Research ProposalWriting A Health Research Proposal
Writing A Health Research ProposalSoha Rashed
 
Running head 11He says here goes the title .docx
Running head  11He says here goes the title .docxRunning head  11He says here goes the title .docx
Running head 11He says here goes the title .docxtoddr4
 
Academic literature review
Academic literature review Academic literature review
Academic literature review Harris Abd Hamid
 
Critical Qualitative Research Designpages 70–76Related to un.docx
Critical Qualitative Research Designpages 70–76Related to un.docxCritical Qualitative Research Designpages 70–76Related to un.docx
Critical Qualitative Research Designpages 70–76Related to un.docxwillcoxjanay
 
MODULE 6 IN PRactical research 1 for junior high
MODULE 6 IN PRactical research 1 for junior highMODULE 6 IN PRactical research 1 for junior high
MODULE 6 IN PRactical research 1 for junior highdiegogalit1986
 
Chapter 1 Introsduction
Chapter 1 IntrosductionChapter 1 Introsduction
Chapter 1 IntrosductionEric Aguilar
 
Running head RESEARCH TYPES .docx
Running head RESEARCH TYPES                                  .docxRunning head RESEARCH TYPES                                  .docx
Running head RESEARCH TYPES .docxtoltonkendal
 
Research Design Planning
Research Design PlanningResearch Design Planning
Research Design PlanningND Arisanti
 
Enjoy writing your dissertation
Enjoy writing your dissertationEnjoy writing your dissertation
Enjoy writing your dissertationBoutkhil Guemide
 

Similar a REQUIREMENTSAs outlined in the Syllabus on Blackboard .docx (20)

University of Guelph, Learning Commons Library (httpwww.lib.uog.docx
University of Guelph, Learning Commons Library (httpwww.lib.uog.docxUniversity of Guelph, Learning Commons Library (httpwww.lib.uog.docx
University of Guelph, Learning Commons Library (httpwww.lib.uog.docx
 
Undertaking a literature review
Undertaking a literature reviewUndertaking a literature review
Undertaking a literature review
 
Workshop Slides on Research Proposal and Procedure 190415
Workshop Slides on Research Proposal and Procedure 190415Workshop Slides on Research Proposal and Procedure 190415
Workshop Slides on Research Proposal and Procedure 190415
 
Writing Your Thesis/Disseration
Writing Your Thesis/DisserationWriting Your Thesis/Disseration
Writing Your Thesis/Disseration
 
Research in Social Psychology [WLOs 1, 3, 4, 5, 6] [CLOs 1, 2, 3.docx
Research in Social Psychology [WLOs 1, 3, 4, 5, 6] [CLOs 1, 2, 3.docxResearch in Social Psychology [WLOs 1, 3, 4, 5, 6] [CLOs 1, 2, 3.docx
Research in Social Psychology [WLOs 1, 3, 4, 5, 6] [CLOs 1, 2, 3.docx
 
How to-prepare-for-journal-club2016
How to-prepare-for-journal-club2016How to-prepare-for-journal-club2016
How to-prepare-for-journal-club2016
 
How To Survive Peer Review
How To Survive Peer ReviewHow To Survive Peer Review
How To Survive Peer Review
 
WRITING AN ACTION RESEARCH DISSERTATION PART TWOINTRODU.docx
WRITING AN ACTION RESEARCH DISSERTATION PART TWOINTRODU.docxWRITING AN ACTION RESEARCH DISSERTATION PART TWOINTRODU.docx
WRITING AN ACTION RESEARCH DISSERTATION PART TWOINTRODU.docx
 
Article Manuscript Structure.pdf
Article Manuscript Structure.pdfArticle Manuscript Structure.pdf
Article Manuscript Structure.pdf
 
Writing A Health Research Proposal
Writing A Health Research ProposalWriting A Health Research Proposal
Writing A Health Research Proposal
 
Running head 11He says here goes the title .docx
Running head  11He says here goes the title .docxRunning head  11He says here goes the title .docx
Running head 11He says here goes the title .docx
 
Academic literature review
Academic literature review Academic literature review
Academic literature review
 
RSM2.pptx
RSM2.pptxRSM2.pptx
RSM2.pptx
 
Critical Qualitative Research Designpages 70–76Related to un.docx
Critical Qualitative Research Designpages 70–76Related to un.docxCritical Qualitative Research Designpages 70–76Related to un.docx
Critical Qualitative Research Designpages 70–76Related to un.docx
 
MODULE 6 IN PRactical research 1 for junior high
MODULE 6 IN PRactical research 1 for junior highMODULE 6 IN PRactical research 1 for junior high
MODULE 6 IN PRactical research 1 for junior high
 
Chapter 1 Introsduction
Chapter 1 IntrosductionChapter 1 Introsduction
Chapter 1 Introsduction
 
Chapter 1
Chapter 1Chapter 1
Chapter 1
 
Running head RESEARCH TYPES .docx
Running head RESEARCH TYPES                                  .docxRunning head RESEARCH TYPES                                  .docx
Running head RESEARCH TYPES .docx
 
Research Design Planning
Research Design PlanningResearch Design Planning
Research Design Planning
 
Enjoy writing your dissertation
Enjoy writing your dissertationEnjoy writing your dissertation
Enjoy writing your dissertation
 

Más de heunice

RequirementsMicrosoft Project is the recommended software tool f.docx
RequirementsMicrosoft Project is the recommended software tool f.docxRequirementsMicrosoft Project is the recommended software tool f.docx
RequirementsMicrosoft Project is the recommended software tool f.docxheunice
 
REQUIREMENTSPart 1 5-page Research paper consisting of 6 parts .docx
REQUIREMENTSPart 1 5-page Research paper consisting of 6 parts .docxREQUIREMENTSPart 1 5-page Research paper consisting of 6 parts .docx
REQUIREMENTSPart 1 5-page Research paper consisting of 6 parts .docxheunice
 
RequirementsOutlineIntended AudienceIdentif.docx
RequirementsOutlineIntended AudienceIdentif.docxRequirementsOutlineIntended AudienceIdentif.docx
RequirementsOutlineIntended AudienceIdentif.docxheunice
 
REQUIREMENTSIntroductionIntroduction includesthe.docx
REQUIREMENTSIntroductionIntroduction includesthe.docxREQUIREMENTSIntroductionIntroduction includesthe.docx
REQUIREMENTSIntroductionIntroduction includesthe.docxheunice
 
requirementslength-3pages longfont- 10or11space- double spacin.docx
requirementslength-3pages longfont- 10or11space- double spacin.docxrequirementslength-3pages longfont- 10or11space- double spacin.docx
requirementslength-3pages longfont- 10or11space- double spacin.docxheunice
 
REQUIREMENTSIntroductionIntroduction includesthe· P.docx
REQUIREMENTSIntroductionIntroduction includesthe· P.docxREQUIREMENTSIntroductionIntroduction includesthe· P.docx
REQUIREMENTSIntroductionIntroduction includesthe· P.docxheunice
 
RequirementsDevelop a formal Teaching Plan for a target audi.docx
RequirementsDevelop a formal Teaching Plan for a target audi.docxRequirementsDevelop a formal Teaching Plan for a target audi.docx
RequirementsDevelop a formal Teaching Plan for a target audi.docxheunice
 
RequirementsIf the student cannot attend the Intensive review, .docx
RequirementsIf the student cannot attend the Intensive review, .docxRequirementsIf the student cannot attend the Intensive review, .docx
RequirementsIf the student cannot attend the Intensive review, .docxheunice
 
RequirementsContent Craft an e-mail message that.docx
RequirementsContent    Craft an e-mail message that.docxRequirementsContent    Craft an e-mail message that.docx
RequirementsContent Craft an e-mail message that.docxheunice
 
REQUIREMENTSexternal environmentA. Natural environment.docx
REQUIREMENTSexternal environmentA. Natural environment.docxREQUIREMENTSexternal environmentA. Natural environment.docx
REQUIREMENTSexternal environmentA. Natural environment.docxheunice
 
