SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 110
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979).
Pergamon Press. Printed in U.S.A.
0047-2352’79/030217-26s2.WO
Copyright @ 1979 Pergamon Press Ltd
INFORMATION, APPREHENSION, AND DETERRENCE:
EXPLORING THE LIMITS OF POLICE PRODUCTIVITY
WESLEY G. SKOGAN
Department of Political Science and Center for Urban Affairs
Northwestern University
Evanston. Illinois 60201
GEORGE E. ANTUNES
Department of Political Science
University of Houston
Houston, Texas 77004
and
Workshop in Political Theory and Policy Analysis
Indiana University
Bloomington, Indiana 47401
ABSTRACT
The capacity of police departments to solve crimes and
apprehend offenders is low for many types of crime, particu-
larly crimes of profit. This article reviews a variety of studies
of police apprehension and hypothesizes that an important
determinant of the ability of the police to apprehend crimi-
nals is information. The complete absence of information for
many types of crime places fairly clear upper bounds on the
ability of the police to effect solutions.
To discover whether these boundaries are high or low we
analyzed data from the 1973 National Crime Panel about the
types and amount of information potentially available to po-
lice through victim reports and patrol activities. The evidence
suggests that if the police rely on information made readily
217
218 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES
available to them, they will never do much better than they
are doing now. On the other hand, there appears to be more
information available to bystanders and passing patrols than
currently is being used, which suggests that surveillance
strategies and improved police methods for eliciting, record-
ing, and analyzing information supplied by victims and wit-
nesses might increase the probability of solving crimes and
making arrests. In light of this we review a few possibly help-
ful innovations suggested in the literature on police produc-
tivity and procedure.
Some characteristics of the crime itself, or of events
surrounding the crime, that are
beyond the control of investigators, determine whether it will be
cleared in most in-
stances. (Greenwood et al., 1975: 65)
There is no feasible way to solve most crimes except by
securing the cooperation of
citizens to link a person to the crime. (Reiss, 1971: 105)
INTRODUCTION
A recent spate of studies of crime and the deterrent
effectiveness of the criminal
justice system has raised anew a question as old as Bentham:
Does raising the cost of
criminal activity signiticantly reduce the level of crime in a
community? In these studies,
the cost of criminal activity has been conceptualized in two
ways: as the loss of time and
opportunity attendant to apprehension (measured by the
certainty of arrest or punish-
ment), and as the stigma, discomfort, and loss of opportunity
that come with conviction
by the courts (measured by the severity of punishment).
Indicators of the distribution of
these costs imposed by the criminal justice system are related in
these studies to mea-
sures of the crime rate in a sample of jurisdictions by employing
their variation across
areas as surrogates for different policy interventions by the
state. The findings are then
addressed to the effectiveness of those “policies.” (For a
prominent example of this sort
of analysis, focusing on the death penalty, see Ehrlich, 1975.)
The studies differ in their
choice of indicators for costs, as well as in their unit of
analysis, sources for data,
statistical design, and the decision rule by which the deterrence
hypothesis will be
confirmed or rejected. Not surprisingly, they differ in their
conclusions as well.
This article sidesteps the issue of the effectiveness of
deterrence policies and fo-
cuses instead on one crucial link in the process-the ability of
the police to gather
evidence, solve crimes, and make arrests. Any measure of the
cost of crime ultimately
rests on this activity, yet there has been little effort to assess
the apprehension activities
of the police within a deterrence perspective. The deterrent
effect of the severity of
sanctions (using as indicators either statutory maximums or
sentences actually imposed)
or the impact of court conviction (employing as an indicator the
ratio of convictions to
crimes known to the police) is mediated by the ability of the
criminal justice system to
Information. Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the
Limits of Police Productivity 219
identify and apprehend individual offenders in the first place.
If, as one reasonable line
of deterrence research indicates (Aniunes and Hunt, 1973).
certainty of punishment is
more important than severity, the importance of the problem
becomes even clearer.
Even the issue of justice points to the centrality of certainty, for
it is the notion that
harsh sentences may be imposed on an unfortunate few who
happen to be caught that so
offends our sensibilities. As we shall see, the plain fact is that
for many types of crimes,
particularly those motivated by profit, the probability that an
arrest will be made is very
low. Explaining why this is so is one of the main goals of this
article.
We argue that an important determinant of the ability of the
police to apprehend
criminals is information. The police enjoy varying amounts of
information in criminal
cases: in some they have positive identifications to go on
(victims or witnesses can tell
them who did it and, frequently, where that person lives), in
others they can gamer only
rough descriptions of offenders, and in some they have only
physical evidence or traces
of the methods employed by perpetrators to guide their
investigations. Police officers on
patrol can spot certain offenses in progress, while in other cases
they must await the
reports of bystanders or victims themselves. All of these affect
the ability of police to
solve crimes, and we argue that the absence of information in
many cases, coupled with
the practical invisibility of many (but fewer) cases to official
scrutiny, places limits on
the ability of the police to do anything.
In an attempt to discover where those limits are high and low,
we employ data from
the 1973 national victimization survey conducted by the U.S.
Census Bureau to analyze
the types and amounts of information potentially available to
the police through victim
reports and patrol activities. As we shall see, the evidence
suggests that if the police rely
on information made readily available to them, they never will
do much better than they
are doing now. On the other hand, there appears to be more
information available to
bystanders and passing patrols than currently is being used,
which suggests that surveil-
lance strategies and improved police methods for eliciting,
recording, and analyzing
information supplied by victims and witnesses might serve to
raise the probability of
solving crimes and making arrests.
INFORMATION AS A DETERMINANT OF ARREST:
PREVIOUS RESEARCH
Crimes differ in the extent to which they admit of solution.
Greenwood’s study of
crime in New York City found that only 4 percent of burglaries
and larcenies, 8 percent
of auto thefts, and 13 percent of robberies were cleared by at
least one arrest (1970:24).
The probability of arrest for crimes of passion was higher,
ranging from 46 percent for
assault to 71 percent for homicide. Other scholars have reported
similar findings in other
cities (e.g., Reiss, 1971).
We believe that the availability and reliability of information
about incidents and
offenders plays a key role in determining the ability of the
police to solve crimes and
apprehend offenders. The most easily solved crimes are those in
which the victim or
others close to the act can identify an alleged offender. For this
to occur, there must
be some interaction between the victim or the victim’s friends
and a suspect. Furtive
crimes that occur in the dead of night or in private places when
the victim is not
present are unlikely to produce either witnesses or useful
information of any type. For
example, burglary typically involves no contact between victim
and offender, is devoid
220 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES
of tangible clues, and usually is not even discovered until hours
after the burglar has
made an exit. Except during a (usually brief) intrusion period,
the event takes place out
of the public eye and screened from the sight of passing police
patrols. As a result,
burglary is a crime that is virtually impossible to solve, and the
role of the police in
responding to it generally is one of record keeping and
sympathetic commiseration with
victims’ (Repetto, 1974; Conklin and Bittner, 1973). In
contrast, robbery offers a great
deal of information that potentially is helpful in identifying and
apprehending the rob-
ber: There is a direct confrontation between the parties and thus
the offender can be
described and perhaps identified, weapons used may be
described, a getaway vehicle
may be observed and traced, and robberies usually are reported
promptly so there is a
chance that the offender may be apprehended near the scene of
the crime (Conklin,
1972; McDonald, 1975).
Several types of offenses feature at least limited interaction
between suspect and
victim, and they all enjoy relatively high rates of apprehension.
This relationship ranges
from modest (personal theft, including purse snatching) to
intense (rapes, assaults, and
murder). Robbery lies in the middle of the spectrum. Interaction
may be intense and
victim observation of the suspect considerable, but the typical
robbery takes only 60 to
90 seconds to complete (Eliot, Strack, and Witter, 1975). Few
robberies take place
between acquaintances. Curtis (1974) sampled police files for
1967 in seventeen cities
and reports that about 86 percent of all robberies found there
were attributed to
strangers. Thus, in most cases descriptions of the suspect’s
appearance and behavior,
although clouded with some uncertainty, constitute the major
clues available to the
police in robberies. “Robbery, more than any other type of
street crime, requires the use
of departmental intelligence (“M.O.“) files designed to identify
suspects” (Greenwood,
1970). The face-to-face confrontation between robber and
victim, while failing of positive
identification, enhances arrest probabilities. * It also gives us
more extensive research
data on the personal characteristics of robbers such as race, sex,
and apparent age, than
on other “criminals for profit” (Sagalyn, 1971).
In contrast to robbery, victims and witnesses of rape, assault,
and homicide often
possess knowledge of the offender’s identity. In many reported
cases victim and of-
fender are at least acquainted before the event. Amir’s (1971)
study of rape in Philadel-
phia indicated that 58 percent of rapes involved individuals who
knew one another under
other circumstances, while the special study of crime in
Washington, D.C. conducted for
the President’s Commission on Law Enforcement and
Administration of Justice (1967)
estimated an “acquaintance rate” for rape of 64 percent. Curtis’
(1974) study of police
files in seventeen cities set that figure at 47 percent.
Acquaintance rates for homicide and aggravated assault usually
are higher. Marvin
Wolfgang (1958) reported that 86 percent of the murder victims
in his sample from
Philadelphia were acquainted with their slayer, and the
President’s Commission study in
Washington, D.C. reported 70 percent; Curtis (1974) set the
figure at 84 percent. A study
of homicide in Chicago for the years 1965 to 1969 found that 71
percent of victims and
offenders were so associated, and that in 21 percent of the cases
the immediate parties
were related by blood (Chicago Police Department, 1970).
Nationally, only 23 percent of
murders known to the police in 1975 were “felony type,”
involving other criminal acts
such as robbery or burglary (another 9 percent were thought to
possibly belong in this
category), about the same percentage as those involving
spouses, parents, and other
information, Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the
Limits of Police Productivity 221
relatives (Federal Bureau of Investigation, 1976). Curtis (1974)
attributed 25 percent of
all homicides to relatives, and an additional 9 percent to close
friends and lovers. Aggra-
vated assaults also are crimes of passion, and many occur within
acquaintance networks
that allow positive identification when they are reported to the
police. Acquaintance
rates for assaults known to the police range from 65 to 80
percent, and several studies
based on police files have estimated that 20 percent take place
within the immediate
family (Curtis, 1974; President’s Commission, 1967; Pittman
and Handy, 1964). Reported
rapes, on the other hand, rarely are family affairs: Amir’s study
(1971) attributed only
2.5 percent to related parties, while Curtis (1974) attributed 7
percent.
Descriptions or positive identifications of offenders are
important to the criminal
justice system, for criminal acts are more likely to generate
arrests if the “circle of
investigation” can be decreased by attaching a name or label to
a suspect. The circle of
investigation defines a pool of possible offenders who must be
considered in designating
a set of plausible suspects. Any information such as the age,
race, or sex of the offender
tightens the circle of persons who must be sorted through to
identify likely suspects. A
national victimization survey conducted for the Law
Enforcement Assistance Adminis-
tration (1975) indicated that only about one-third of all personal
crimes of violence were
committed by persons who were known to the victim and could
be identified (presuma-
bly) immediately. Thus, in two-thirds of personal crimes of
violence the circle of investi-
gation is fairly large and the task of identifying an assailant is
rather arduous. In these
instances the cooperation of victims and witnesses is crucial
since even simple informa-
tion about the physical characteristics of offenders aids in
reducing the circle of investi-
gation and identifying suspects.3
If a crime were committed by someone known to the victim, the
circle of investiga-
tion would encompass only one person, and the job of the police
would be reduced to
finding the suspect. This may account for the extremely high
clearance rate for homicide
even though victims themselves are not in a position to shed
much light on the matter. I
Many murder suspects even fail to flee the scene of the-crime,
due perhaps to the bond
between the parties. A Chicago murder analysis (Chicago Police
Department, 1970)
revealed a high incidence of what local police call “smoking
gun” homicides: 35 percent
of all murders during the 196% 1969 period were cleared when
responding patrol officers
arrested a suspect at the site (much the same obtains in
Philadelphia-see Rubenstein,
1973:343). An additional 20 percent were cataloged as “known
but flown” cases, those
in which the suspect was not present but proved immediately
identifiable by witnesses,
neighbors, bystanders, or family members.
The frequency of murder, fortunately, is relatively low. Crimes
other than homicide
in which virtually no information is available to the police (or
at least almost none is
recorded by police) are much more common. In a recent study
of the disposition of cases
by the police in Rochester, New York, conducted for the Police
Foundation, Bloch and
Bell (1976) judged the utility of suspect and property
identifications contained in police
reports on burglary and robbery cases. They found descriptions
of suspects nonexistent
in 68 percent of all burglary reports, but in only 3 percent of
robbery reports. (This does
not mean that information on robbers was all that good-the
modal category for those
data was “vague.“) Thus, for many common crimes the amount
of information available
in police records may be so low as to render unrealistic the
expectation that offenders
will be identified, arrested, and brought to trial.
222 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES
INFORMATION: NEW DATA
Previous research on the information problem faced by
participants in the criminal
justice system has been based largely on data extracted from
local police files. Given the
irregular and often unreliable nature of that data, it is
remarkable how regular the
emerging picture remains.
Victimization surveys now being conducted by the Census
Bureau provide a new
perspective on these same problems. Victim surveys were
designed to bypass the organ-
izational and procedural difficulties involved in collecting