Requirements1. Using a patient seen in their clinical setting, .docx
Requirements1. Using a patient seen in their clinical setting, .docxRequirements1. Using a patient seen in their clinical setting, .docx
Requirements1. Using a patient seen in their clinical setting, .docxheunice
 
RequirementsAll writing must be appropriate for an academi.docx
RequirementsAll writing must be appropriate for an academi.docxRequirementsAll writing must be appropriate for an academi.docx
RequirementsAll writing must be appropriate for an academi.docxheunice
 
RequirementsAllow users to login to the siteAllow users t.docx
RequirementsAllow users to login to the siteAllow users t.docxRequirementsAllow users to login to the siteAllow users t.docx
RequirementsAllow users to login to the siteAllow users t.docxheunice
 
RequirementsAfter completion of the Nurse Executive CGE Proj.docx
RequirementsAfter completion of the Nurse Executive CGE Proj.docxRequirementsAfter completion of the Nurse Executive CGE Proj.docx
RequirementsAfter completion of the Nurse Executive CGE Proj.docxheunice
 
Requirements1.Length of paper – 2 pages of content (excl.docx
Requirements1.Length of paper – 2 pages of content (excl.docxRequirements1.Length of paper – 2 pages of content (excl.docx
Requirements1.Length of paper – 2 pages of content (excl.docxheunice
 
Requirements1) Due 6 Oct2) APA 6th Ed format3) 6 Pages.docx
Requirements1) Due 6 Oct2) APA 6th Ed format3) 6 Pages.docxRequirements1) Due 6 Oct2) APA 6th Ed format3) 6 Pages.docx
Requirements1) Due 6 Oct2) APA 6th Ed format3) 6 Pages.docxheunice
 
Requirements1) Length of the paper 4-5 pages, double space.docx
Requirements1) Length of the paper 4-5 pages, double space.docxRequirements1) Length of the paper 4-5 pages, double space.docx
Requirements1) Length of the paper 4-5 pages, double space.docxheunice
 
Requirements- Combining several essays into 1 research paper.- .docx
Requirements- Combining several essays into 1 research paper.- .docxRequirements- Combining several essays into 1 research paper.- .docx
Requirements- Combining several essays into 1 research paper.- .docxheunice
 
Requirements of this assignment1) Watch a TED Talk. or an.docx
Requirements of this assignment1) Watch a TED Talk. or an.docxRequirements of this assignment1) Watch a TED Talk. or an.docx
Requirements of this assignment1) Watch a TED Talk. or an.docxheunice
 
Requirements Write a three to five-sentences caption for the fo.docx
Requirements Write a three to five-sentences caption for the fo.docxRequirements Write a three to five-sentences caption for the fo.docx
Requirements Write a three to five-sentences caption for the fo.docxheunice
 

Más de heunice (20)

RequirementsMicrosoft Project is the recommended software tool f.docx
RequirementsMicrosoft Project is the recommended software tool f.docxRequirementsMicrosoft Project is the recommended software tool f.docx
RequirementsMicrosoft Project is the recommended software tool f.docx
 
REQUIREMENTSPart 1 5-page Research paper consisting of 6 parts .docx
REQUIREMENTSPart 1 5-page Research paper consisting of 6 parts .docxREQUIREMENTSPart 1 5-page Research paper consisting of 6 parts .docx
REQUIREMENTSPart 1 5-page Research paper consisting of 6 parts .docx
 
RequirementsOutlineIntended AudienceIdentif.docx
RequirementsOutlineIntended AudienceIdentif.docxRequirementsOutlineIntended AudienceIdentif.docx
RequirementsOutlineIntended AudienceIdentif.docx
 
REQUIREMENTSIntroductionIntroduction includesthe.docx
REQUIREMENTSIntroductionIntroduction includesthe.docxREQUIREMENTSIntroductionIntroduction includesthe.docx
REQUIREMENTSIntroductionIntroduction includesthe.docx
 
requirementslength-3pages longfont- 10or11space- double spacin.docx
requirementslength-3pages longfont- 10or11space- double spacin.docxrequirementslength-3pages longfont- 10or11space- double spacin.docx
requirementslength-3pages longfont- 10or11space- double spacin.docx
 
REQUIREMENTSIntroductionIntroduction includesthe· P.docx
REQUIREMENTSIntroductionIntroduction includesthe· P.docxREQUIREMENTSIntroductionIntroduction includesthe· P.docx
REQUIREMENTSIntroductionIntroduction includesthe· P.docx
 
RequirementsDevelop a formal Teaching Plan for a target audi.docx
RequirementsDevelop a formal Teaching Plan for a target audi.docxRequirementsDevelop a formal Teaching Plan for a target audi.docx
RequirementsDevelop a formal Teaching Plan for a target audi.docx
 
RequirementsIf the student cannot attend the Intensive review, .docx
RequirementsIf the student cannot attend the Intensive review, .docxRequirementsIf the student cannot attend the Intensive review, .docx
RequirementsIf the student cannot attend the Intensive review, .docx
 
RequirementsContent Craft an e-mail message that.docx
RequirementsContent    Craft an e-mail message that.docxRequirementsContent    Craft an e-mail message that.docx
RequirementsContent Craft an e-mail message that.docx
 
REQUIREMENTSexternal environmentA. Natural environment.docx
REQUIREMENTSexternal environmentA. Natural environment.docxREQUIREMENTSexternal environmentA. Natural environment.docx
REQUIREMENTSexternal environmentA. Natural environment.docx
 
Requirements1. Using a patient seen in their clinical setting, .docx
Requirements1. Using a patient seen in their clinical setting, .docxRequirements1. Using a patient seen in their clinical setting, .docx
Requirements1. Using a patient seen in their clinical setting, .docx
 
RequirementsAll writing must be appropriate for an academi.docx
RequirementsAll writing must be appropriate for an academi.docxRequirementsAll writing must be appropriate for an academi.docx
RequirementsAll writing must be appropriate for an academi.docx
 
RequirementsAllow users to login to the siteAllow users t.docx
RequirementsAllow users to login to the siteAllow users t.docxRequirementsAllow users to login to the siteAllow users t.docx
RequirementsAllow users to login to the siteAllow users t.docx
 
RequirementsAfter completion of the Nurse Executive CGE Proj.docx
RequirementsAfter completion of the Nurse Executive CGE Proj.docxRequirementsAfter completion of the Nurse Executive CGE Proj.docx
RequirementsAfter completion of the Nurse Executive CGE Proj.docx
 
Requirements1.Length of paper – 2 pages of content (excl.docx
Requirements1.Length of paper – 2 pages of content (excl.docxRequirements1.Length of paper – 2 pages of content (excl.docx
Requirements1.Length of paper – 2 pages of content (excl.docx
 
Requirements1) Due 6 Oct2) APA 6th Ed format3) 6 Pages.docx
Requirements1) Due 6 Oct2) APA 6th Ed format3) 6 Pages.docxRequirements1) Due 6 Oct2) APA 6th Ed format3) 6 Pages.docx
Requirements1) Due 6 Oct2) APA 6th Ed format3) 6 Pages.docx
 
Requirements1) Length of the paper 4-5 pages, double space.docx
Requirements1) Length of the paper 4-5 pages, double space.docxRequirements1) Length of the paper 4-5 pages, double space.docx
Requirements1) Length of the paper 4-5 pages, double space.docx
 
Requirements- Combining several essays into 1 research paper.- .docx
Requirements- Combining several essays into 1 research paper.- .docxRequirements- Combining several essays into 1 research paper.- .docx
Requirements- Combining several essays into 1 research paper.- .docx
 
Requirements of this assignment1) Watch a TED Talk. or an.docx
Requirements of this assignment1) Watch a TED Talk. or an.docxRequirements of this assignment1) Watch a TED Talk. or an.docx
Requirements of this assignment1) Watch a TED Talk. or an.docx
 