reliable national crime data
through police departments (see Wolfgang, 1963). Surveys
acquire, directly from vic-
tims, reports of events that .can be analyzed with some
confidence (for a description of
these surveys and a critical appraisal of their methodology see
Skogan, 1976~; National
Academy of Sciences, 1976). While published official reports
based on these data have
emphasized the frequency of various types of victimization
(e.g., Law Enforcement
Assistance Administration, 1975 and 1976), the surveys
themselves gather extensive data
on detailed characteristics of incidents and victims’
recollections of their perpetrators.
These data can be analyzed to reveal under what circumstances
useful information
appears to be available to the criminal justice system, and the
frequency with which
information about the very existence of criminal events remains
hidden from the police
and the courts.
The data analyzed in Table 1 were collected through interviews
with a national
panel of approximately 132,000 people. They were questioned
every six months about
events occurring during the previous six months for periods
between February 1973 and
June 1974 to produce estimates of the yearly distribution of
crime for calendar 1973 (for
a description, see Argana, 1975). The data are analyzed here to
uncover “who knows
what” about criminals that might be useful to police, improve
apprehension rates, and
promote the deterrent consequences of that activity.
Table 1 explores the distribution of “don’t know” responses to a
series of questions
asked of all victims uncovered in the survey about their
perpetrators. This allows us to
judge the frequency with which descriptions of offenders are
potentially available to the
police.4 Like research based on police files, the victim data
indicate that the most
important distinction between crimes is the duration of personal
contact between the
parties. Interpersonal violence (assaults and rapes) and
robberies are characterized by
high levels of information at the descriptive level. 5 About 95
percent of the victims of
those crimes were willing to assess the sex and race of their
assailants, somewhat fewer
hazarded an estimate (choosing among a series of ranges) of
their ages. Purse snatchings
and picked pockets, on the other hand, are personal thefts
characterized by brief inter-
changes of information; only about 50 percent of the victims of
those crimes have much
of a tale to tell the police if they report the event to them. When
we examine property
crimes, the paucity of information potentially available to the
criminal justice system is
apparent. In the case of burglary, only about 5 percent of the
victims have anything
concrete to tell the police about “whodunit,” and the figure is
only marginally better for
auto theft.
Table 1 also tells us the distribution of positive identifications
of suspects as op-
posed to simple descriptions of perpetrators. The analysis is
based on a series of ques-
tions probing victim-offender relationships. 6 It categorizes
incidents into those in which
victims do not even know what the relationship might be, so
slight is their information;
Information, Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the
Limits of Police Productivity
TABLE 1
223
DESCRIPTIONS AND IDENTIFICATIONS’
Information
Interpersonal Personal Vehicle
Violence* Robbery Theft3 Burglary Larceny Theft
Percentage of Victims
Able to Describe
Perpetrator’s:
Sex 96.8 %.l 53.4 5.4 4.3 7.6
Race 96.0 93.9 52.5 4.8 4.2 7.0
Age 90.5 92.1 51.0 4.8 4.1. 7.1
Percentage of Victims
Who:
Don’t Know Relationship 3.2 3.6 46.0 90.5 92.9 91.8
Know Relationship-
Is a Stranger 53.8 78.6 49.0 8.5 5.6 5.8
Some Identification
Possible 43.0 17.8 5.0 1.0 1.5 2.4
(N of cases) (3.777) (1,023) (512) (5,789) (19,601) (1,198)
‘Based on all regular and series incidents from the National
Crime Panel for reference year 1973; computations
by the authors. The figures reported here are refined and differ
slightly from those previously reported.
*Includes crimes classified by the FBI as assault (simple and
aggravated) and rape.
%cludes purse snatchings and pocket picking.
cases in which they know enough to know they did not know the
other party; and those
in which there is a chance that some identification might be
made (offenders are de-
scribed as “casual acquaintances,” etc.). To the extent to which
positive identifications
realistically are required to solve crimes, the data in Table 1 do
not speak well to the
potential ability of the criminal justice system to solve crimes
and deter criminals. While
descriptions abound for certain personal crimes, identifications
do not. Only in interper-
sonal violence, which we have seen includes a considerable
number of nonstranger
assaults and some rapes, can victims positively identify a
substantial number of of-
fenders. The frequency of descriptions in robberies is
unmatched by identifications, for
most robberies are attributed to strangers. Among property
crimes, the situation appears
hopeless.
This hopelessness reflects our reading of how the police do
their work. The fact of
policing-as opposed to reconstructions of police work depicted
by the media and in
detective novels-is that investigatory follow-up work, the
gathering of physical evi-
dence, and the ferreting out of criminals through detective
work, p1ay.a relatively unim-
portant role in identifying and apprehending offenders. In Bloch
and Bell’s (1976) study
in Rochester, only about 5 percent of burglaries were solved
through follow-up investiga-
tions, and most of those came from interviews with victims and
new witnesses. There,
224 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES
only 0.1 percent of all burglaries were solved by physical
evidence, and an additional 0.3
percent by the recovery of traceable property (Bloch and Bell.
1976:45). Of the few
burglaries that were solved, most were either cleared by arrests
at the scene based on
information supplied by victims and (mostly) witnesses, or by
obtaining confessions
from persons later arrested for something else. As the RAND
Corporation concluded in
a recent analysis of the criminal investigation process
(Greenwood et al., 1975:ix):
The single most important determinant of whether or not a case
will be solved is the
information the victim supplies to the immediately responding
patrol officer. If informa-
tion that uniquely identifies the perpetrator is not presented at
the time the crime is
reported, the perpetrator, by and large. will not be subsequently
identified.
These data suggest that “unique identifications” are rarely
available.
INFORMATION: UNREPORTED CRIME
While duration of contact and other determinants of the
perceptual acuity of the
victims of crime shape the kind and accuracy of the information
they potentially can
offer the criminal justice system, even this potential is not fully
realized. Two major
social and organizational processes -citizen crime-reporting and
police incident-record-
ing-mediate the flow of information between victim and system.
It is widely known
that the police do not record, or record accurately, all of the
complaints that are
brought to their attention (Seidman and Couzens, 1974). That
source of error in crime
statistics is beyond the scope of this article, but see Skogan
(1976b). Victimization
surveys, on the other hand, were designed to reveal something
of the dimensions of
“the dark figure of unreported crime” by probing for the
frequency and rationale for
nonreporting. What those data reveal is substantial amounts of
citizen nonreporting.
even in major crime categories (Skogan, 1976a). Nonreporting
may serve to shield from
police attention a large amount of potential strategic
intelligence about the distribution
of crime (useful for planning, manpower allocation, and the
like), and it may also
protect individual offenders from the “long arm of the law.” It
has also led us to
overestimate seriously the extent to which the police are able to
solve crimes in the
United States since measures of “certainty” of punishment in
most empirical research
on deterring crime have been ratios of the number of arrests for
a type of crime (e.g.,
larceny) divided by the number of crimes of that type officially
known to the police.
Assuming that unreported crimes would be unlikely to generate
additional arrests if
they were known to the police,7 then to the extent that crimes
are underreported our
estimation of the capacity of police to make arrests (certainty)
is erroneously inflated.
Table 2 summarizes National Crime Panel estimates of citizen
reporting practices
for the major types of crime measured by the survey. The data
indicate that over 67
percent of all attempted and successful auto theft was reported
to the police (or they
were on the scene of the crime), while the comparable figure
was about 50 percent for
robbery and only about 22 percent for simple property larceny.
The failure of many or
most crimes to come to the attention of the police in the first
place clearly sets severe
upper limits on their ability to solve an appreciable portion of
them, given current
patterns of citizen reporting and police activity.
When we examine the next filter in the flow of information to
the police, the ability
Information. Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the
Limits of Police Productivity
TABLE 2
225
NONREPORTING AND THE FLOW OF INFORMATION'
Crime
Percentage Reported Percentage Reported
Percentage Reported? and Know Race of and Positive
or Police on Scene Offender identification N
Interpersonal
Violence
Robbery
Personal Theft
Burglary
Larceny
Vehicle Theft
40. I 38.1 16.9 3.777
49.6 43.6 7.4 1,023
31.6 18.9 1.4 512
45.8 2.6 0.2 5.789
22.5 I.0 0.3 19.603
67.2 3.5 0.5 1.198
‘Computed by the authors from the 1973 National Crime Panel
regular and series incidents.
*A small number of “don’t know” responses to the question on
reporting to the police are treated as nonre-
porters in this analysis.
of victims to pass on some description of the offender in crimes
that are reported, we
find further attenuation in the ability of the police to ensure
high levels of certainty of
arrest. The effect is not large for interpersonal violence, for
most of those crimes that are
reported to the police carry with them some modicum of
information. In the case of
robbery, and especially purse snatching, the effect of
nonreporting and a lack of descrip-
tive information each make serious inroads into the solvability
of offenses, however.
Only 19 percent of all personal thefts were reported to the
police and carried with them
some information about the offender. Again, in the case of
property offenses only an
extremely small number of cases admit of much useful
information for the criminal
justice system.
The most realistic column of Table 2, that which isolates
reported crimes in which
some positive identification might be made, indicates that
almost all of the cases that
might come to the attention of the police can be solved only
with some difficulty. Only
interpersonal violence, crimes that are characterized by victim-
offender relationships of
some duration and often take place within acquaintanceship
networks, have the potential
to yield many positive identifications. Identifications could be
made only in about 7
percent of all robberies, 1 percent of personal thefts, and even
fewer burglaries.
The data in Tables 1 and 2 indicate that there are severe
structural obstacles to the
solution of large numbers of serious crimes by the police. The
obstacles are “structural”
in the sense that they rest on enduring human patterns of
cognition and behavior under
conditions of uncertainty and stress. Offenders in many of these
events are seen only
fleetingly, often under bad conditions. and estimates of such
personal attributes as age
are doubtlessly difficult under any circumstances. Research by
Buckhout (1974) has
226 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES
TABLE 3
NATIONAL APPREHENSION RATIOS (1973)
Crime’
Survey’ Apprehension Ratioss
UCR Arrests* UCR Incidents3 Incidents UCR Survey
Forcible Rape
Aggravated
Assault
Robbery
Vehicle Theft
Burglary
25,720 51,000 153,000 0.50 0.17
208,100 416.270 1,313,180 0.50 0.16
127,530 382,680 1,214,884 0.33 0.10
155,800 923,600 1,330,470 0.17 0.12
434,000 2,540,OOO 7,818,026 0.17 0.06
‘Following standard UCR (Uniform Crime Reports) definitions,
these are crimes that generally are compatible
between the UCR and LEAA’s (Law Enforcement Assistance
Administration) victim surveys. Robbery and
burglary counts include both household and commercial survey
data; FBI conventions regarding incident and
victimization counting rules are used here. Personal theft is
excluded here for lack of good comparable UCR
figures see Skogan (19766).
*Uniform Crime Report, 1973, Table 24; FBI 100 percent
estimates for U.S. population.
Wniform Crime Report, 1973, pp. I l-29; FBI 100 percent
estimates for U.S. population.
4Estimated U.S. totals calculated from unpublished tabulations
supplied by the Census Bureau (incident series)
from the 1973 reference year National Victim Survey data.
SEstimated U.S. totals computed by dividing UCR arrests by
UCR, U.S. estimates, and by survey incident
estimates for 1973.
documented the well-known unreliability of eyewitness
testimony. These problems are
not likely to go away, or even to change very dramatically in
magnitude.
These data thus may be seen as setting some rough upper limits
on the extent to
which society can deal effectively with major crimes. The
ability of the criminal justice
system to solve, by arrest, crimes with a positive identification
is high, but those crimes
are a very small portion of all crimes. Effective and innovative
police work may extend
the ability of the system to apprehend criminals some distance
into the pool of crimes
with suspect descriptions, but, given the limited resources and
the great number of cases
coming to the attention of the police daily, such sleuthing never
will extend very far.
Thus, even crimes that do come to the attention of the police
will include a great number
(perhaps 20 to 90 percent) of successful predations. When we
add to that number the
vast pool of events that go unreported in the first place, it is
clear that the certainty of
arrest for specific crimes is quite low indeed.
Table 3 attempts to estimate more realistic certainty levels for
various types of
crimes. It presents both official and survey data on crimes
relevant to the computation of
“apprehension ratios,” or the ratio of arrests to crimes. Such
ratios typically are used to
measure certainty of arrest or police effectiveness in a
jurisdiction (Hatry, 1975) and
serve as indicators of the certainty component of deterrence
models. Table 3 indicates
that the use of officially reported crime in the denominator of
such measures greatly
enhances the apparent certainty of arrest in each category, for
those data underestimate
Information, Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the
Limits of Police Productivity 227
the frequency of crime in the population. The ratio of arrests to
officially known crime in
the aggravated assault category reaches nearly 1:2, while the
ratio of arrests to the
National Crime Panel’s estimate of the number of aggravated
assaults in the United
States is closer to 1:61/4.
By this measure, the certainty of arrest for assault (a crime
involving serious physi-
cal injury or the use of a dangerous weapon) is much lower than
previously calculated. A
difference of the same magnitude can be noted in the forcible
rape category and (less
dramatically) for robbery and burglary. The apprehension ratio
for burglary in particular
is extraordinarily small when we calculate it as the ratio of
arrests to survey-measured
crime.8 These figures, in fact, raise some question whether
deterrence processes have
ever “been given a chance” in this country. Much academic
research on deterrence has
concluded that certainty of punishment, not severity of
punishment, is the key to effec-
tive crime reduction (see Antunes and Hunt, 1973). The low
apprehension rates pre-
sented here indicate that the holes in the preventive net cast by
the criminal justice
system are so large that we should not be surprised that the
threat of criminal sanction
appears to have so little impact.g
POLICE ORGANIZATION AS AN INFORMATION-
GATHERING STRATEGY
The criminal justice system will depend on the development of
dramatically more
effective means of coping with many types of crime to raise the
certainties of arrest
significantly. Only by extending the arm of the law rather
deeply into the pool of
currently unidentified offenders will such inroads be made. It
thus is useful to examine
policing strategies as organizational devices for gathering
information. Such strategies
are numerous. Patrol officers cruise looking for “activity,” and
attempt to arrive on the
scene rapidly after the reporting of a crime. Technicians gather
fingerprints and other
physical evidence. Detectives assemble photographs of
suspicious persons and descrip-
tions of their methods of operation.
While these activities reflect conventional wisdom about the
effective organization
of policing, recent studies have begun to challenge their
efficacy. Reiss has pointed out
that arrest productivity in general is low, and that when arrests
are made, they are likely
to be made by patrol officers rather than detectives. In an
analysis of 1965 Washington,
D.C. data, Reiss found the 87 percent of arrests for Part I Index
crimes (crimes against
persons) were made by the patrol division (1971: 104). He
believes that the reason for
this is that “arrests do not result from investigation by a
specialized division of the
department, such as the detective division, but rather from the
routine activity of patrol
as it responds to citizen calls for assistance” (1971: 105).
Similarly, Greenwood et al.
(1975) sharply questioned the productivity of most detective
work. They found that little
time is in fact devoted to the investigation of most serious
crimes, and that most investi-
gatory effort is spent in essentially administrative activity. The
detective practice of
reinterviewing victims, duplicating in many instances the work
of patrol officers, does
not expand the amount of information available in most cases.
As Greenberg et al.,
found in their study of robbery and other crimes, “unless
relevant field information had
been obtained at the crime scene by the responding [patrol]
officer, if the offender had
not been apprehended, the chances of the case being solved at
the detective level were
minimal” (1977:Xx).
228 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES
Greenwood et al., (1975) also argue that most physical evidence
is unproductive,
and that fingerprinting as it is currently organized rarely itself
leads to arrests. They
conclude that these problems are in part structural, relating less
to the quality of police
work (they found that neither training nor the organization of
detective work much
affected these matters) than to the nature of the task at hand,
and that certain crimes.
“which constitute the majority, are difficult, if not impossible,
to solve, regardless of the
efforts expended by police” (Greenwood et al., 1975:65). Or, as
Conklin and Bittner
(1973) noted while traveling with burglary detectives, some
“evidence” of any sort was
present in only one-third of all cases. Repetto’s study of
residential burglaries in Boston
indicated similar low investigative productivity levels+nly 2.0
percent of the cases
were solved by detective follow-up investigation (1974:50).
Other research has begun to challenge the assumptions
underlying police patrol
strategies: that the visible presence of patrol cars on the streets
prevents crimes from
occurring, and that when crimes do occur, police officers will
be able to either observe
them in progress or arrive on the scene rapidly enough to gather
fast-dissipating informa-
tion. The preventive effect of patrol is difficult to assess, and it
is becoming apparent
that the Kansas City South Patrol Experiment (Kelling et al.,
1974) has provided neither
a definitive nor generalizable answer to that question (Larson,
1975; Davis and Knowles,
1975). Data gathered from victims in the national crime survey
indicate that there is an
upper limit to the number of crimes that potentially can be
observed by police patrols,
and that very few crimes currently are being interrupted in
progress by passing patrols.
However, the vulnerability of certain crimes to police
intervention, which is shaped by
the information available to the criminal justice system, is not
as severely constrained as
the potential for positive eyewitness identification. Although
apprehension is much more
problematic when that information is not available, crimes that
are visible or in which a
rapid “hue and cry” may be raised provide opportunities for
intervention that may raise
certainty levels if appropriate policing strategies are adopted.
Table 4 presents 1973 National Crime Panel data on the location
of criminal of-
fenses, categorized to shed some light on the extent to which
those incidents were likely
to occur in public places where they could be observed either by
bystanders or passing
police patrols. Incidents in Table 4 are classified as taking place
(1) in a home, (2) in a
public building, or (3) on a street, field, park, or “near home”
(and, of course. some fell
in the ubiquitous “other” category). Assuming that those that
took place on a street,
etc., were most visible to police patrols, we can see in Table 4
that the potential for
police intervention varies considerably from crime to crime.
Robbery, more than assault
and rape,‘O takes place in public places, as do many larcenies
and vehicle thefts.
Rapes in particular tend to occur in the home (almost 30 percent
of the total re-
ported in the 1973 survey). a place legally and culturally almost
completely shielded from
official view (Stinchcombe, 1963). In between fall many
personal thefts and larcenies
(purse snatchings and the like) that occur in shops or office
buildings, but not in places
highly visible to official police. It is most likely that private
security guards, along with
controlled access and egress from buildings and employee
training in vigilance, would
have the highest deterrent payoff in such cases. The large
number of robberies that take
place on the street (visible to the police) or in public buildings
(visible to bystanders able
to raise a rapid hue and cry), combined with the high amount of
descriptive information
available in such crimes, may recommend them for special
attention by criminal justice
planners. Policies to increase police or public surveillance of
the streets, reduce neigh-
Information, Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the
Limits of Police Productivity
TABLE 4
229
VULNERABILITY OF CRIMES TO PATROL’
Crime
At In a On Street or 9% of Police
Home Building* Near Home Other “On Scene”3
Total Assault 11.0 23.4 54.2. 11.4 3.7
Forcible Rape 29.3 7.8 45.5 17.4 1.4
Robbery 10.9 18.8 64.8 5.5 1.6
Personal Theft 2.1 47.7 39.3 10.8 0.7
Larceny 4.8 27.0 60.0 8.2 0.3
Burglary 96.3 3.7 0.0 0.0 0.4
Vehicle Theft 0.0 1.5 94.8 2.8 1.8
‘Based on all regular and series incidents in the 1973 National
Crime Panel data. Incident N'S are the same as
previous tables.
*The “in a building” category combines events occurring in
schools, hotels, office buildings, and personal
crimes occurring in commercial establishments.
3Percentage of incidents in each type of crime, regardless of the
place ofoccurrence, in which the police were
present or arrived at the scene without being called.
borhood anonymity, and encourage bystander reporting to the
police may have substan-
tial payoffs in raising certainty rates for robbery. It is important
to note, however, that
these visibility figures probably again set important upper
bounds on the solvability of
crimes. As Rubinstein has noted (1973:339-40), the ordinary
routines of police work
virtually preclude the extension of patrol intervention into
private or even semipublic
places:
For the policeman, the determining factor of any crime’s
importance is its setting. He
defines the location of all crimes by the deceptively simple
distinction between “inside”
and “outside.” [Olutside means any location a patrolman can be
reasonably expected to
see while on patrol. . . . Any outside crime is an afront to the
patrolman’s notion of
himself as a guardian of his territory, an occurrence which
suggests to his superiors that
he was not doing his job properly.
Thus, for example (p. 339), “If a burglar breaks into a building
through a rear door or
cuts a hole in a roof, his act is considered an inside crime,
because the patrolman had no
chance to notice anything amiss, even if he was patrolling
alertly.” And (p. 341), “The
majority of all murders and aggravated assaults occur inside and
therefore they are not
‘on’ the patrolman.”
The result is that relatively few crimes currently are being
interrupted in progress. l1
As Table 4 also indicates, in no major category were as many as
4 percent of crimes so
described by victims in the 1973 survey. Robbery was typical of
personal crimes: in that
year only 1.6 percent were in progress when the police arrived
or the police arrived
before they could be summoned. For larceny and burglary
similar percentages were
230 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES
vanishingly small. The data on the visibility of crimes presented
in Table 4 indicate that
these figures potentially could become much larger, however.
Many crimes, including
some with currently low apprehension rates, frequently occur on
the street or in other
public places where police and citizen activities might have
more productive conse-
quences if they were effectively organized.12
SHOULD ONLY “GOOD” ARRESTS BE COUNTED?
Thus far we have focused on the problem of identifying and
arresting offenders.
However, all arrests are not equally productive. Some arrests
result in a guilty plea or
conviction, but many do not. Measures of arrest productivity
must look beyond the
simple counting of arrests. Making an arrest is one thing;
making an arrest that will
result in an indictment and conviction is something else
entirely. In some senses a better
measure of arrest productivity is the ratio of arrests resulting in
conviction to crimes
known to the police. This reflects, at least in part, the diligence
with which police have
conducted investigations and built cases. In a related
suggestion, Hatry (1975) has pro-
posed as one measure of productivity the number of arrests that
survive the first judicial
screening per police man-year. He argues that simply counting
arrests without regard to
quality, particularly if used as a measure of the performance of
a department or of
individual officers within a department, may produce perverse
effects; specifically, po-
lice “may be encouraged to make excessive, unreasonable, or at
least marginal arrests”
(Hatry, 1975: 103). Whatever the specific measure, attempts to
gauge arrest productivity
must reflect the quality of those arrests; the way in which the
police do their job in
investigating a case has a profound effect on the eventual
disposition of those who are
arrested. As Jacob has noted (1978:173), “The quality of police
work determines, to a
considerable extent, what the courts can do with those whom the
police arrest.”
Of course, as police are quick to point out, these productivity
measures also reflect
the influence of other actors such as prosecutors, judges,
witnesses, and juries, over
whom the police have little or no control. Still, considering
productivity from the per-
spective of arrests that result in conviction has much to
recommend it. While over
three-fourths of all Index crimes are not cleared by arrest, there
also is considerable
slippage out of the justice system after an arrest has been made.
An analysis of data from
Kansas City (Pate, Bowers, and Parks, 1976) revealed that of
those persons arrested for
Part I Index crimes half were not indicted, and thus were simply
released. Of those who
were indicted, only one-third were eventually convicted or
pleaded guilty. Two-thirds,
the bulk of those indicted, had charges dropped by the
prosecutor, were dismissed by
the judge, or were found not guilty. Given that such a small
portion of offenses is solved
by arrest, the substantial attrition (over 80 percent) within the
prosecution and court
system of the cases of those few who are arrested is disturbing.
This means that the
certainty of conviction for many types of crime is virtually nil,
and police productivity
measured in terms of “good” arrests (i.e., those that hold up in
court and result in a
conviction or guilty plea) is similarly low. The data reported by
Pate and his associates
indicate that the Kansas City Police Department, widely
respected as one of the best in
the country, manages one felony conviction per 14,720 patrol
hours, or about one con-
viction per seven work years!13
Similar results have been reported for Washington D.C.
Analyzing data from the
Information. Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the
Limits of Police Productivity 231
Prosecutor’s Management Information System (PROMIS),
researchers from the Institute
for Law and Social Research found tl~ it only about 9 percent of
commercial robberies
were cleared by an arrest. I4 Furthermore, about half of those
arrested dropped out of the
system; the result was that only about 5 percent of felonious
commercial robberies
reported to the police resulted in either a felony or misdemeanor
conviction or guilty
plea. The figures for commercial burglaries were even worse.
Only about 4 percent of
commercial burglaries known to police resulted in an arrest, and
over half of those
arrested dropped out of the system; thus, about 2 percent of the
commercial burglaries
known to police resulted in a conviction or guilty plea of some
kind.
A second analysis of Washington, D.C. PROMIS data reported
the curious finding
that arrest productivity in the Washington police primarily
results from the actions of a
small portion of the force; the department’s arrest productivity
appears to be concen-
trated in a small subset of the force (Forst, Luclanovic, and
Cox, 1977). The analysis
revealed that 46 percent of the department’s sworn personnel
made no arrests at all in
l974; a total of 63 percent made no arrest resulting in a guilty
plea or conviction. Forst
and his associates found that over half of all arrests that
resulted in a guilty plea or
conviction were produced by 368 officers, about 8 percent of
the department’s sworn
personnel. Why these officers were so productive while most of
the force was notably
unproductive is unknown. The limited data in the PROMIS files
allow Forst and his
colleagues to point to some interesting possibilities, but a
definite answer is impossible.
They believe that arrests succeed in court only when the
arresting officer presents the
prosecutor with strong physical evidence and good witnesses.
Only 32 percent of the
department’s 1974 robbery arrests resulted in a conviction (or
guilty plea), but “the
number of convictions per 100 robbery arrests was 60 percent
higher when tangible
evidence was recovered than when it was not, and it was 40
percent higher when the
MPD secured at least two lay witnesses than when they did not”
(Forst, Lucianovic,
and Cox, 1977:24). Thus, although both the RAND study of
detectives (Greenwood et
al., 1975) and the Rochester team policing study (Bloch and
Bell, 1976) found that
officers seldom gathered physical evidence, and that when they
did it did not prove
helpful in solving cases, neither of those studies followed cases
after arrest to learn their
fate in the courts. Had they done so, the role of tangible
evidence might have taken on
more importance in their analysis.
The extent to which blame for post-arrest attrition should be
apportioned among
police, courts, prosecutors, victims, and witnesses is unclear.
However, an analysis of
the Washington PROMIS data by Cannavale and Falcon (1976)
provides some indication
of a few of the things that can go wrong. They found a serious
problem caused by
interrogation practices used by police in field settings.
Investigating why a substantial
number of witnesses. could not be located by either follow-up
detective investigators or
the prosecutor’s office, Cannavale and Falcon found that this
was often the result of
witnesses being interrogated by police within earshot of the
offender. In these circum-
stances they tended to give false names and addresses to avoid
the possibility of retribu-
tion at the hands of the offender. Of course when they did this,
witnesses could not be
contacted to appear in court, thus allowing the offender to go
free in many instances.15
The research also brought to light major problems within the
prosecutor’s office in the
management of cases and witnesses. A program was devised to
modify police field
interrogation practices, and the prosecutor’s office totally
revamped its previously er-
ratic witness management system. These changes should reduce
somewhat the 23 per-
232 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES
cent of cases that were dropped by the prosecutor’s office
because witnesses were
classified as unfindable or uncooperative. Of course, some
victims and witnesses truly
are uncooperative. A study by the Vera Institute (1976) of the
100,739 felony arrests
made by the New York City police in 1971 found that only
about 5 percent of those
arrests resulted in felony prison sentences. In about half of the
cases involving a victim
there was a prior relationship between the victim and the
defendant, and in many of
these cases the victim refused to press charges.
WHAT IS TO BE DONE?
In this article we have pointed out some unpleasant empirical
generalizations about
the operation of police in local communities.
The capacity of police to solve most kinds of crime is low. Most
Part I offenses
known to the police are nor solved.
When crimes are solved and arrests made, it is almost always
due to extensive
information supplied to police by witnesses and victims as to
the identity and where-
abouts of the offender.
The amount of information about the offender that could be
supplied by victims and
witnesses is a function of the type of crime and the
circumstances in which it
occurs. For many ordinary crimes the amount of information
that can be supplied
police is extremely slight.
Most arrests are made by patrol officers: the arrest productivity
of detectives is very
low.
The capacity of police and prosecutors to successfully prosecute
those who are
arrested is low. Most of those arrested for Part I offenses will
eventually have
charges dropped or the case dismissed.
In the face of these grim realities what, if anything, can be
done? Could the police do
better if they tried? Would increased efforts by citizens improve
arrest productivity? We
believe that although a dramatic improvement in arrest rates is
unlikely (and indeed may
be impossible), there are a number of promising ways by which
police and communities
may be able to effect real gains in arrest productivity. Our
approach is based on two
premises: (1) the importance of citizens as co-producers of
police outputs and (2) the
crucial importance of information as the basis for solving
crimes and arresting offenders.
As Kelling and his associates concluded:
Assigning the police full responsibility for the maintenance of
order, the prevention of
crime and the apprehension of criminals constitutes far too great
a burden on far too
few. Primary responsibility rests with families, the community
and its individual mem-
bers. The police can only facilitate and assist members of the
community in the mainte-
nance of order, and no more. (1974:533)
Information. Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the
Limits of Police Productivity 233
The solution of crimes is the shared responsibility of both the
citizens and the police
in a community; both are in a very real sense co-producers of
police services.r6 The role
of the police in solving crime has long been taken for granted;
the notion that citizens
also play an important role in this process is somewhat more
novel. Citizens are impor-
tant because most information about crime, including the fact
that it has occurred,
comes from citizens who are victims or witnesses. Because the
success of the police in
making arrests is so dependent on the actions of witnesses and
victims, citizens share
responsibility for police productivity. l7 Reiss underscored this
when he pointed out
(1967: 105) that, “the capacity of the police to solve any crime
is severely limited by
citizens, partly owing to the fact that there is no feasible way to
solve most crimes
except by securing the cooperation of citizens to link a person
to the crime.” Convincing
the public that this is true may require a considerable effort.
Popular films, novels, and
television dramas generally portray crime as a matter that will
be dealt with effectively
by the police. As a result, the public has formed certain
expectations about crime that
underemphasize the role of victims and witnesses and
overestimate the efficacy of the
police. As Sanders wryly pointed out (1977: lOO), because they
have seen it on television,
burglary victims expect that police will dust for fingerprints,
and when prints are found
they believe that “if you send the prints to the FBI, they’ll send
you the name, photo-
graph, and address of the burglar.” Police departments must
convince the public that the
realities of policing differ from the sometimes flattering images
depicted in the mass
media, and that crimes can be solved only when citizens report
them to the police and
assist in identifying the offender.
To be sure, police departments contribute to the solution of
crimes, but communi-
ties must be aware that, realistically, the police acring on their
own have a very limited
impact. The recent response-time study by the Kansas City
Police Department (1977)
dramatically underscores this point. The researchers found that
the average victim did
not contact police until more than six minutes after the incident
occurred. They esti-
mated however that reporting crimes within two minutes of
completion would increase
arrests 10 percent. Further, if burglaries observed by victims or
witnesses were reported
within one minute, the probability of making a response-related
arrest would improve 40
percent. It is ironic that for all the public concern expressed in
recent years about
improving police response time, as far as improving on-scene
arrests goes, the average
amount of delay introduced by citizens is so great that it
virtually precludes the transla-
tion of reductions in police response time into increased arrest
productivity.18
The police cannot be everywhere; in fact the police “discover”
very few crimes in
progress. Communities must rely on their citizens to call police
to the scene of a crime
and, if the offender has already fled, to provide as complete a
description as possible of
the incident, the offender, and any missing property. We believe
that community aware-
ness campaigns should stress the importance of those witnessing
a crime (or suspicious
behavior) reporting the incident to police and making notes on
the description of sus-
pects, getaway car, etc., to improve the quality of information
they can provide the
police. Some communities have undertaken citizen “anticrime”
programs, but the focus
of many of these campaigns has been limited to crime
deterrence through such tactics as
personal self-defense courses or installing better door and
window locks. Our approach
to the crime of burglary would not ignore household
“hardening” measures, but it would
also stress the importance of vigilant and concerned neighbors
and the necessity of
preparing an inventory and description of major items of
property in each household.
234 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES
Citizens need to know that burglars are generally apprehended
only when their activities
are noticed by neighbors who alert the authorities or when
police are able to trace stolen
property based on information provided by the victim.
Once citizens have been prodded into reporting crimes and
information about crime,
the focus of responsibility shifts from citizens to the police. It
is possible that some
improvement in arrest productivity may result from modest
changes in the way police go
about their work. In policing, innovations and proposed
innovations to enhance produc-
tivity abound. We will not undertake a systematic review of
suggested police innova-
tions; that is beyond the scope of this article. lg However, we
do wish to consider some
selected examples to illustrate how the ideas we have developed
can serve as a frame-
work for thinking about innovation and arrest productivity.
We believe that the key to greater arrest productivity lies in
increasing the amount
and quality of information available to the police, preserving
(i.e., not losing) that infor-
mation, and efficiently making use of the information. Police
practices must be scrutin-
ized in terms of the way they affect the acquisition and use of
information.
The initial police response to citizen reports is made by patrol
officers. Most arrests
are made either by patrol officers or by detectives using
information that was recorded by
patrol officers. The way patrol officers do their job has
important consequences. If the
investigating officer does not ask appropriate questions about
the incident, witnesses may
not volunteer and important information will be omitted. Or if
information is not recorded
accurately and in some comprehensible format, any follow-up
investigation will be im-
peded. For this reason many police departments have begun to
revise their training proce-
dures to emphasize the importance of preliminary investigation
in accurately gathering
relevant information while events are still fresh in the minds of
witnesses. This position is
summarized by Chief George Hart of the Oakland Police
Department who argues that,
“we must reevaluate our traditional thinking concerning the role
of the patrol officer in the
investigative process, and we must give very careful attention to
our training and record-
ing function to ensure that maximum attention is given to those
investigative elements of
information that have been shown to be useful in the solution of
crimes” (1977:~~).
In connection with revised training, many police departments
(Greenwood et al.,
1975:83) are restructuring their incident report forms so that
patrol officers routinely
record on a standardized form information about offenders and
their characteristics.20 It
is expected that this will increase the amount of information
recorded by patrol officers,
and, because of the standardized format, the information
gathered in the initial investiga-
tion should be more accessible to follow-up investigators and
police planners. However,
as Greenberg et al. (1977) point out, implementing these ideas
in the investigation pro-
cess involves some serious trade-offs; it is not true that more
information necessarily is
more productive. In some circumstances, the use of lengthy,
preceded incident forms
may be counterproductive. Greenberg and his associates at
Stanford Research Institute
agree that information is essential to apprehension and
prosecution, but they are pessi-
mistic about the way in which this notion has been implemented
in some departments
where investigating officers must wade through long, general
lists of questions and
preceded investigation forms. They believe that the “main
objective of patrol-to ensure
the safety of the victim and quickly ascertain what information
can be derived to hasten
the offender’s apprehension-can be thwarted by undue delay in
running over a list of
data that are likely to be useless” (1977:xxii). Greenberg and
his colleagues argue that
the key to enhanced productivity lies in collecting only that
information that is likely to
lnformation, Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the
Limits of Police Productivity ‘235
be useful in identifying and apprehending an offender. They
have developed and tested
specialized investigation forms for robbery that minimize the
amount of information
collected but maximize the likelihood that it will be useful
information (Greenberg et al.,
1977). In earlier research Greenberg, Yu, and Lang (1973),
developed a similar special-
ized form and decision model for investigating burglaries. A
replication testing this
burglary model in some thirty police departments currently is
being conducted by the
Police Executive Research Forum (Farmer, 1978).
Another area of police department improvement involves liaison
between patrol
officers and detectives. As Bloch and Bell (1976) note, relations
between patrol officers
and detectives are frequently characterized more by a spirit of
hostility and mistrust
rather than a spirit of cooperation. Unless the patrol officer sees
an opportunity to
personally make an arrest, the preliminary investigation is often
perfunctory. Detective
investigators, in turn, tend to ignore preliminary investigation
reports and begin anew,
duplicating work already done. The goal of the Rochester Team
Policing experiment was
to improve productivity by improving patrol detective liaison.
In the Rochester experi-
ment, instead of dividing patrol officers into geographically
based units and relegating
detectives to a single, centralized detective division, detectives
were paired with patrol
officers to form police teams; each team was assigned to a
specific geographic area.
Teams had their own offices and were responsible for most
police services within their
areas on a twenty-four-hour basis. Team commanders were
responsible for both patrol
and investigative operations. Both Rochester police officials
and outside evaluators
judged the experiment to be a success (Bloch and Bell, 1976);
detectives were able to use
preliminary investigation reports much more productively, and
their follow-up investiga-
tion developed significantly greater additional information. The
result was a substantial
increase in arrest productivity.*’
The Rochester experiment also developed two management
techniques that were
credited with improving productivity. One was centralized case
management, which
allowed a team commander to assign specific investigative tasks
rather than entire cases,
to specific patrol officers or detectives. This allowed work to
proceed very quickly, and
also allowed work to go round the clock when needed. The
second management tech-
nique was “early case closure.” This recognized that some
crimes are unsolvable-
information is scant, there are no leads, and prospects for
identifying the offender are
miniscule. As Greenwood et al. showed (1975:65), regardless of
the amount of effort
expended by investigators, cases like this will not be solved.
Under early case closure
“hopeless” cases are identified as quickly as possible on the
basis of a number of
explicit solvability factors and they are officially closed. This
allows investigators to
concentrate on those cases where there is at least a reasonable
chance of solving the
case and making an arrest.22
A final area of productivity enhancement that we wish to
discuss is that of compu-
terized data analysis and data management systems. These
systems, which are coming
into use in many police departments, potentially can improve
the capacity of police
departments to make use of the voluminous data that they
routinely gather. Some police
departments are using their data systems to follow cases through
the prosecution and
trial phase of the judicial system. Proper use of a data analysis
system allows police
departments to keep track of the number of “good” (i.e.,
prosecutable) arrests they are
making and pinpoint problems of departmental practices
needing improvement (Canna-
vale and Falcon, 1976).
236 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES
Other possible applications include computerized M.O. (modus
operandi, or,
method of operation) files, computerized fingerprint files, and
computer-assisted mug
shot systems. However, the practical utility of existing systems
‘designed to apply com-
puters to these problems is open to question. Greenberg et al.
(1977), found the compu-
terized M.O. systems, fingerprint systems, and physical
description inventories they
examined to be of little value in identifying offenders. Among
the factors they felt were
responsible for this failure were the often low quality of
information entered into the
systems and the fact that few offenders seem to specialize in a
single type of street
crime. Although an automated, national fingerprint search
capability would be particu-
larly helpful (Greenwood et al., 1975; Greenberg, Yu, and Lang,
1973), it appears that
the availability of any operational and practical system is still
some years in the future.
The examples we have reviewed are only a few of many
proposals for enhancing
police productivity and improving the likelihood of identifying
and apprehending felons.
We have presented these examples as illustrations of the kinds
of innovation being tried
by a number of police departments. Unfortunately, as Antunes
and Hunt (1973) pointed
out, while the most effective deterrent policy may be one that
increases the certainty of
apprehension as much as possible, neither criminal justice
researchers nor police admin-
istrators know how this might be accomplished or what it might
cost. We have no
specific remedies to propose. Our purpose has been to focus
attention on the capacity of
the police to identify and apprehend offenders. We have shown
that the probability of
apprehension varies according to the type of crime and the