Requirements Write a three to five-sentences caption for the fo.docx
Requirements Write a three to five-sentences caption for the fo.docxRequirements Write a three to five-sentences caption for the fo.docx
Requirements Write a three to five-sentences caption for the fo.docx
 

Último

Science 7 Quarter 4 Module 2: Natural Resources.pptx
Science 7 Quarter 4 Module 2: Natural Resources.pptxScience 7 Quarter 4 Module 2: Natural Resources.pptx
Science 7 Quarter 4 Module 2: Natural Resources.pptxMaryGraceBautista27
 
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTS
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTSGRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTS
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTSJoshuaGantuangco2
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxthorishapillay1
 
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)lakshayb543
 
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdfACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdfSpandanaRallapalli
 
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptxmary850239
 
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designKeynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designMIPLM
 
Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)
Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)
Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)cama23
 
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptxmary850239
 
Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdf
Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdfGrade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdf
Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdfJemuel Francisco
 
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITYISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITYKayeClaireEstoconing
 
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptxJudging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptxSherlyMaeNeri
 
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for ParentsChoosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parentsnavabharathschool99
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...Jisc
 
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxMULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxAnupkumar Sharma
 
INTRODUCTION TO CATHOLIC CHRISTOLOGY.pptx
INTRODUCTION TO CATHOLIC CHRISTOLOGY.pptxINTRODUCTION TO CATHOLIC CHRISTOLOGY.pptx
INTRODUCTION TO CATHOLIC CHRISTOLOGY.pptxHumphrey A Beña
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
FILIPINO PSYCHology sikolohiyang pilipino
FILIPINO PSYCHology sikolohiyang pilipinoFILIPINO PSYCHology sikolohiyang pilipino
FILIPINO PSYCHology sikolohiyang pilipinojohnmickonozaleda
 

Último (20)

Science 7 Quarter 4 Module 2: Natural Resources.pptx
Science 7 Quarter 4 Module 2: Natural Resources.pptxScience 7 Quarter 4 Module 2: Natural Resources.pptx
Science 7 Quarter 4 Module 2: Natural Resources.pptx
 
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTS
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTSGRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTS
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTS
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
 
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
 
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdfACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
 
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
 
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designKeynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
 
Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)
Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)
Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)
 
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx
 
Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdf
Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdfGrade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdf
Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdf
 
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITYISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
 
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptxJudging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
 
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for ParentsChoosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
 
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
 
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxMULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
 
INTRODUCTION TO CATHOLIC CHRISTOLOGY.pptx
INTRODUCTION TO CATHOLIC CHRISTOLOGY.pptxINTRODUCTION TO CATHOLIC CHRISTOLOGY.pptx
INTRODUCTION TO CATHOLIC CHRISTOLOGY.pptx
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
 
FILIPINO PSYCHology sikolohiyang pilipino
FILIPINO PSYCHology sikolohiyang pilipinoFILIPINO PSYCHology sikolohiyang pilipino
FILIPINO PSYCHology sikolohiyang pilipino
 

REQUIREMENTSAs outlined in the Syllabus on Blackboard .docx

  • 1. REQUIREMENTS As outlined in the Syllabus on Blackboard: Article Review (80 points). Article review papers must include a brief summary of the study, explanation of the results, and your opinion of the conclusions. In addition, the paper should include relevant course material and a critique of the article’s statistics when discussing the results and conclusions. It should be between 2 to 3 pages in length. Prior to completing an article review you MUST get the article approved by Dr. Mohler. Goal: to demonstrate knowledge of statistical concepts discussed in class and that you can apply these to research. REQUIREMENTS Must use APA format � Should include a title and references page. � You only need to reference your article. You DO NOT need to find alternative sources. � Any idea that is not your own should be cited within the body of the paper. � Size 12 font, Times of New Roman font, double spaced.
  • 2. � Given the length of your paper being short, you DO NOT need to include headers, footers, graphs, or an abstract within the body of the paper. Rather, I just want to you get comfortable making a title page, using running heads, citing sources, and referencing your article APA style. Resources � https://owl.purdue.edu/owl/research_and_citation/apa_style/apa _formatting_and_style_guide/reference_list_electronic_source s.html � http://my.ilstu.edu/~jhkahn/apastats.html � http://info.csp.edu/globalassets/academic-resources/writing- center/docs/apa-sample-paper-6th-edition.pdf. � Helpful Video: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=9pbUoNa5tyY � No Plagiarism! This will result in an automatic score of 0 for the assignment. � 2-3 pages in length (NOT including your title and reference pages) � If it is less than 2 pages, it will be reflected in your grade https://owl.purdue.edu/owl/research_and_citation/apa_style/apa _formatting_and_style_guide/reference_list_electronic_sources. html http://my.ilstu.edu/~jhkahn/apastats.html http://info.csp.edu/globalassets/academic-resources/writing- center/docs/apa-sample-paper-6th-edition.pdf https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=9pbUoNa5tyY
  • 3. TYPES OF ARTICLES TO CHOOSE Can be on any topic of interest! Needs to be a peer reviewed/academic journal article. � Library journal (online accessible) � Search Psychology or PsychINFO databases in UALR Library � Search “Psychology” related journal articles & limit results to be peer reviewed journals � Google Scholar Look for a study, NOT a literature review or a news article An appropriate article will typically have the following categories: ❖Literature review ❖Rationale for the current study/Hypotheses ❖Methods/Participants ❖Statistical Analyses ❖Results ❖Discussion THOUGHTS TO CONSIDER: This is a time to apply and demonstrate your critical thinking skills! DO NOT WRITE ABOUT � If you agree/disagree with the outcome of the study. � A long summary of the article and results (brief only) Rather, write about: � Concepts discussed in class
  • 4. THINGS TO POTENTIALLY WRITE ABOUT: � Who is part of the sample? Can the conclusions be generalized to the entire population? Think population and sample. � Did they use adequate sampling (e.g., random sampling vs. convenience sampling?) � What are the variables they used? How do they operationally define them? � What type of data was used (e.g., nominal, ordinal, interval, ratio OR continuous, discrete). � Identify the IV and DV and describe how they used these in the study. � Did they display the data adequately? Why or why not? � What types of graphs did they use? � Does the data adequately support the conclusion drawn by the researcher(s)? � Measures of central tendency used within the article. � Discuss concepts related to Variance. � Discuss distributions and probabilities and how they were used within the article. � Did they identify a hypotheses and undergo hypotheses testing? � Discuss Type I and Type II Error as related to your article. � Any other concepts discussed in class that may be relevant to your article! � It is okay if you cannot write about everything (e.g., statistical analyses used or all the concept outlined above). Write about what you have learned already and what is relevant to your article.
  • 5. TIMELINE 3/6/19: Last day to have Dr. Mohler Approve article � Email me the article you are wanting to use and I will approve or let you know my concerns. Let me know if you need assistance. I’d be happy to assist you in locating an article before or after class. Please email me to set up a time to do this. Come with some topics of interest. 4/15/19 by 7:30pm: Article Review Paper due � You will upload your paper AND the chosen article you wrote about on Blackboard. � Your paper will NOT be accepted and graded without your article attached to Blackboard. QUESTIONS ABOUT THE ARTICLE REVIEW PAPER? 245 The Journal of Social Psychology, 2005, 145(3), 245–264 Rethinking Procrastination: Positive Effects of “Active” Procrastination Behavior on Attitudes and Performance
  • 6. ANGELA HSIN CHUN CHU Department of Organizational Psychology Columbia University New York, New York JIN NAM CHOI Faculty of Management McGill University Montreal, Quebec, Canada ABSTRACT. Researchers and practitioners have long regarded procrastination as a self- handicapping and dysfunctional behavior. In the present study, the authors proposed that not all procrastination behaviors either are harmful or lead to negative consequences. Specifically, the authors differentiated two types of procrastinators: passive procrastinators versus active procrastinators. Passive procrastinators are procrastinators in the traditional sense. They are paralyzed by their indecision to act and fail to complete tasks on time. In contrast, active procrastinators are a “positive” type of procrastinator. They prefer to work under pressure, and they make deliberate decisions to procrastinate. The present results showed that although active procrastinators procrastinate to the same degree as passive pro- crastinators, they are more similar to nonprocrastinators than to passive procrastinators in terms of purposive use of time, control of time, self-efficacy belief, coping styles, and out- comes including academic performance. The present findings offer a more sophisticated
  • 7. understanding of procrastination behavior and indicate a need to reevaluate its implications for outcomes of individuals. Key words: coping strategies, procrastination, self-efficacy, time management INVESTIGATORS HAVE DEFINED PROCRASTINATION as the lack or absence of self-regulated performance and the behavioral tendency to postpone what is necessary to reach a goal (Ellis & Knaus, 1977; Knaus, 2000). The grow- ing body of literature has demonstrated that procrastination is not just a problem of time management. It is a complex process that involves affective, cognitive, and behavioral components (Fee & Tangney, 2000). Blunt and Pychyl (1998) and Harriott and Ferrari (1996) have found procrastination to be a prevalent phe- nomenon in the general population, chronically affecting a substantial portion of adults as well as university students. Procrastination may have particularly seri- ous consequences for university students, whose lives are characterized by fre- quent deadlines. For example, Tice and Baumeister (1997) reported that univer- sity students who rated high on procrastination not only received low grades but also reported a high level of stress along with poor self-rated health.
  • 8. Most of the existing literature on procrastination has contrasted procrasti- nators with nonprocrastinators. Procrastination has been considered a self- handicapping behavior that leads to wasted time, poor performance, and increased stress. Investigators such as Ferrari (2001) have often depicted pro- crastinators as lazy or self-indulgent individuals who are unable to self-regu- late. In contrast, nonprocrastination has been associated with high efficiency, productivity, and superior performance, and nonprocrastinators are often described as organized and highly motivated individuals (e.g., Bond & Feath- er, 1988; Ellis & Knaus, 1977). Although both the practical literature and the academic literature have asso- ciated negative connotations to procrastination, investigators have found that pro- crastination can induce some short-term benefits. For example, Tice and Baumeister (1997) reported that compared with nonprocrastinators, procrastina- tors experience less stress and have better physical health when deadlines are far off. In this sense, procrastination can be seen as a strategy that they use to regu- late negative emotions, thereby making the individual feel better, at least tem- porarily (Baumeister, Heatherton, & Tice, 1994). Moreover, in principle, whether a person does a task far ahead of a deadline or only slightly
  • 9. ahead of it does not necessarily affect the quality of the work (Tice & Baumeister). Therefore, prac- tically speaking, procrastination does not necessarily have a negative impact on the effectiveness of the task performance. In a similar vein, Knaus (2000) argued that not all delays lead to negative out- comes. For example, delays resulting from time that was spent planning and gath- ering vital preparatory information can be beneficial (Knaus). Many people claim that even when they start to work at the last minute, they can still finish on time and that they tend to work better and faster or generate more creative ideas under time pressure. This line of thought on procrastination suggests that there might be more than one kind of procrastinator and that in some cases procrastination behav- ior might lead to positive outcomes. In the present study, we identify two types of procrastinators and examine whether they have distinct characteristics in terms of the use and perception of time, their self-efficacy beliefs, their motivational ori- entation, their stress-coping strategies, and their personal outcomes. 246 The Journal of Social Psychology Address correspondence to Jin Nam Choi, McGill University, Faculty of Management, 1001 Sherbrooke Street West, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3A 1G5; [email protected]
  • 10. mcgill.ca (e-mail). Passive and Active Procrastination Addressing the possibility that not all procrastination behaviors have nega- tive effects, we conceptually distinguished two different types of procrastinators: passive versus active procrastinators. Passive procrastinators are procrastinators in the traditional sense. Cognitively, passive procrastinators do not intend to pro- crastinate, but they often end up postponing tasks because of their inability to make decisions quickly and to thereby act on them quickly. Active procrastina- tors, in contrast, are capable of acting on their decisions in a timely manner. How- ever, they suspend their actions deliberately and focus their attention on other important tasks at hand. Therefore, passive procrastinators differ from active pro- crastinators on cognitive, affective, and behavioral dimensions. Affectively, when a deadline approaches, passive procrastinators feel pres- sured and become pessimistic in their outlook, especially about their ability to achieve satisfactory results (Ferrari, Parker, & Ware, 1992). Their thoughts of self-doubt and inadequacy increase the chance of failure and induce feelings of guilt and depression (Steel, Brothen, & Wambach, 2001). Active procrastinators,