circumstances surrounding
the crime. For crimes of profit, apprehension probabilities are
very low, and the reason
for this is the paucity of information about the identity of the
offender. Our analysis of
victim survey data indicates that, while there is much room for
improvement, the infor-
mation problem is real and fixes discernable limits on police
productivity.
We believe that scholars and police administrators must
forthrightly address the
productivity problem and shift their attention to apprehension
strategies and tactics that
involve the public and that are formed within the information
acquisition and utilization
framework we have discussed. Crime is a problem for which
there are no easy solutions.
But we believe that communities can, and should, ameliorate the
problem by gradually
improving their capacity to identify and apprehend offenders.
Given our present state of
ignorance about these matters, there is a need for a major effort
to identify potentially
helpful programs and innovations, and to rigorously evaluate
their usefulness in a variety
of community settings. This, combined with programs that
involve the public in the fight
against crime, holds the key to improving the currently dismal
situation.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
Wesley G. Skogan was supported in part during the
development of this paper by a Visiting
Fellowship grant (76NI-994032) from the National institute of
Law Enforcement and Criminal
Justice, Law Enforcement Assistance Administration, and by the
Center for Urban Affairs at
Northwestern University’s Research Agreement Program grant
(77Nl-994018) from the same
source. George E. Antunes was partially supported by a grant
(5T32-MH-15222-02) from the Na-
tional Institute of Mental Health to the Workshop in Political
Theory and Policy Analysis at Indiana
University. Points of view or opinions stated here are those of
the authors and do not represent the
official position of the United States Department of Justice or
the National Institute of Mental
Health. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the
1978 meeting of the Midwest Political
Science Association.
Information. Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the
Limits of Police Productivity 237
NOTES
In this regard, Greenwood et al.. note (1975:7) that “it may be
obvious at the outset that the
crime is not likely to be solved. The police view in these
situations is that a distraught victim
deserves attention. regardless of the eventual result.”
Indeed, one study (Ward, Ward, and Feely, 197564-65). points
out that within the set of events
classified as robbery, clearance rates are systematically lower
for unarmed compared with armed
robbery. The study attributes this difference to the substantially
lower amount of information
available in unarmed robberies that, as a group, are briefer in
duration than those in which a
weapon is employed.
A substantial proportion of arrests for serious crime occurs
because a victim or witness is able to
supply police with the name and address of the offender
(Greenwood et al., 1975:68). Similar
findings are reported by Isaacs (l%7). Folk (1971), Feeney et al.
(1973). Conklin (1972). and
Conklin and Bittner (1973).
Of course, the total amount of information available
undoubtedly includes additional information
provided by witnesses who were not victims. The National
Victim Survey focuses on informa-
tion recalled by those who were victims. We have no data on the
amount of information available
from respondents who witnessed crimes other than those in
which they were victimized. There
may be an important amount of untapped information available
from nonvictim witnesses that
might improve arrest productivity. Unfortunately, we know of
no victim-survey data that ad-
dress this point, and thus we must leave the question
unresolved. That this is something worth
considering in the design of future surveys is suggested by the
Kansas City response-time study
that found that about 9 percent of crimes reported to police were
reported by witnesses who
were not victims (Kansas City Police Department, 1977).
Assaults and rapes have been combined here for methodological
reasons. Validity research by
the Census Bureau indicates that rapes are often “confused”
with assaults by the survey instru-
ment (Turner, 1972). Also, only 158 rapes were uncovered by
the 1973 national survey, although
this weights to over 150,000 for estimation pruposes.
Validity research by the Census Bureau indicates that the survey
figures for assault substantially
underestimate the amount of nonstranger assault in the
population, for those events often are not
recalled to the interviewer (Turner, 1972).
The resistance of unreported crimes to solution (had they in fact
been reported to police) is
suggested by the explanations citizens give for not reporting
incidents to the police. The most
frequent reason is that “nothing can be done” and the second
most frequent rationale is that “the
police wouldn’t want to be bothered.” Both of these responses
suggest citizen awareness that less
serious offenses that lack clear evidence as to the identity of the
perpetrator are very unlikely to be
solved. For further discussion of the nonreporting phenomenon
see Skogan (1976~).
However, it should be noted that even if the probability of
arrest for any given criminal act is
low. if one commits such acts regularly, the probability of being
arrested at least once is much
higher. Take as a starting point Greenwood’s finding (1970:24)
that New York police solved by
arrest four of every one hundred burglaries; this is a fairly low
(but not unusual) probability of
apprehension. Repetto (1974:21) found in his sample of burglars
that it was common for them to
make at least two hits per week. If we allow a burglar two
weeks off for vacation, this would still
amount to one hundred burglaries per year. Applying the
appropriate formula (Owen, 1970:229),
we find that the probability of no arrest (if the probability of
arrest is 0.04 and one hundred
burglaries are committed during a year) is 0.016; subtracting
from 1 .O we find that the probability
of being arrested at least once during the year is 0.984! This
implies that while the police do not
solve many burglaries, they eventually catch most burglars.
Our suspicion is that this estimate is far too high for
professional burglars. Nationally, the
median age of those arrested for burglary is 17. If, as Repetto
suggests (1974:24), professional
burglars are older (25+) and more skilled than burglars below
the age of 18, then the probability
of arrest is not likely to be uniform across all classes of
burglars. We believe that the probability
of arrest is higher for younger, less experienced burglars, and
much lower for experienced
professionals. This would mean that the probability of a
professional being arrested in any given
year would be substantially lower than the 0.984 computed in
our example.
9 For a thorough review of the substantive findings and
methodological questions raised by the
research literature on the deterrent effect of criminal sanctions
see Blumstein, Cohen, and
Nagin, 1978.
238 WESLEY Cl. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES
lo We examine rape separately here because of the relatively
large amount of “at home” crime it
accounts for.
I’ A study of the Los Angeles Police Department estimated that,
by chance, every police patrol
officer would pass the scene of a burglary about once every
three months, and that of a robbery
in progress about once every fourteen years (Institute for
Defense Analysis, 1%7: 12). However,
due to the fact that most burglaries (particularly of residences)
offer few tangible clues that might
alert passing officers, the likelihood of detection and
intervention for that crime is low.
I2 In addition, the use of alarms may effectively convert many
crimes not occurring in public places
from “inside” crimes to “outside” crimes for which police
officers can be held accountable.
Early Robbery Warning systems (Eliot, Strack, and Witter,
1975), for example, link silent alarms
in commercial establishments directly to patrol cars, thus
decreasing response time while effec-
tively moving the crime “outdoors.” Traditional burglar alarm
systems also may reduce re-
sponse time and increase arrest rates (Conklin and Bittner,
1973), and they surely have the same
effect as preventive patrol-to scatter intruders.
I3 A specialized unit in the Kansas City Department that spent
most of its time staked out at
locations thought to be likely robbery or burglary targets did
somewhat better, but even it
managed only one felony conviction per work year (Pate,
Bowers, and Parks, 1976:98).
I* Computed by the authors from data presented by the Institute
for Law and Social Research
(1977: IO).
I5 Cannavale and Falcon (1976) also found that the major
determinant of successful prosecution
was the number of prosecution witnesses. This implies that in
cases where there are multiple
witnesses, a loss of witnesses that is insufficient to justify
dropping the case may seriously
jeopardize chances of obtaining a conviction. Conversely,
retaining all possible witnesses max-
imizes the probability of a guilty plea or conviction.
is As Gam et al., (1976) point out, co-productivity is a general
feature in the production of all social
services; it should not be construed as somehow unique to the
provision of police services.
I7 Citizens may also be thought of as sharing with offenders
responsibility for the production of
crime. Many citizen actions, such as leaving the keys in a car,
encourage crime, while other
actions, such as getting and using good locks in a home,
discourage crime.
I8 It should be noted that, at least in Kansas City, police
response time is very good; obviously,
this may not be true of departments in other communities.
According to data collected in the
response-time study (Kansas City Police Department, 1977) the
average time to receive a call
and dispatch a patrol unit is under three minutes. More
importantly, the study revealed that the
amount of time consumed in this process is primarily
determined by the reporting style of those
calling the police; there is little likelihood that this time can be
reduced significantly. Once a
patrol unit is dispatched, the amount of time it takes to reach
the scene of a crime is almost
entirely a function of the physical distance between the scene of
the crime and the location of the
patrol unit at the time of dispatch. This time could be reduced
somewhat by increasing the
number of patrol units, thus reducing the average distance to be
traveled, but this would sharply
escalate costs. (In departments staffing patrol cars with two
officers, this could be accomplished
by switching to one-officer patrol units.) In general, however,
because of citizen delay in report-
ing, police in Kansas City are racing to the scene of a crime
long after there is any reasonable
chance of making an arrest at or near the scene of the crime.
Unless some way can be found to
substantially reduce the time between the conclusion of a
victimization incident and notification
of police, the speed of police response will have only a limited
effect on arrest productivity. If
citizen delay in reporting could be reduced, the generally rapid
police response time in Kansas
City means that there is at least some hope of effecting an on-
scene arrest. In other jurisdictions
increased arrest productivity may require improvement in both
the citizen and the police compo-
nents of response time.
[email protected] For a general overview of the innovation and
police productivity literature, see the report of the
Advisory Group on Productivity in Law Enforcement (1973).
Many of the proposals it reviews
have to do with productivity in areas other than the
identification and apprehension of offenders
(e.g., non-crime related calls for service, preventive patrol,
etc.), but others clearly bear on the
question of arrest productivity.
*O For a discussion of a variety of specific techniques intended
to improve the information gathering
activities of patrol officers conducting investigations see Gay,
Schell, and Schack (1977:117-19).
Information, Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the
Limits of Police Productivity 239
*I Not all innovations produce such felicitous results. A
demonstration program in Wilmington
intended to enhance patrol productivity by separating calls for
service from deterrence-oriented
patrol succeeded in raising the arrest productivity of patrol
officers, but it had such a negative
effect on detective investigations that the overall arrest
productivity of the force declined (Tien,
Simon, and Larson, 1978).
2* This notion of deciding on the basis of a set of objective
criteria that a case is unlikely to be
solved by follow-up investigation and should be dropped was
pioneered by Greenberg, Yu, and
Lang (1973) in their work on burglary investigation.
REFERENCES
Advisory Group on Productivity in Law Enforcement (1973).
Opportunities for improving produc-
tivity in police services. Washington, DC: National Commission
on Productivity.
Amir, M. (1971). Patterns in forcible rape. Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
Antunes, G. E., and Hunt, A. L. (1973). The impact of certainty
and severity of punishment on
levels of crime in American states: An extended analysis.
Journal of criminal law, criminology ond
police science, 64:48&93.
Argana, M. (1975). Development of a national victimization
survey. In Emilio Viano (ed.) Victimol-
ogy: A New Focus, Vol. Ill. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books,
pp. 171-79.
Bloch, P. B., and Bell, J. (1976). Managing investigations: The
Rochester system. Washington, DC:
The Police Foundation.
Blumstein, A.; Cohen, J.; and Nagin, D., eds. (1978).
Deterrence and incapacitation: Estimating
the effects of criminal sanctions on crime rates. Washington,
DC: National Academy of Sciences.
Buckhout, R. (1974). Eyewitness testimony. [email protected]
American, 231:23-31.
Cannavale, F., and Falcon, W. (1976). Improving witness
cooperation. Washington, DC: National
Institute of Law Enforcement and Criminal Justice.
Chicago Police Department (1970). Murder analysis, 1965-
1969. Chicago: Chicago Police Depart-
ment.
Conklin, J., and Bittner, E. (1973). Burglary in a suburb.
Criminology, 11:206-32.
Conklin, J. (1972). Robbery and the criminal justice system.
Philadelphia: J.B. Lippincott.
Curtis, L. A. (1974). Criminal violence: National patterns and
behavior. Lexington, MA: Lexington
Books.
Davis, E. M., and Knowles, L. (1975). An evaluation of the
Kansas City Preventive Patrol Experi-
ment. Police Chief, pp. 22-27.
Ehrlich, I. (1975). The deterrent effect of capital punishment: A
question of life and death. Ameri-
can economic review, 65~397-417.
Eliot, W.A.; Strack, J.R.; and Witter, A.E. (1975). Early
warning robbery reduction projects: An
assessment of performance. McLean, VA: The MITRE
Corporation.
Farmer, M. (1978). Personal communication.
Federal Bureau of Investigation (1976). Uniform Crime Report,
1976. Washington, DC: U.S. Gov-
ernment Printing Gflice.
Feeney, F. et al. (1973). The prevenrion and control of robbery.
(Five volumes). Davis, CA: Center
on Administration of Criminal Justice, University of California
at Davis.
Folk, J. (1971). Municipal detective systems: A quantitative
approach. Cambridge, MA: Operations
Research Center, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
Forst, B.; Lucianovic, J.; and Cox, S. (1977). What happens
after arrest? A court perspective of
police operations in the District of Columbia. Washington, DC:
Institute for Law and Social
Research.
240 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES
Gartt, H.; Flax. M.; Springer, M.; and Taylor, J. (1976). Models
for indicator development: A
framework for policy analysis. Washington, DC: The Urban
Institute.
Gay, W.; Schell, T.; and Schack, S. (1977). Improving patrol
productivity: Volume 1. Routine
patrol. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Greenberg, B.; Yu, 0.; and Lang, K. (1973). Enhancement of the
investigativefunction, Volume I.
NTIS # PB-222 895. Menlo Park, CA: Stanford Research
Institute.
Greenberg, B. et al. (1977). Felony investigation decision
model: An analysis of investigative
elements of information. Washington, DC: U.S. Government
Printing Office.
Greenwood, P. (1970). An analysis of the apprehension
activities of the New York City Police
Department. New York: The New York City RAND Institute.
Greenwood. P.W.; Chaiken, J.M.; Petersilia, J.; and Prusoff, L.