  • 11. on the other hand, like to work under pressure. When faced with last-minute tasks, they feel challenged and motivated, and that feeling immunizes them against the kind of suffering common in passive procrastinators. Different cog- nitive pathways and affective responses interact to produce different behavioral patterns: Active procrastinators are persistent and able to complete tasks at the last minute. Passive procrastinators, on the other hand, are more likely to give up and fail to complete tasks. On the basis of the above circumstances, we proposed that active pro- crastination is a multifaceted phenomenon that includes cognitive (decision to procrastinate), affective (preference for time pressure), and behavioral (task completion by the deadline) components as well as the physical results and satisfaction with them. Because of these fundamental differences (cog- nitive, affective, and behavioral), we expected active and passive procrasti- nators to have distinct psychological characteristics and to achieve different outcomes. Time Use and Perception One of the most common recommendations for effective management of time is to properly structure one’s use of time in combination with a clear sense
  • 12. of purpose. For example, Dipboye and Phillips (1990) showed that university stu- dents who had more sense of purpose and structure in their use of time reported greater psychological well-being and more efficient habits of study. Also, Vodanovich and Seib (1997) illustrated that procrastinators tend to have weak, if any, structure in their time use. Therefore, we expected nonprocrastinators, who tend to plan their activities on a day-to-day basis, to have time use that is more Chu & Choi 247 structured and to perceive their use of time to be more purposive. Traditional (pas- sive) procrastinators, who incorporate less structure in their time use, may drift aimlessly from one activity to another (Bond & Feather, 1988). Active procrasti- nators, in contrast, are expected to be different from passive procrastinators in having more time structure and a better sense of purpose in their time use because they are able to make deliberate decisions regarding their time use on the basis of urgency or priority. In this sense, active procrastinators are similar to nonpro- crastinators in terms of engagement in time-structuring behaviors and purposive use of time. Despite the fact that we can measure time, our perception of
  • 13. time is a truly subjective experience (Macan, 1994). Lay (1990) found passive procrastinators to underestimate the overall time that was required to complete tasks. Conse- quently, they often failed to complete tasks on time, triggering the perception of reduced control of time (Lay & Schouwenburg, 1993). In contrast, nonprocrasti- nators, who constantly engage in planning and organizing, tend to have more real- istic perceptions of time and to perceive more control over their time (Macan). Active procrastinators may be similar to nonprocrastinators in that they take charge of their time and try to maximize the efficiency of their time use. Conse- quently, they would develop an awareness of their time use and a perception of time control. Hypothesis 1: Both nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators will report more time structure, more purposive use of time, and greater perception of control of time than will passive procrastinators. Nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators are not different on these dimensions. Self-Efficacy The association between self-efficacy and procrastination was first introduced by Bandura (1986). Self-efficacy refers to the belief that one can reliably perform the tasks that are required for successful goal achievement
  • 14. (Bandura, 1977). Bandura (1986) hypothesized that when adequate levels of ability and motivation exist, self- efficacy belief will affect a person’s task initiation and persistence. Several investi- gators have examined the relationship between self-efficacy and procrastination. For example, Ferrari et al. (1992) and Tuckman (1991) have found an inverse relation- ship between self-efficacy belief and academic procrastination among college stu- dents. In line with the belief that self-efficacy plays a role in task initiation and task persistence, in the present study we expected that nonprocrastinators will have a stronger self-efficacy belief than passive procrastinators. As with nonprocrastinators, we expected active procrastinators to have a stronger self- efficacy belief than do pas- sive procrastinators. Active procrastinators postpone tasks and direct their attention toward more urgent issues because they feel that they have control over their time use (Hypothesis 1) and are confident in their ability to finish tasks on time. 248 The Journal of Social Psychology Hypothesis 2: Both nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators will report stronger self-efficacy beliefs than will passive procrastinators. Nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators are not different on this dimension.
  • 15. Motivational Orientation Motivation is a force that drives a person to engage in a particular activity. Investigators have identified two types of motivation: intrinsic and extrinsic. According to Deci and Ryan (1985), intrinsic motivation refers to motivation that results from an internal drive. Extrinsic motivation, in contrast, refers to motiva- tion that results from either positive or negative external contingencies. Brown- low and Reasinger (2000) found that both intrinsic motivation and extrinsic moti- vation were negatively related to academic procrastination. Conti (2000) suggested that although extrinsic motivation was necessary to prevent task delay, participants with intrinsic motivation devoted more time to their projects than did participants with extrinsic motivation. On the basis of these findings (Brownlow & Reasinger, 2000; Conti, 2000), we hypothesized that nonprocrastinators are motivated by both extrinsic motiva- tion and intrinsic motivation because nonprocrastinators tend to not delay task performance and to perhaps spend more time on the task. In contrast, because of their frequent task delay and task incompletion, passive procrastinators seem to have low intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation. Finally, we speculated that active procrastinators have relatively high levels of extrinsic motivation but low
  • 16. levels of intrinsic motivation. Active procrastinators are able to finish tasks at the last minute, but they do not devote more time than necessary to each task. Their orientation toward work and life may be to achieve as much as possible in the least possible time, manifesting a value that is more closely aligned with extrin- sic motivation than with intrinsic motivation. Hypothesis 3a: Both nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators will report a high- er level of extrinsic motivation than that which passive procrastinators will report. Nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators are not different on this dimension. Hypothesis 3b: Nonprocrastinators will report a higher level of intrinsic motivation than those that active procrastinators and passive procrastinators will report. Stress-Coping Strategy When dealing with stress, individuals try to remove the threat from the stres- sor or to reduce the discomfort caused by the stressor (Latack & Havlovic, 1992). The three most frequently mentioned coping strategies include task-oriented strate- gies, emotion-oriented strategies, and avoidance-oriented strategies (Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989; Endler & Parker, 1990, 1994; Kosic, 2004). Task-ori- ented coping strategies reduce stress by focusing on immediate problems. Emotion-
  • 17. Chu & Choi 249 oriented coping strategies involve diminishing the emotional distress that is induced by the stressors. And avoidance-oriented coping strategies involve ignoring a prob- lem or distracting oneself from it. Although most stressors elicit a mixture of cop- ing strategies, task-oriented strategies predominate when individuals feel that they can do something constructive in regard to the situation, and emotion- and avoid- ance-oriented strategies emerge when people feel that they do not have much con- trol over the stressors (Folkman & Lazarus, 1980). Therefore, a person’s self-effi- cacy level partially determines which coping strategy he or she will use. Expanding this line of thought, we expected that nonprocrastinators will use task-oriented coping strategies to alleviate stress by considering realistic alterna- tives in response to problems they face. Nonprocrastinators may have a higher level of self-efficacy belief (Hypothesis 2) and thus believe that they have some control over the stressors and are capable of improving their situation. As a result, instead of engaging in irrelevant tasks or self-handicapping behaviors, they will choose to work on solving the problems at hand. Similarly, active procrastinators
  • 18. will use task-oriented coping, because their high level of self- efficacy belief makes them feel competent to overcome stressful circumstances. In contrast, we expected passive procrastinators to use either emotion- or avoidance-oriented coping strategies when encountering stressful events. Ferrari and Tice (2000) found that procrastinators were more concerned about their self- image than were nonprocrastinators, offering another explanation for the coping strategy that was used. Low self-efficacy belief and concern for poor self-image together make passive procrastinators feel powerless and cause them to actively avoid negative situations. As a result, they may cry, complain, or whine to express their frustration and fears; they may engage in trivial, irrelevant tasks to distract themselves; or they may ignore tasks completely. Hypothesis 4: In stressful situations, nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators will use task-oriented coping strategies, and passive procrastinators will use either emotion- or avoidance-oriented coping strategies. Personal Outcomes Investigators have reported that procrastination leads to low academic achievement, elevated levels of anxiety, stress, feelings of hopelessness, depres- sion, and poor physical health; the opposite patterns have been observed among
  • 19. nonprocrastinators (Beswick, Rothblum, & Mann, 1988; Bond & Feather, 1988; Dipboye & Phillips, 1990; Ellis & Knaus, 1977; Owens & Newbegin, 1997; Tice & Baumeister, 1997). In the present study, we examined four outcome variables: stress, depression, life satisfaction, and performance level as indicated by grade point average (GPA). Because previous studies have revealed opposite outcome patterns between passive procrastination and nonprocrastination with the four outcome variables, we expected to obtain similar results in the present study. 250 The Journal of Social Psychology On the basis of our conceptualization of active procrastinators, we expected them to achieve positive outcomes rather than negative outcomes. As discussed earlier in the present article, active procrastinators are more likely to engage in behaviors of time management, perceive their use of time to be more purposive, and feel more control over their use of time than do passive procrastinators. Based on the existing findings of the positive association between patterns of time use or perception and individual well-being (Bond & Feather, 1988; Macan, 1994; Macan, Shahani, Dipboye, & Phillips, 1990), constructive time- related percep- tion and behavior among active procrastinators will reduce their
  • 20. stress and depression, inducing increased life satisfaction and academic performance. The positive outcomes of active procrastinators may also derive from their self-efficacy belief, motivation, and stress-coping strategies that we hypothesized in the preceding sections. Compared with passive procrastinators, active procras- tinators have greater confidence in their own abilities in overcoming obstacles. As task-oriented individuals, active procrastinators direct their attention and actions toward pressing problems, offering themselves a greater chance of solving them in a satisfactory manner. Active procrastinators are extrinsically motivated and attempt to maximize their time utility, possibly leading to more successful task completion. Because successful task completion among nonprocrastinators reduces stress and feelings of depression and increases life satisfaction and aca- demic performance (e.g., Dipboye & Phillips, 1990; Tice & Baumeister, 1997), in the present study we expected the same positive outcomes among active pro- crastinators. Hypothesis 5: Both nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators will report a lower level of stress and depression, greater life satisfaction, and a higher GPA than pas- sive procrastinators. Nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators are not different
  • 21. on these dimensions. Method Participants and Procedure Participants were 230 undergraduate students (166 women [72.2%], 64 men [27.8%]) from three Canadian universities. The mean age of the sample was 21.49 years (SD = 2.23 years). The distribution of participants’ racial background was as follows: 53.7% Asian, 33.3% White, 6.5% Hispanic, 1.5% African Amer- ican, and 5.0% other. Regarding first language, 47.8% of the participants report- ed English, 32% reported Chinese, 7.9% reported French, and 12.3% reported other. The majority (96.5%) of participants were full-time students, and the aver- age year at university was 2.67 years. We invited participants to take part in the study by filling out a questionnaire entitled “Survey of University Students’ Time Use.” Participation was entirely voluntary, and participants completed the ques- tionnaire at their own convenience. Chu & Choi 251 Measures To measure the study variables, we used multi-item scales with acceptable
  • 22. internal consistencies. Whenever possible, we adopted existing measures with demonstrated validity and reliability. A 7-point Likert-type scale (1 = not at all true, 7 = very true) was used as the response format for all items. Academic procrastination. To measure the degree of procrastination, we adopted six items (α = .82) from two existing measures of procrastination (L. Mann’s [1982] Decisional Procrastinational Scale, as cited in Ferrari, Johnson, & McCown, 1995; H. C. Schouwenburg’s [1995] “Academic Procrastination: Theoretical Notions, Measurement, and Research,” as cited in Ferrari et al.; e.g., “I tend to leave things until the last minute”). Active procrastination. Based on our conceptualization, we developed a 12-item scale (α = .67) to distinguish active procrastinators from passive procrastinators. The scale was designed to measure four defining characteristics of active procras- tinators: (a) preference for pressure (e.g., “I tend to work better under pressure”), (b) intentional procrastination (e.g., “I intentionally put off work to maximize my motivation”), (c) ability to meet deadlines (e.g., “Since I often start working on things at the last moment, I have trouble finishing assigned tasks most of the time” [reverse coded]), and (d) outcome satisfaction (e.g., “I feel that putting work off until the last minute does not do me any good” [reverse coded]). We tested the fac-
  • 23. tor structure of the 12 items measuring active procrastination using principal-com- ponent extraction with varimax rotation. This exploratory analysis generated four factors that confirm the hypothesized factor structure, with high factor loadings on the corresponding factors (all greater than .50) and low cross- loadings (all less than .24). (Complete results of factor analysis and items are available from Jin Nam Choi on request.) We used a composite measure of these four subscales to assess the over- all level of the tendency of individuals toward active procrastination. Patterns of time use. Adopting items from the Time Structure Questionnaire (Bond & Feather, 1988), we assessed two aspects of time use. One was the struc- ture of time use (3 items, α = .70, e.g., “I have a daily routine, which I follow”), and the other was purposive use of time (3 items, α = .73, e.g., “I often feel that my life is aimless, with no definite purpose”). Perception of time control. To measure participants’ perceptions of control of their time, we used two items (α = .66) developed by Macan et al. (1990; e.g., “I feel in control of my time” and “I can use my time the way I want to use it”). Self-efficacy belief. The self-efficacy belief scale included four items (α = .76) drawn from the Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale (Schwarzer & Jerusalem,
  • 24. 252 The Journal of Social Psychology 1995) to measure individuals’ beliefs about their ability to successfully per- form desired tasks (e.g., “I believe that I can solve most problems if I invest enough effort”). Motivational orientation. The motivation scale contained seven items from Shia’s (1998) Academic Motivation Questionnaire. We measured both extrinsic motiva- tion (3 items, α = .70, e.g., “Getting a good grade in my classes is the most sat- isfying thing for me right now”) and intrinsic motivation (4 items, α = .75, e.g., “I like courses that arouse my curiosity, even if they are difficult”) for school activities. Stress-coping strategy. To assess how participants respond to stressful situations, we adopted items from the Proactive Coping Inventory (Greenglass, Schwarzer, & Taubert, 1999): (a) task-oriented coping behavior (3 items, α = .71, e.g., “I just focus on the task to get what I want”), (b) emotion-oriented coping behavior (3 items, α = .82, e.g., “I get out and talk to others to deal with stress”), and (c) avoidance-oriented coping behavior (3 items, α = .67, e.g., “I try to turn my atten- tion away from the problem”).
  • 25. Stress. We used a four-item scale (α = .77) to measure the level of stress that indi- viduals had experienced in the previous month (MacArthur & MacArthur, 2001; e.g., “In the past one month, how often did you feel difficulties were piling up so high that you could not overcome them?”). Each statement was rated on a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 = never, 7 = very often). Depression. We used a four-item scale (α = .73) to measure the level of depres- sion (Sheikh & Yesavage, 1982; e.g., “I often feel downhearted and blue”). Life satisfaction. This scale consisted of four items (α = .82) that assess stu- dents’ general satisfaction with life (e.g., “In general, I am satisfied with my life”). Academic performance. For a measure of academic performance, participants reported their GPA. Results Table 1 presents means, standard deviations, and correlation coefficients among the scales used in the present study. The correlation between the academ- ic procrastination scale and the active procrastination scale was .03 (p > .60), indi- cating that these two scales were independent and that they measured nonover-
  • 26. lapping conceptual domains. Virtually all correlations involving academic procrastination and active procrastination were in the hypothesized directions. Chu & Choi 253 254 The Journal of Social Psychology T A B L E 1 . M ea n s, S ta n d ar d
  • 47. 02 (t a b le c o n ti n u es ) Chu & Choi 255 T A B L E 1 . C on ti
  • 56. . *p < . 05 . ** p < . 01 . ** *p < . 00 1. Comparing the Three Groups Using One-Way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) The present hypotheses involved a series of comparisons among three dis-
  • 57. tinct groups of people: nonprocrastinators, passive procrastinators, and active procrastinators. To test the hypotheses, we created three equal- sized subgroups from the entire sample in a two-step process. First, nonprocrastinators were sep- arated from procrastinators. We categorized participants who scored less than 4 (which was the midpoint of a 7-point scale) on the academic procrastination scale as nonprocrastinators and categorized those who scored greater than 4 as pro- crastinators. In our sample of 230 participants, 77 were categorized as nonpro- crastinators, and 153 were categorized as procrastinators. Admittedly, using a scale midpoint to distinguish between procrastinators and nonprocrastinators might be somewhat arbitrary for the purpose of identifying procrastinators (cf. Brownlow & Reasinger, 2000). However, we used this procedure intentionally here to compare nonprocrastinators with different types of procrastinators with increased statistical power, rather than comparing “chronic” procrastinators with “pathological” procrastinators. In the second step, we further divided the 153 procrastinators into two groups: passive procrastinators and active procrastinators. To create two groups of comparable sizes, we chose 4.33 as the cutoff point on the active procrastina- tion scale. Participants who scored less than 4.33 were categorized as passive pro-