(1975). The criminal investigation
process; Volume III: Observations and analysis. Santa Monica,
CA: The RAND Corporation.
Hart, G. (1977). Foreword. In B. Greenberg et al., Felony
investigation decision model: An analysis
of investigative elements of information. Washington, DC: U.S.
Government Printing Office.
Hatry, H.P. (1975). Wrestling with police crime control
measurement. In J. L. Wolfe and J. A.
Heaphy (eds.) Readings on productivity in policing.
Washington, DC: The Police Foundation, 86
128.
Institute for Defense Analysis (1967). Task force report:
Science and technology, a report to the
President’s Commission on Law Enforcement and the
Administration of Justice. Washington, DC:
U.S. Government Printing Office.
Institute for Law and Social Research (1977). Expanding the
perspective of crime data: Perfor-
mance implications for policymakers. Washington, DC: U.S.
Government Printing Office.
lsaacs, H. (1%7). A study of communications, crimes, and
arrests in a metropolitan police depart-
ment. Appendix B of Institute for Defense Analyses, Task force
report: Science and technology, a
report to the President’s Commission on Law Enforcement and
the Administration of Justice.
Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office, pp. 88-106.
Jacob, H. (1978). Justice in America: Courts, lawyers, and the
judicial process, 3rd ed. Boston:
Little, Brown & Co.
Kansas City Police Department (1977). Response time analysis;
Vol. II. Analysis. Kansas City MO:
Board of Police Commissioners.
Kelling, G.; and Pate, T.; Dieckman, D.; and Brown, C.E.
(1974). The Kansas City Preventive
Patrol Experiment: A technical report. Washington, DC: The
Police-Foundation.
Larson, R. C. (1975). What happened to patrol operations in
Kansas City. Journal of criminal
justice. 31267-97.
Law Enforcement Assistance Administration (1975). Criminal
victimization in the United States:
1973 advance report. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of
Justice.
- (1976). Criminal victimization in the United States: A
comparison of 1973 and 1974jindings.
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice.
McClintock, F.H., and Gibson, E. (1960). Robbery,in London.
London: Macmillan.
McDonald, J. M. (1975). Armed robbery: Offenders and their
victims. Springfield, IL: Charles C.
Thomas.
National Academy of Sciences (1976). Surveying crime: Report
of the Panel for rhe Evaluation of
Crime Surveys. Washington, DC: Committee on National
Statistics, National Academy of Sciences.
Owen, G. (1970). Mathematics for the social and management
sciences: Finite mathematics. Phila-
delphia: W.B. Saunders.
Pate, T.; Bowers, R.; and Parks, R. (1976). Three approaches to
criminal apprehension in Kansas
City: An evaluation report. Washington, DC: The Police
Foundation.
Information, Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the
Limits of Police Productivity 241
Pittman, D. J., and Handy, W. (1964). Patterns in criminal
aggravated assault. Journ ofcriminal
law,, criminology and police science, 551462-70.
President’s Commission on Law Enforcement and
Administration of Justice (1%7). Task force
report: Crime and its assessment. Washington, DC: U.S.
Government Printing Office.
Reiss, A. (1971). The police and the public. New Haven, CT:
Yale University Press.
Repetto. T. A. (1974). Residential crime. Cambridge, MA:
Ballinger Publishing Co.
Rubinstein, J. (1973). City police. New York: Farrar, Straus and
Giroux.
Sagalyn, A. (1971). The crime ofrobbery in the United States.
Washington, DC: Law Enforcement
Assistance Administration, U.S. Department of Justice.
Sanders, W. (1977). Detective work: A study of criminal
investigations. New York: Free Press.
Seidman. D., and Couzens, M. (1975). Getting the crime rate
down: Political pressure and crime
reporting. Law and society review, 8:457-93.
Skogan, W. G. (197Q). Citizen reporting of crime: Some
National Panel data. Criminology, 1353%
49.
- (1976b). Crime and crime rates. In W. G. Skogan (ed.) Sample
surveys of the victims of
crime. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger Publishing Co., pp. 105-20.
- (1976~) Sample surveys of the victims of crime. Review of
public data use, 4:23-28.
- (1976d). The victims of crime: Some National Survey
findings. In A. L. Guenther (ed.)
Criminal behavior and social systems. Chicago: Rand McNally,
pp. 131-48.
Stinchcombe, A. L. (1963). Institutions of privacy in the
determination of police practices. Ameri-
can journal of sociology, 69: 150-60.
Tien. J.; Simon, J.; and Larson, R. (1978). An alternative
approach in police patrol: The Wilming
ton splif-force experiment. Washington, DC: U.S. Government
Printing Office.
Turner, A. (1972). San Jose methods test of known crime
victims. Washington, DC: Law Enforce-
ment Assistance Administration.
Vera Institute of Justice (1976). Felony arrests: Their
prosecution and disposition in New York
Cify’s courts. New York: Vera Institute of Justice.
Ward, R.; Ward, T.; and Feeley, J. (1975). Police robbery
control manual. Washington, DC:
National Institute of Law Enforcement and Criminal Justice.
Wolfgang, M. E. (1958). Patterns in criminal homicide.
Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania
Press.
- ( 1963). Uniform crime reports: A critical appraisal.
Universiw of Pennsylvania law review,
11:708-38.
Citation: 13 Am. J. Police 149 1994
Content downloaded/printed from
HeinOnline (http://heinonline.org)
Wed Jul 8 16:55:15 2015
-- Your use of this HeinOnline PDF indicates your acceptance
of HeinOnline's Terms and Conditions of the license
agreement available at http://heinonline.org/HOL/License
-- The search text of this PDF is generated from
uncorrected OCR text.
American Journal of Police Vol. XIII, No. 3 1994 149
THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN EVIDENCE,
DETECTIVE EFFORT, AND THE DISPOSITION
OF BURGLARY AND ROBBERY INVESTIGATIONS
Steven G. Brandl
University of Wisconsin - Milwaukee
James Frank
University of Cincinnati
The police detective is commonly cast by the media and de-
tective fiction as a highly skilled and intelligent investigator
who,
through the use of complex and extraordinary means, is capable
of
solving virtually any crime (Klockars, 1985; see also Garofalo,
1981).
The public, in turn, has largely accepted this portrayal as an
accurate
reflection of reality (Wilson, 1975). Further, the police
organization
reflects and perpetuates this institutionalized belief by dividing
the in-
vestigative task among detectives, who specialize in
investigating
crimes, and patrol officers, who perform the countless other
tasks asso-
ciated with police work.
With the appearance of empirical inquiries into the operations
of the criminal investigation process, the capabilities of
detectives in
investigating crimes have been directly questioned. However,
not only
have these studies been relatively few in number, but they have
also
produced contradictory findings. For example, Greenwood,
Chaiken,
and Petersilia (1977), explain that "detective work is not
characterized
by hard work leading to case solutions . . ." rather, "the cases
that get
cleared are primarily the easy ones to solve" (p. 112). Thus,
"any justi-
fication for the work of [detectives] must lie in areas other than
crime
solution" (p. 141). On the other hand, Eck (1983) argues that
"the work
of... detectives is extremely important with respect to the
subsequent
making of follow-up arrests" (p. 243). As a result of the
divergent
150 American Journal of Police Vol. XIII, No. 3 1994
findings, definitive conclusions concerning the contributions of
detec-
tives in solving crimes cannot be drawn.
The analyses reported in this study are intended to cast addi-
tional light on the debate concerning the extent to which
detective ef-
fort contributes to the identification and apprehension of
criminals.
Specifically, we test hypotheses that link burglary and robbery
follow-
up investigation arrests to the amount of time spent on follow-
up in-
vestigations and the strength of the evidence associated with the
cases.
The study addresses some shortcomings evident in the prior
research
and provides some empirical evidence where speculation has
gone be-
fore. In so doing, the study represents an extension of previous
re-
search.
PREVIOUS RESEARCH
In the first study to examine the contribution of detectives in
solving crimes, Isaacs (1967) analyzed data on 1,905 crimes
reported to
two of 15 Los Angeles Police Department field divisions in
January
1966. Isaacs found that of the 1,905 crimes, 336 (18%) resulted
in an
actual arrest while 146 (8%) resulted in "other clearances" (e.g.,
victim
refused to prosecute, unfounded). Of the 336 cases that resulted
in an
arrest, only 27 (8%) were as a result of an arrest by a detective.
Based
on the limited statistical analyses performed on the data, the
author
concluded that "[o]ne of the most significant characteristics of a
case,
affecting its chances of being cleared, is whether the suspect
was
named in the crime report by the victim" (p. 96). For example,
of the
1,905 crimes, 1,556 (82%) involved suspects who were not
named in
the initial crime report. Of these 1,556 cases, 1,375 (88%) were
not
cleared. Conversely, of the 1,905 crimes, 349 (18%) involved
suspects
who were named in the crime report. Of these cases, only 48
(14%)
were not cleared.
Greenwood (1970), in an analysis of burglary, robbery, and
grand larceny crimes that were reported to several precincts of
the New
York City Police Department during the first six months of
1967, also
found that when a suspect could be named by the victim, the
chances of
an arrest were significantly greater compared to when only a
descrip-
tion was provided, or other evidence was available. For
example, the
analyses showed that approximately nine percent of reported
robberies
resulted in an on-scene arrest while the remaining 91 percent
were ini-
American Journal of Police Vol. XIII, No. 3 1994 151
tially unsolved. Of the unsolved cases, the victim could name a
suspect
in about two percent and could describe the suspect in 67
percent. For
those cases with a named suspect, 46 percent resulted in an
arrest,
while only two percent of the cases with a description resulted
in an ar-
rest. Once again, circumstances-namely, the victim providing
the
suspect's name-were portrayed as determining the outcome of
the
case.
The study conducted by Greenwood, Chaiken, and Petersilia
of the RAND Corporation in 1977 supported, and elaborated on,
the
conclusions of Isaacs (1967) and Greenwood (1970). Their
results
were derived from analyses of primarily two sets of data. First,
based
on an analysis of the computer-readable case assignment file
main-
tained by the Kansas City (MO) Police Department, the
researchers
provided insight into how detectives spend their working time.
In
comparing the amount of time spent on uncleared cases with the
amount of time spent on cleared cases, they found that the "time
spent
on uncleared cases [was] significantly higher than time spent
prior to
clearance on cleared cases with no initial patrol arrest" (p.
115).1 On
the basis of this comparison, they conclude that there are
basically two
types of investigations: "Some investigations are lengthy but do
not re-
sult in a clearance. Others lead to a clearance after a small
amount of
work but entail substantial efforts by the investigator once the
crime is
cleared" (p. 116). Thus, "the cases that get cleared are primarily
the
easy ones to solve" (p. 112).
Second, a post hoc analysis of cleared crimes (homicide, ag-
gravated assault, felony morals, robbery, burglary, theft, auto
theft, and
forgery/fraud) from five police departments (Berkeley, Long
Beach,
Los Angeles, Miami, and Washington D.C.) 2 provided a means
by
which to describe the method in which cleared cases were
solved.
Greenwood et al. (1977) found that about 22 percent (38 of 172)
of the
cleared crimes resulted from an arrest by a patrol officer at the
scene of
the crime. In another 44 percent (76 of 172), the perpetrator was
"known" when the crime report was first taken and, as described
by the
researchers, "the main job for the investigator in these cases
[was] to
locate and apprehend the perpetrator, and to assemble evidence
ade-
quate to charge him" (p. 135). The remaining 34 percent (58 of
172) of
all cleared crimes were cleared primarily as a result of "simple
routine
actions" of detectives (e.g., showing mug shot books, acting on
precise
and detailed informant tips). According to Greenwood et al.
(1977),
only about three percent of these cleared crimes could be
attributed to
152 American Journal of Police Vol. XIII, No. 3 1994
"special actions" of detectives (e.g., preparation of investigative
bul-
letins, matching latent prints to inked prints in a file organized
by
M.O.).
Like Isaacs (1967) and Greenwood (1970), Greenwood et al.
(1977) explain that the overwhelming majority of solved crimes
are
solved because of arrests by patrol officers or because the
identity of
the perpetrator is known when the crime report reached the
detective.
"If information that uniquely identifies the perpetrator is not
presented
at the time the crime is reported, the perpetrator, by and large,
will not
be subsequently identified" (Greenwood, Chaiken, and
Petersilia,
1976:65). Thus, "it is not true in general that the greater the
investiga-
tive effort devoted to a crime the more likely it will be cleared"
(p. 64).
Eck (1983) provides contrast with Isaacs (1967), Greenwood
(1970), and Greenwood et al. (1977) by emphasizing, or at least
not
minimizing, the importance of detective effort in solving
crimes. Us-
ing data collected from three police departments (DeKalb Co.,
GA, St.
Petersburg, FL, and Wichita, KS), he examined activities
performed by
detectives and patrol officers to produce crime and/or suspect
informa-
tion, and the relationship between this information and a
suspect being
arrested. The study focused on burglaries and robberies. Eck
found
that certain types of information produced during the
preliminary in-
vestigation (i.e., information pertaining to the suspect) were
predictive
of whether or not an arrest was made during the follow-up
investigation
as were some of the activities performed by detectives during
the fol-
low-up investigation (e.g., record checks, informant interviews).
As
such, Eck provides evidence to suggest that circumstances of
criminal
incidents as well as efforts of detectives contribute to the
making of ar-
rests during follow-up investigations.
On the basis of his analyses, Eck (1983) proposed what he re-
ferred to as the "triage hypothesis" (see also Eck, 1979).
According to
this hypothesis, cases can be grouped into three categories
depending
on the strength of the evidence (i.e., favorable circumstances)
associ-
ated with the case. The hypothesis predicts that, first, cases
with weak
evidence will rarely conclude with an arrest, regardless of the
inves-
tigative effort or time allocated to them. Second, cases with
moderate
evidence, where there are sufficient leads to suggest that the
cases
could be solved, will often result in an arrest but only if effort
is allo-
cated to them. Third, cases with strong evidence, where the
suspect has
been positively identified, will usually result in an arrest, even
with
only minimal investigative effort. Accordingly, based on this
hypothe-
American Journal of Police Vol. XIII, No. 3 1994 153
sis, circumstances determine the outcome of the cases in the
first and
third group and thus, for these cases, the probability of an arrest
does
not increase with more time spent. However, effort determines
the out-
come of the cases in the second group and as a result, for these
cases,
the probability of an arrest does increase with more time spent
on the
investigation. Therefore, like Greenwood et al. (1977), Eck
implies
that, in general, the likelihood of an arrest may not increase
with more
effort devoted to the case. However, Eck (1983) suggests that
this re-
lationship--that more time spent on an investigation increases
the
probability of an arrest--does hold true for cases with moderate
suspect information. And, as hypothesized by Eck (1979), it is
cases in
this second category that have the most time devoted to them.
There are several issues that need to be considered when
judging the overall validity of the conclusions offered by the
previous
research. First, Isaacs (1967) and some of the analyses
performed by
Greenwood et al. (1977:97-119) used whether cases were
"cleared" as
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx
Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx

More Related Content

Similar to Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx

Field%20 Operations[1]
Field%20 Operations[1]Field%20 Operations[1]
Field%20 Operations[1]taylor024519
 
The Importance Of Intelligence-Led Policing
The Importance Of Intelligence-Led PolicingThe Importance Of Intelligence-Led Policing
The Importance Of Intelligence-Led PolicingMelissa Dudas
 
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACASantosConleyha
 
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACABenitoSumpter862
 
Merseyside Crime Analysis
Merseyside Crime AnalysisMerseyside Crime Analysis
Merseyside Crime AnalysisParang Saraf
 
Unit 9 Hall Elizabeth Investigation Essay
Unit 9 Hall Elizabeth Investigation EssayUnit 9 Hall Elizabeth Investigation Essay
Unit 9 Hall Elizabeth Investigation EssayElizabeth Hall
 
Mapping Business Challenges to Types of Control © 201.docx
Mapping Business Challenges to Types of Control © 201.docxMapping Business Challenges to Types of Control © 201.docx
Mapping Business Challenges to Types of Control © 201.docxgertrudebellgrove
 
Literature Review for third party crime reporting.pdf
Literature Review for third party crime reporting.pdfLiterature Review for third party crime reporting.pdf
Literature Review for third party crime reporting.pdfsdfghj21
 
3Victimization inthe United StatesAn OverviewCHAPTE.docx
3Victimization inthe United StatesAn OverviewCHAPTE.docx3Victimization inthe United StatesAn OverviewCHAPTE.docx
3Victimization inthe United StatesAn OverviewCHAPTE.docxlorainedeserre
 
Police and crime trends
Police and crime trendsPolice and crime trends
Police and crime trendsRoel Palmaers
 
Crime Rate And Crime Rates
Crime Rate And Crime RatesCrime Rate And Crime Rates
Crime Rate And Crime RatesKrystal Green
 
Example Of Predictive Policing
Example Of Predictive PolicingExample Of Predictive Policing
Example Of Predictive PolicingSherry Bailey
 
Crime prediction based on crime types
Crime prediction based on crime typesCrime prediction based on crime types
Crime prediction based on crime typesIJDKP
 
Effectiveness of Crime Control Measures in Nigeria
Effectiveness of Crime Control Measures in NigeriaEffectiveness of Crime Control Measures in Nigeria
Effectiveness of Crime Control Measures in NigeriaGabriel Ken
 
KKeith - Writing Sample 3
KKeith - Writing Sample 3KKeith - Writing Sample 3
KKeith - Writing Sample 3Kresenda Keith
 

Similar to Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx (15)

Field%20 Operations[1]
Field%20 Operations[1]Field%20 Operations[1]
Field%20 Operations[1]
 
The Importance Of Intelligence-Led Policing
The Importance Of Intelligence-Led PolicingThe Importance Of Intelligence-Led Policing
The Importance Of Intelligence-Led Policing
 
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
 
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
10.11770002716203262548 ARTICLETHE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACA
 
Merseyside Crime Analysis
Merseyside Crime AnalysisMerseyside Crime Analysis
Merseyside Crime Analysis
 
Unit 9 Hall Elizabeth Investigation Essay
Unit 9 Hall Elizabeth Investigation EssayUnit 9 Hall Elizabeth Investigation Essay
Unit 9 Hall Elizabeth Investigation Essay
 
Mapping Business Challenges to Types of Control © 201.docx
Mapping Business Challenges to Types of Control © 201.docxMapping Business Challenges to Types of Control © 201.docx
Mapping Business Challenges to Types of Control © 201.docx
 
Literature Review for third party crime reporting.pdf
Literature Review for third party crime reporting.pdfLiterature Review for third party crime reporting.pdf
Literature Review for third party crime reporting.pdf
 
3Victimization inthe United StatesAn OverviewCHAPTE.docx
3Victimization inthe United StatesAn OverviewCHAPTE.docx3Victimization inthe United StatesAn OverviewCHAPTE.docx
3Victimization inthe United StatesAn OverviewCHAPTE.docx
 
Police and crime trends
Police and crime trendsPolice and crime trends
Police and crime trends
 
Crime Rate And Crime Rates
Crime Rate And Crime RatesCrime Rate And Crime Rates
Crime Rate And Crime Rates
 
Example Of Predictive Policing
Example Of Predictive PolicingExample Of Predictive Policing
Example Of Predictive Policing
 
Crime prediction based on crime types
Crime prediction based on crime typesCrime prediction based on crime types
Crime prediction based on crime types
 
Effectiveness of Crime Control Measures in Nigeria
Effectiveness of Crime Control Measures in NigeriaEffectiveness of Crime Control Measures in Nigeria
Effectiveness of Crime Control Measures in Nigeria
 
KKeith - Writing Sample 3
KKeith - Writing Sample 3KKeith - Writing Sample 3
KKeith - Writing Sample 3
 

More from priestmanmable

9©iStockphotoThinkstockPlanning for Material and Reso.docx
9©iStockphotoThinkstockPlanning for Material and Reso.docx9©iStockphotoThinkstockPlanning for Material and Reso.docx
9©iStockphotoThinkstockPlanning for Material and Reso.docxpriestmanmable
 
a 12 page paper on how individuals of color would be a more dominant.docx
a 12 page paper on how individuals of color would be a more dominant.docxa 12 page paper on how individuals of color would be a more dominant.docx
a 12 page paper on how individuals of color would be a more dominant.docxpriestmanmable
 
978-1-5386-6589-318$31.00 ©2018 IEEE COSO Framework for .docx
978-1-5386-6589-318$31.00 ©2018 IEEE COSO Framework for .docx978-1-5386-6589-318$31.00 ©2018 IEEE COSO Framework for .docx
978-1-5386-6589-318$31.00 ©2018 IEEE COSO Framework for .docxpriestmanmable
 
92 Academic Journal Article Critique  Help with Journal Ar.docx
92 Academic Journal Article Critique  Help with Journal Ar.docx92 Academic Journal Article Critique  Help with Journal Ar.docx
92 Academic Journal Article Critique  Help with Journal Ar.docxpriestmanmable
 
A ) Society perspective90 year old female, Mrs. Ruth, from h.docx
A ) Society perspective90 year old female, Mrs. Ruth, from h.docxA ) Society perspective90 year old female, Mrs. Ruth, from h.docx
A ) Society perspective90 year old female, Mrs. Ruth, from h.docxpriestmanmable
 
9 dissuasion question Bartol, C. R., & Bartol, A. M. (2017)..docx
9 dissuasion question Bartol, C. R., & Bartol, A. M. (2017)..docx9 dissuasion question Bartol, C. R., & Bartol, A. M. (2017)..docx
9 dissuasion question Bartol, C. R., & Bartol, A. M. (2017)..docxpriestmanmable
 
9 AssignmentAssignment Typologies of Sexual AssaultsT.docx
9 AssignmentAssignment Typologies of Sexual AssaultsT.docx9 AssignmentAssignment Typologies of Sexual AssaultsT.docx
9 AssignmentAssignment Typologies of Sexual AssaultsT.docxpriestmanmable
 
9 0 0 0 09 7 8 0 1 3 4 4 7 7 4 0 4ISBN-13 978-0-13-44.docx
9 0 0 0 09 7 8 0 1 3 4 4 7 7 4 0 4ISBN-13 978-0-13-44.docx9 0 0 0 09 7 8 0 1 3 4 4 7 7 4 0 4ISBN-13 978-0-13-44.docx
9 0 0 0 09 7 8 0 1 3 4 4 7 7 4 0 4ISBN-13 978-0-13-44.docxpriestmanmable
 
900 BritishJournalofNursing,2013,Vol22,No15©2.docx
900 BritishJournalofNursing,2013,Vol22,No15©2.docx900 BritishJournalofNursing,2013,Vol22,No15©2.docx
900 BritishJournalofNursing,2013,Vol22,No15©2.docxpriestmanmable
 
9 Augustine Confessions (selections) Augustine of Hi.docx
9 Augustine Confessions (selections) Augustine of Hi.docx9 Augustine Confessions (selections) Augustine of Hi.docx
9 Augustine Confessions (selections) Augustine of Hi.docxpriestmanmable
 
8.3 Intercultural CommunicationLearning Objectives1. Define in.docx
8.3 Intercultural CommunicationLearning Objectives1. Define in.docx8.3 Intercultural CommunicationLearning Objectives1. Define in.docx
8.3 Intercultural CommunicationLearning Objectives1. Define in.docxpriestmanmable
 
8413 906 AMLife in a Toxic Country - NYTimes.comPage 1 .docx
8413 906 AMLife in a Toxic Country - NYTimes.comPage 1 .docx8413 906 AMLife in a Toxic Country - NYTimes.comPage 1 .docx
8413 906 AMLife in a Toxic Country - NYTimes.comPage 1 .docxpriestmanmable
 
8. A 2 x 2 Experimental Design - Quality and Economy (x1 and x2.docx
8. A 2 x 2 Experimental Design - Quality and Economy (x1 and x2.docx8. A 2 x 2 Experimental Design - Quality and Economy (x1 and x2.docx
8. A 2 x 2 Experimental Design - Quality and Economy (x1 and x2.docxpriestmanmable
 
800 Words 42-year-old man presents to ED with 2-day history .docx
800 Words 42-year-old man presents to ED with 2-day history .docx800 Words 42-year-old man presents to ED with 2-day history .docx
800 Words 42-year-old man presents to ED with 2-day history .docxpriestmanmable
 
8.1 What Is Corporate StrategyLO 8-1Define corporate strategy.docx
8.1 What Is Corporate StrategyLO 8-1Define corporate strategy.docx8.1 What Is Corporate StrategyLO 8-1Define corporate strategy.docx
8.1 What Is Corporate StrategyLO 8-1Define corporate strategy.docxpriestmanmable
 
8.0 RESEARCH METHODS These guidelines address postgr.docx
8.0  RESEARCH METHODS  These guidelines address postgr.docx8.0  RESEARCH METHODS  These guidelines address postgr.docx
8.0 RESEARCH METHODS These guidelines address postgr.docxpriestmanmable
 
95People of AppalachianHeritageChapter 5KATHLEEN.docx
95People of AppalachianHeritageChapter 5KATHLEEN.docx95People of AppalachianHeritageChapter 5KATHLEEN.docx
95People of AppalachianHeritageChapter 5KATHLEEN.docxpriestmanmable
 
9 781292 041452ISBN 978-1-29204-145-2Forensic Science.docx
9 781292 041452ISBN 978-1-29204-145-2Forensic Science.docx9 781292 041452ISBN 978-1-29204-145-2Forensic Science.docx
9 781292 041452ISBN 978-1-29204-145-2Forensic Science.docxpriestmanmable
 
8-10 slide Powerpoint The example company is Tesla.Instructions.docx
8-10 slide Powerpoint The example company is Tesla.Instructions.docx8-10 slide Powerpoint The example company is Tesla.Instructions.docx
8-10 slide Powerpoint The example company is Tesla.Instructions.docxpriestmanmable
 
8Network Security April 2020FEATUREAre your IT staf.docx
8Network Security  April 2020FEATUREAre your IT staf.docx8Network Security  April 2020FEATUREAre your IT staf.docx
8Network Security April 2020FEATUREAre your IT staf.docxpriestmanmable
 

More from priestmanmable (20)

9©iStockphotoThinkstockPlanning for Material and Reso.docx
9©iStockphotoThinkstockPlanning for Material and Reso.docx9©iStockphotoThinkstockPlanning for Material and Reso.docx
9©iStockphotoThinkstockPlanning for Material and Reso.docx
 
a 12 page paper on how individuals of color would be a more dominant.docx
a 12 page paper on how individuals of color would be a more dominant.docxa 12 page paper on how individuals of color would be a more dominant.docx
a 12 page paper on how individuals of color would be a more dominant.docx
 
978-1-5386-6589-318$31.00 ©2018 IEEE COSO Framework for .docx
978-1-5386-6589-318$31.00 ©2018 IEEE COSO Framework for .docx978-1-5386-6589-318$31.00 ©2018 IEEE COSO Framework for .docx
978-1-5386-6589-318$31.00 ©2018 IEEE COSO Framework for .docx
 
92 Academic Journal Article Critique  Help with Journal Ar.docx
92 Academic Journal Article Critique  Help with Journal Ar.docx92 Academic Journal Article Critique  Help with Journal Ar.docx
92 Academic Journal Article Critique  Help with Journal Ar.docx
 
A ) Society perspective90 year old female, Mrs. Ruth, from h.docx
A ) Society perspective90 year old female, Mrs. Ruth, from h.docxA ) Society perspective90 year old female, Mrs. Ruth, from h.docx
A ) Society perspective90 year old female, Mrs. Ruth, from h.docx
 
9 dissuasion question Bartol, C. R., & Bartol, A. M. (2017)..docx
9 dissuasion question Bartol, C. R., & Bartol, A. M. (2017)..docx9 dissuasion question Bartol, C. R., & Bartol, A. M. (2017)..docx
9 dissuasion question Bartol, C. R., & Bartol, A. M. (2017)..docx
 
9 AssignmentAssignment Typologies of Sexual AssaultsT.docx
9 AssignmentAssignment Typologies of Sexual AssaultsT.docx9 AssignmentAssignment Typologies of Sexual AssaultsT.docx
9 AssignmentAssignment Typologies of Sexual AssaultsT.docx
 
9 0 0 0 09 7 8 0 1 3 4 4 7 7 4 0 4ISBN-13 978-0-13-44.docx
9 0 0 0 09 7 8 0 1 3 4 4 7 7 4 0 4ISBN-13 978-0-13-44.docx9 0 0 0 09 7 8 0 1 3 4 4 7 7 4 0 4ISBN-13 978-0-13-44.docx
9 0 0 0 09 7 8 0 1 3 4 4 7 7 4 0 4ISBN-13 978-0-13-44.docx
 
900 BritishJournalofNursing,2013,Vol22,No15©2.docx
900 BritishJournalofNursing,2013,Vol22,No15©2.docx900 BritishJournalofNursing,2013,Vol22,No15©2.docx
900 BritishJournalofNursing,2013,Vol22,No15©2.docx
 
9 Augustine Confessions (selections) Augustine of Hi.docx
9 Augustine Confessions (selections) Augustine of Hi.docx9 Augustine Confessions (selections) Augustine of Hi.docx
9 Augustine Confessions (selections) Augustine of Hi.docx
 
8.3 Intercultural CommunicationLearning Objectives1. Define in.docx
8.3 Intercultural CommunicationLearning Objectives1. Define in.docx8.3 Intercultural CommunicationLearning Objectives1. Define in.docx
8.3 Intercultural CommunicationLearning Objectives1. Define in.docx
 