  • 58. crastinators (n = 74), and participants who scored greater than 4.33 were catego- rized as active procrastinators (n = 79). Table 2 presents the results of a one-way ANOVA, in which we compared the means of the study variables among the three groups. The first row in Table 2 shows the comparison of the level of procrastination across the three groups of individuals as identified earlier in the present article. Paired t tests indicate that passive and active procrastinators reported significantly more academic procras- tination than did nonprocrastinators (p < .001), whereas they were not different from each other, mean difference = .10, t(151) = .84. Because we compared three groups, applying the Bonferroni procedure (Rosenthal & Rosnow, 1991, pp. 329–332), we used an adjusted alpha level (.5 ÷ 3 = .017) as the criterion for sta- tistical significance of t tests. In addition, for each inferential test, effect estimates in Cohen’s d were provided (Rosenthal & Rosnow, pp. 301– 317). Time use and perception. The results of the ANOVA shown in Table 2 indicated that, as we hypothesized, there were significant mean differences in patterns of time use and time perception among the three groups. Nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators showed higher levels of purposive use of time and time con- trol than did passive procrastinators (F = 7.34, p < .01, and F =
  • 59. 19.50, p < .001, respectively; for all F tests hereafter, df = 2, 227, effect size estimates in Cohen’s d ranging between 0.41 and 1.06, all significant at p < .001). Nonprocrastinators reported greater perceived time control than did active procrastinators (d = .56, 256 The Journal of Social Psychology Chu & Choi 257 T A B L E 2 . R es u lt s of O n
  • 77. nt a ve ra ge . p < .001), but the difference between the two groups in purposive use of time was not significant (d = .17, ns). Surprisingly, however, active procrastinators report- ed a lower level of time structure than did the other two groups (both ds > .49, p < .01), and the difference between nonprocrastinators and passive procrastinators was not significant (d = .25, ns). We will address this counterintuitive pattern regarding time structure later in the present article. Self-efficacy belief. Passive procrastinators had a significantly lower self-effica- cy belief than did the other two groups (both ds > .53, p < .001). No meaningful difference was found between nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators (d = .15, ns). Therefore, Hypothesis 2 was supported. Motivational orientation. In Hypotheses 3a and 3b, we suggested that nonpro- crastinators and active procrastinators would display higher extrinsic motivation
  • 78. than would passive procrastinators and that nonprocrastinators would exhibit high- er intrinsic motivation than would both types of procrastinators. A significant dif- ference was found in extrinsic motivation (F = 4.09, p < .05) but not in intrinsic motivation. Interestingly, passive procrastinators exhibited a higher level of extrin- sic motivation than did active procrastinators (d = .43, p < .01). On the basis of the Bonferroni-adjusted criterion, the other two mean comparisons were not signifi- cant (d < .33, p > .04). Therefore, Hypotheses 3a and 3b were not supported. Stress-coping strategy. Hypothesis 4 posited that when facing stressful situations, nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators will employ task- oriented coping strategies, whereas passive procrastinators will be inclined to use either emotion- oriented coping strategies or avoidance-oriented coping strategies. The ANOVA results in Table 2 supported this hypothesis. We identified significant differences among the three groups in task-oriented coping strategy (F = 6.21, p < .01) and avoidance-oriented coping strategy (F = 16.31, p < .001) but not in emotion-ori- ented coping strategy. Active procrastinators reported significantly greater use of the task-coping strategy than did passive procrastinators (d = .55, p < .001), although the other two mean comparisons were not statistically significant. Both nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators reported
  • 79. significantly less use of avoidance-coping behavior than did passive procrastinators (ds = .92 and .68, respectively, both ps < .001). The results support Hypothesis 4. Personal outcomes. According to Hypothesis 5, nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators will experience less stress and depression and more life satisfac- tion and higher GPAs than will passive procrastinators. The present data largely confirmed this hypothesis. Among the three groups, significant differences were observed in stress (F = 14.94, p < .001), depression (F = 5.78, p < .01), and GPA (F = 9.79, p < .001). We found no significant differences between nonprocrasti- nators and active procrastinators on any of the four outcome measures, indicat- 258 The Journal of Social Psychology ing that these two groups achieved positive outcomes to a comparable degree (all ds < .22, ns). In contrast, a series of paired t tests showed that passive procrasti- nators were significantly different from both nonprocrastinators and active pro- crastinators, reporting greater stress, more feelings of depression, and lower GPAs (all ds > .39, ps < .017). Post Hoc Regression Analysis
  • 80. We could have directly tested the present hypotheses through a series of com- parisons among the three different groups of individuals as shown in Table 2. However, because the present article describes the construct of active procrasti- nation for the first time and because it is a continuous variable, it would be ben- eficial to understand how various attitude and coping variables that we use in the present study predict this new construct. To this end, we conducted regression analyses that evaluate the prediction of academic procrastination and active pro- crastination. As shown in Table 3, we used all variables that were included in this study as predictors, except the four outcome measures (stress, life satisfaction, depression, and GPA), because it is conceptually unlikely that these outcomes determine procrastination behavior. The results showed that both (a) the traditional procrastination behavior as indicated by academic procrastination and (b) active procrastination behavior were negatively related to perceived time structure (β = −.19, p < .01, and β = −.13, p < .05, respectively). However, only academic procrastination was associ- Chu & Choi 259 TABLE 3. Regression Analyses Predicting Academic Procrastination and Active Procrastination
  • 81. Dependent variable Academic procrastination Active procrastination Time structure –.19** –.13* Purposive use of time –.10 .11 Perceived time control –.35*** .05 Self-efficacy .09 .18* Extrinsic motivation –.04 –.09 Intrinsic motivation .00 .01 Task coping .05 .14* Emotion coping .08 –.01 Avoidance coping .14* –.03 Adjusted R 2 .22 .14 F(9, 220) 8.06*** 5.10*** Note. Entries are standardized regression coefficients (β). *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. ated with decreased perception of time control and increased avoidance-coping behavior (β = −.35, p < .001, and β = .14, p < .05, respectively). In contrast, active procrastination was positively related to self-efficacy and task- coping behavior (β = .18, p < .05, and β = .14, p < .05, respectively). Discussion The purpose of the present study was to empirically test whether there are different types of procrastinators. In particular, we attempted to identify a posi-
  • 82. tive type of procrastinator, which we termed active procrastinators. Contrary to prevailing assumptions about procrastinators, we revealed that a subset of pro- crastinators (i.e., active procrastinators) indeed possess desirable attitudinal and behavioral characteristics, even though they may engage in the same level of pro- crastination as “traditional” negative procrastinators (i.e., passive procrastina- tors). Members of this newly identified group of procrastinators, active procras- tinators, prefer to work under pressure and make deliberate procrastination decisions. They are more likely to accomplish tasks with satisfactory outcomes than passive procrastinators, who are often paralyzed by indecision regarding action and haunted by past failure to complete tasks. The present results indicate the possibility that nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators share similar characteristics and are significantly different from passive procrastinators. Nonprocrastinators and active procrastinators both tend to have higher levels of purposive use of time, time control, and self-efficacy than do passive procrastinators. In addition, nonprocrastinators and active procrasti- nators are more likely to experience positive outcomes. This pattern indicates that while active procrastinators procrastinate to the same degree as do passive pro- crastinators, mean difference = −.10, t(151) = −.84, their personal characteristics
  • 83. and outcomes are quite more similar to those of nonprocrastinators. Implications for Future Research We expected active procrastinators to structure their time to a high degree, as do nonprocrastinators. Unexpectedly, however, active procrastinators scored the lowest on the time structure scale, and there was no significant difference between nonprocrastinators and passive procrastinators. One possible explana- tion for this finding is that even though active procrastinators may plan their activ- ities in an organized fashion, they do not restrict themselves to following a pre- planned schedule or time structure. If something unexpectedly comes up, they will switch gears and engage in new tasks that they perceive as more urgent. In other words, active procrastinators may have more flexibly structured time and are more sensitive to changing demands in their environment. For this reason, they will act more spontaneously, resulting in more frequent temporal changes than with the other two groups. This interpretation, however, is only speculation. 260 The Journal of Social Psychology Future studies are needed to investigate the precise mechanisms underlying this
  • 84. unexpected pattern by examining variables such as degree of awareness of envi- ronmental demands or willingness to make changes. In line with Conti’s (2000) study, we hypothesized that both nonprocrastina- tors and active procrastinators will have a higher level of extrinsic motivation than will passive procrastinators. Interestingly, passive procrastinators turned out to have the highest level of extrinsic motivation. In the research setting of the present study, we constantly evaluated students’ performance on the basis of externally imposed requirements, which often effectively reduce an individual’s sense of autonomy and enjoyment in regard to a task (Deci & Ryan, 1985). Therefore, stu- dents who have extrinsic reasons for studying may feel controlled and pressured and find the task to be aversive. To avoid these unpleasant feelings and percep- tions of being forced to engage in particular activities, they might be more likely to postpone their academic work (Blunt & Pychyl, 1998; Lay, 1990). Passive pro- crastinators, who have weak self-efficacy beliefs, may find imposed requirements unpleasant. For this reason, they may develop task aversion and postpone acade- mic work to avoid feelings of incompetence. From this perspective, the only rea- son for passive procrastinators to perform tasks may be the threat of serious neg- ative consequences for failing to perform.
  • 85. Potential Limitations Overall, the present study confirms the profile of nonprocrastinators and tra- ditional procrastinators (i.e., passive procrastinators) in terms of their personal characteristics and outcomes (see Dipboye & Phillips, 1990; Ferrari et al., 1992; Lay & Schouwenburg, 1993; Tice & Baumeister, 1997; Vodanovich & Seib, 1997). This consistency with prior findings increases our confidence in the present results. Nevertheless, the current findings should be interpreted with caution because of several limitations of the present study. Our sample consisted of university stu- dents, who may possess different characteristics than other populations such as organizational employees. Therefore, the generalizability of the current results to populations engaging in other types of tasks is limited. Another limitation is that all variables were self-reported at one point in time. This cross- sectional nature of the data, which was obtained from a single source, raises the possibility of insen- sitivity to temporal changes of variables, ambiguous causal directions of the observed relationships, and boosted correlations among variables (Podsakoff & Organ, 1986). Practical Implications Despite these limitations, the present study offers a new perspective on pro-
  • 86. crastination and how people perceive and use their time. In terms of practical implications, the current findings present a need to break our long-standing view Chu & Choi 261 of procrastination as an unhealthy or unproductive behavior. We should acknowl- edge the possibility that some forms of procrastination may actually enhance the well-being and performance of individuals. Active procrastination may be partic- ularly beneficial, or even necessary, for individuals who work in highly demand- ing, unpredictable, and fast changing environments. In this type of context, active procrastinators may function more effectively than others by not restricting them- selves to previously established plans and thus would be able to deal spontaneously with unexpected changes. They may constantly engage in a process of reprioritiz- ing each of their tasks and thereby increase their capacity to more effectively meet frequently changing situational demands. Therefore, active procrastinators may function very effectively in highly uncertain situations with frequently changing environmental contingencies and demands. Future studies of this issue of per- son–situation fit may offer valuable contributions to managerial practice.
  • 87. In the present study, we identified a positive form of procrastination. Future studies may expand the present findings with the use of different samples to repli- cate the presence of active procrastinators in other populations. Future investiga- tors might also use a longitudinal research design to detect temporal changes in variables and to clarify the causal directions of the observed relationships. More- over, conceptual efforts could be devoted to further clarification of the active pro- crastination construct. Once investigators have clearly defined the construct of active procrastination, and a valid measurement tool becomes available, the active procrastination construct could be compared with other potentially related per- sonality constructs, such as those of Type A behavior or high- achievement ori- entation. It would also be meaningful to develop more precise descriptions of active procrastinators in terms of other personality traits such as the Big 5 factors or locus of control. REFERENCES Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psy- chological Review, 84, 191–215. Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  • 88. Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., & Tice, D. M. (1994). Losing control: How and why people fail at self-regulation. San Diego: Academic Press. Beswick, G., Rothblum, E., & Mann, L. (1988). Psychological antecedents to student pro- crastination. Australian Psychologists, 23, 207–217. Blunt, A., & Pychyl, T. A. (1998). Volitional action and inaction in the lives of under- graduate students: State orientation, procrastination and proneness to boredom. Per- sonality Individual Difference, 24, 837–846. Bond, M. J., & Feather, N. T. (1988). Some correlates of structure and purpose in the use of time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 321– 329. Brownlow, S., & Reasinger, R. D. (2000). Putting off until tomorrow what is better done today: Academic procrastination as a function of motivation toward college work. Jour- nal of Social Behavior and Personality, 15, 15–34. Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A 262 The Journal of Social Psychology theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 267–283.
  • 89. Conti, R. (2000). Competing demands and complimentary motives: Procrastination on intrinsically and extrinsically motivated summer projects. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 15, 47–59. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press. Dipboye, R. L., & Phillips, A. P. (1990). College students’ time management: Correlations with academic performance and stress. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 760–768. Ellis, A., & Knaus, W. J. (1977). Overcoming procrastination. New York: Signet. Endler, N. S., & Parker, J. D. A. (1990). Multidimensional assessment of coping: A criti- cal evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 844–854. Endler, N. S., & Parker, J. D. A. (1994). Assessment of multidimensional coping: Task, emotion, and avoidance strategies. Psychological Assessment, 6, 50–60. Fee, R. L., & Tangney, J. P. (2000). Procrastination: A means of avoiding shame or guilt? Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 15, 167–184. Ferrari, J. R. (2001). Procrastination as self-regulation failure of performance: Effects of cognitive load, self-awareness, and time limits on “working best
  • 90. under pressure.” Euro- pean Journal of Personality, 15, 391–406. Ferrari, J. R., Johnson, J. L., & McCown, W. G. (1995). Procrastination and task avoid- ance: Theory, research, and treatment. New York: Plenum Press. Ferrari, J. R., Parker, J. T., & Ware, C. B. (1992). Academic procrastination: Personality correlates with Myers-Briggs types, self-efficacy, and academic locus of control. Jour- nal of Social Behavior and Personality, 7, 595–602. Ferrari, J. R., & Tice, D. M. (2000). Procrastination as a self- handicap for men and women: A task-avoidance strategy in a laboratory setting. Journal of Research in Psychology, 34, 73–83. Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1980). An analysis of coping in a middle-aged communi- ty sample. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 21, 219–239. Greenglass, E., Schwarzer, R., & Taubert, S. (1999). The Proactive Coping Inventory (PCI): A multidimensional research instrument. Retrieved February 17, 2003, from http://userpage.fuberlin.de/~health/greenpci.htm Harriott, J., & Ferrari, J. (1996). Prevalence of procrastination among samples of adults. Psychological Reports, 78, 611–616. Knaus, W. J. (2000). Procrastination, blame, and change. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 15, 153–166.
  • 91. Kosic, A. (2004). Acculturation strategies, coping process and acculturative stress. Scan- dinavian Journal of Psychology, 45, 269–278. Latack, J. C., & Havlovic, S. J. (1992). Coping with job stress: A conceptual evaluation framework for coping measures. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 13, 479–508. Lay, C. H. (1990). Working to schedule on personal projects: An assessment of person- object characteristics and trait procrastination. Journal of Social Behavior and Person- ality, 5, 91–104. Lay, C. H., & Schouwenburg, H. C. (1993). Trait procrastination, time management, and academic behavior. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 8, 647–662. Macan, T. H. (1994). Time management: Test of a process model. Journal of Applied Psy- chology, 79, 381–391. Macan, T. H., Shahani, C., Dipboye, R. L., & Phillips, A. P. (1990). College students’ time management: Correlations with academic performance and stress. Journal of Education Psychology, 82, 760–768. Chu & Choi 263 MacArthur, J. D., & MacArthur, C. T. (2001). Perceived Stress
  • 92. Scale. Retrieved February 18, 2003, from Department of Psychology, University of California, San Francisco, Web site: http://www.macses.ucsf.edu/Research/Psychosocial/notebook/P SS10.html Owens, A., & Newbegin, I. (1997). Procrastination in high school achievement: A causal structural model. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 12, 869–887. Podsakoff, P. M., & Organ, D. W. (1986). Self-reports in organizational research: Prob- lems and prospects. Journal of Management, 12, 531–544. Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1991). Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and data analysis (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1995). Generalized Self- Efficacy Scale. In J. Weinman, S. Wright, & M. Johnston, Measures in health psychology: A user’s portfolio: Causal and control beliefs (pp. 35–37). Windsor, England: NFER- Nelson. Sheikh, J. L., & Yesavage, J. A. (1982). Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS). Retrieved Feb- ruary 18, 2003, from Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford Uni- versity, Web site: http://www.stanford.edu/~yesavage/GDS.english.long.html Shia, R. M. (1998). Assessing academic intrinsic motivation: A look at student goals and
  • 93. personal strategy. Unpublished college thesis, Wheeling Jesuit University, Wheeling, WV. Retrieved February 17, 2003, from http://www.cet.edu/research/papers/motiva- tion/motivation.pdf Steel, P., Brothen, T., & Wambach, C. (2001). Procrastination and personality perfor- mance, and mood. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 95–106. Tice, D., & Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Longitudinal study of procrastination, performance, stress, and health: The cost and benefits of dawdling. Psychological Science, 8, 454–458. Tuckman, B. W. (1991). The development and concurrent validity of the procrastination scale. Education and Psychological Measurement, 51, 473–480. Vodanovich, S. J., & Seib, H. M. (1997). Relationship between time structure and pro- crastination. Psychological Reports, 80, 211–215. Received December 29, 2003 Accepted January 25, 2005 264 The Journal of Social Psychology