8413 906 AMLife in a Toxic Country - NYTimes.comPage 1 .docx
8413 906 AMLife in a Toxic Country - NYTimes.comPage 1 .docx8413 906 AMLife in a Toxic Country - NYTimes.comPage 1 .docx
8413 906 AMLife in a Toxic Country - NYTimes.comPage 1 .docx
 
8. A 2 x 2 Experimental Design - Quality and Economy (x1 and x2.docx
8. A 2 x 2 Experimental Design - Quality and Economy (x1 and x2.docx8. A 2 x 2 Experimental Design - Quality and Economy (x1 and x2.docx
8. A 2 x 2 Experimental Design - Quality and Economy (x1 and x2.docx
 
800 Words 42-year-old man presents to ED with 2-day history .docx
800 Words 42-year-old man presents to ED with 2-day history .docx800 Words 42-year-old man presents to ED with 2-day history .docx
800 Words 42-year-old man presents to ED with 2-day history .docx
 
8.1 What Is Corporate StrategyLO 8-1Define corporate strategy.docx
8.1 What Is Corporate StrategyLO 8-1Define corporate strategy.docx8.1 What Is Corporate StrategyLO 8-1Define corporate strategy.docx
8.1 What Is Corporate StrategyLO 8-1Define corporate strategy.docx
 
8.0 RESEARCH METHODS These guidelines address postgr.docx
8.0  RESEARCH METHODS  These guidelines address postgr.docx8.0  RESEARCH METHODS  These guidelines address postgr.docx
8.0 RESEARCH METHODS These guidelines address postgr.docx
 
95People of AppalachianHeritageChapter 5KATHLEEN.docx
95People of AppalachianHeritageChapter 5KATHLEEN.docx95People of AppalachianHeritageChapter 5KATHLEEN.docx
95People of AppalachianHeritageChapter 5KATHLEEN.docx
 
9 781292 041452ISBN 978-1-29204-145-2Forensic Science.docx
9 781292 041452ISBN 978-1-29204-145-2Forensic Science.docx9 781292 041452ISBN 978-1-29204-145-2Forensic Science.docx
9 781292 041452ISBN 978-1-29204-145-2Forensic Science.docx
 
8-10 slide Powerpoint The example company is Tesla.Instructions.docx
8-10 slide Powerpoint The example company is Tesla.Instructions.docx8-10 slide Powerpoint The example company is Tesla.Instructions.docx
8-10 slide Powerpoint The example company is Tesla.Instructions.docx
 
8Network Security April 2020FEATUREAre your IT staf.docx
8Network Security  April 2020FEATUREAre your IT staf.docx8Network Security  April 2020FEATUREAre your IT staf.docx
8Network Security April 2020FEATUREAre your IT staf.docx
 

Recently uploaded

1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdfQucHHunhnh
 
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptxRole Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptxNikitaBankoti2
 
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptxUnit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptxVishalSingh1417
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxUnit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxVishalSingh1417
 
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.christianmathematics
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsTechSoup
 
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDMeasures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDThiyagu K
 
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...christianmathematics
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingTechSoup
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104misteraugie
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfJayanti Pande
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfAyushMahapatra5
 
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibitjbellavia9
 
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxBasic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxDenish Jangid
 
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdfMaking and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdfChris Hunter
 
Food Chain and Food Web (Ecosystem) EVS, B. Pharmacy 1st Year, Sem-II
Food Chain and Food Web (Ecosystem) EVS, B. Pharmacy 1st Year, Sem-IIFood Chain and Food Web (Ecosystem) EVS, B. Pharmacy 1st Year, Sem-II
Food Chain and Food Web (Ecosystem) EVS, B. Pharmacy 1st Year, Sem-IIShubhangi Sonawane
 

Recently uploaded (20)

1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptxRole Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
 
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptxUnit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
 
Asian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptx
Asian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptxAsian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptx
Asian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptx
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxUnit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
 
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
 
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDMeasures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
 
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
 
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
 
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxBasic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
 
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdfMaking and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
 
Food Chain and Food Web (Ecosystem) EVS, B. Pharmacy 1st Year, Sem-II
Food Chain and Food Web (Ecosystem) EVS, B. Pharmacy 1st Year, Sem-IIFood Chain and Food Web (Ecosystem) EVS, B. Pharmacy 1st Year, Sem-II
Food Chain and Food Web (Ecosystem) EVS, B. Pharmacy 1st Year, Sem-II
 

Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Per.docx

  • 1. Journal of-Criminal Justice, Vol. 7, pp. 217-241 (1979). Pergamon Press. Printed in U.S.A. 0047-2352’79/030217-26s2.WO Copyright @ 1979 Pergamon Press Ltd INFORMATION, APPREHENSION, AND DETERRENCE: EXPLORING THE LIMITS OF POLICE PRODUCTIVITY WESLEY G. SKOGAN Department of Political Science and Center for Urban Affairs Northwestern University Evanston. Illinois 60201 GEORGE E. ANTUNES Department of Political Science University of Houston Houston, Texas 77004 and Workshop in Political Theory and Policy Analysis Indiana University Bloomington, Indiana 47401 ABSTRACT The capacity of police departments to solve crimes and apprehend offenders is low for many types of crime, particu- larly crimes of profit. This article reviews a variety of studies
  • 2. of police apprehension and hypothesizes that an important determinant of the ability of the police to apprehend crimi- nals is information. The complete absence of information for many types of crime places fairly clear upper bounds on the ability of the police to effect solutions. To discover whether these boundaries are high or low we analyzed data from the 1973 National Crime Panel about the types and amount of information potentially available to po- lice through victim reports and patrol activities. The evidence suggests that if the police rely on information made readily 217 218 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES available to them, they will never do much better than they are doing now. On the other hand, there appears to be more information available to bystanders and passing patrols than currently is being used, which suggests that surveillance strategies and improved police methods for eliciting, record- ing, and analyzing information supplied by victims and wit- nesses might increase the probability of solving crimes and making arrests. In light of this we review a few possibly help- ful innovations suggested in the literature on police produc- tivity and procedure. Some characteristics of the crime itself, or of events surrounding the crime, that are beyond the control of investigators, determine whether it will be cleared in most in- stances. (Greenwood et al., 1975: 65) There is no feasible way to solve most crimes except by
  • 3. securing the cooperation of citizens to link a person to the crime. (Reiss, 1971: 105) INTRODUCTION A recent spate of studies of crime and the deterrent effectiveness of the criminal justice system has raised anew a question as old as Bentham: Does raising the cost of criminal activity signiticantly reduce the level of crime in a community? In these studies, the cost of criminal activity has been conceptualized in two ways: as the loss of time and opportunity attendant to apprehension (measured by the certainty of arrest or punish- ment), and as the stigma, discomfort, and loss of opportunity that come with conviction by the courts (measured by the severity of punishment). Indicators of the distribution of these costs imposed by the criminal justice system are related in these studies to mea- sures of the crime rate in a sample of jurisdictions by employing their variation across areas as surrogates for different policy interventions by the state. The findings are then addressed to the effectiveness of those “policies.” (For a prominent example of this sort of analysis, focusing on the death penalty, see Ehrlich, 1975.) The studies differ in their choice of indicators for costs, as well as in their unit of analysis, sources for data, statistical design, and the decision rule by which the deterrence hypothesis will be confirmed or rejected. Not surprisingly, they differ in their conclusions as well.
  • 4. This article sidesteps the issue of the effectiveness of deterrence policies and fo- cuses instead on one crucial link in the process-the ability of the police to gather evidence, solve crimes, and make arrests. Any measure of the cost of crime ultimately rests on this activity, yet there has been little effort to assess the apprehension activities of the police within a deterrence perspective. The deterrent effect of the severity of sanctions (using as indicators either statutory maximums or sentences actually imposed) or the impact of court conviction (employing as an indicator the ratio of convictions to crimes known to the police) is mediated by the ability of the criminal justice system to Information. Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the Limits of Police Productivity 219 identify and apprehend individual offenders in the first place. If, as one reasonable line of deterrence research indicates (Aniunes and Hunt, 1973). certainty of punishment is more important than severity, the importance of the problem becomes even clearer. Even the issue of justice points to the centrality of certainty, for it is the notion that harsh sentences may be imposed on an unfortunate few who happen to be caught that so offends our sensibilities. As we shall see, the plain fact is that for many types of crimes, particularly those motivated by profit, the probability that an arrest will be made is very
  • 5. low. Explaining why this is so is one of the main goals of this article. We argue that an important determinant of the ability of the police to apprehend criminals is information. The police enjoy varying amounts of information in criminal cases: in some they have positive identifications to go on (victims or witnesses can tell them who did it and, frequently, where that person lives), in others they can gamer only rough descriptions of offenders, and in some they have only physical evidence or traces of the methods employed by perpetrators to guide their investigations. Police officers on patrol can spot certain offenses in progress, while in other cases they must await the reports of bystanders or victims themselves. All of these affect the ability of police to solve crimes, and we argue that the absence of information in many cases, coupled with the practical invisibility of many (but fewer) cases to official scrutiny, places limits on the ability of the police to do anything. In an attempt to discover where those limits are high and low, we employ data from the 1973 national victimization survey conducted by the U.S. Census Bureau to analyze the types and amounts of information potentially available to the police through victim reports and patrol activities. As we shall see, the evidence suggests that if the police rely on information made readily available to them, they never will do much better than they are doing now. On the other hand, there appears to be more
  • 6. information available to bystanders and passing patrols than currently is being used, which suggests that surveil- lance strategies and improved police methods for eliciting, recording, and analyzing information supplied by victims and witnesses might serve to raise the probability of solving crimes and making arrests. INFORMATION AS A DETERMINANT OF ARREST: PREVIOUS RESEARCH Crimes differ in the extent to which they admit of solution. Greenwood’s study of crime in New York City found that only 4 percent of burglaries and larcenies, 8 percent of auto thefts, and 13 percent of robberies were cleared by at least one arrest (1970:24). The probability of arrest for crimes of passion was higher, ranging from 46 percent for assault to 71 percent for homicide. Other scholars have reported similar findings in other cities (e.g., Reiss, 1971). We believe that the availability and reliability of information about incidents and offenders plays a key role in determining the ability of the police to solve crimes and apprehend offenders. The most easily solved crimes are those in which the victim or others close to the act can identify an alleged offender. For this to occur, there must be some interaction between the victim or the victim’s friends and a suspect. Furtive crimes that occur in the dead of night or in private places when the victim is not
  • 7. present are unlikely to produce either witnesses or useful information of any type. For example, burglary typically involves no contact between victim and offender, is devoid 220 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES of tangible clues, and usually is not even discovered until hours after the burglar has made an exit. Except during a (usually brief) intrusion period, the event takes place out of the public eye and screened from the sight of passing police patrols. As a result, burglary is a crime that is virtually impossible to solve, and the role of the police in responding to it generally is one of record keeping and sympathetic commiseration with victims’ (Repetto, 1974; Conklin and Bittner, 1973). In contrast, robbery offers a great deal of information that potentially is helpful in identifying and apprehending the rob- ber: There is a direct confrontation between the parties and thus the offender can be described and perhaps identified, weapons used may be described, a getaway vehicle may be observed and traced, and robberies usually are reported promptly so there is a chance that the offender may be apprehended near the scene of the crime (Conklin, 1972; McDonald, 1975). Several types of offenses feature at least limited interaction between suspect and victim, and they all enjoy relatively high rates of apprehension.
  • 8. This relationship ranges from modest (personal theft, including purse snatching) to intense (rapes, assaults, and murder). Robbery lies in the middle of the spectrum. Interaction may be intense and victim observation of the suspect considerable, but the typical robbery takes only 60 to 90 seconds to complete (Eliot, Strack, and Witter, 1975). Few robberies take place between acquaintances. Curtis (1974) sampled police files for 1967 in seventeen cities and reports that about 86 percent of all robberies found there were attributed to strangers. Thus, in most cases descriptions of the suspect’s appearance and behavior, although clouded with some uncertainty, constitute the major clues available to the police in robberies. “Robbery, more than any other type of street crime, requires the use of departmental intelligence (“M.O.“) files designed to identify suspects” (Greenwood, 1970). The face-to-face confrontation between robber and victim, while failing of positive identification, enhances arrest probabilities. * It also gives us more extensive research data on the personal characteristics of robbers such as race, sex, and apparent age, than on other “criminals for profit” (Sagalyn, 1971). In contrast to robbery, victims and witnesses of rape, assault, and homicide often possess knowledge of the offender’s identity. In many reported cases victim and of- fender are at least acquainted before the event. Amir’s (1971) study of rape in Philadel- phia indicated that 58 percent of rapes involved individuals who
  • 9. knew one another under other circumstances, while the special study of crime in Washington, D.C. conducted for the President’s Commission on Law Enforcement and Administration of Justice (1967) estimated an “acquaintance rate” for rape of 64 percent. Curtis’ (1974) study of police files in seventeen cities set that figure at 47 percent. Acquaintance rates for homicide and aggravated assault usually are higher. Marvin Wolfgang (1958) reported that 86 percent of the murder victims in his sample from Philadelphia were acquainted with their slayer, and the President’s Commission study in Washington, D.C. reported 70 percent; Curtis (1974) set the figure at 84 percent. A study of homicide in Chicago for the years 1965 to 1969 found that 71 percent of victims and offenders were so associated, and that in 21 percent of the cases the immediate parties were related by blood (Chicago Police Department, 1970). Nationally, only 23 percent of murders known to the police in 1975 were “felony type,” involving other criminal acts such as robbery or burglary (another 9 percent were thought to possibly belong in this category), about the same percentage as those involving spouses, parents, and other information, Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the Limits of Police Productivity 221 relatives (Federal Bureau of Investigation, 1976). Curtis (1974)
  • 10. attributed 25 percent of all homicides to relatives, and an additional 9 percent to close friends and lovers. Aggra- vated assaults also are crimes of passion, and many occur within acquaintance networks that allow positive identification when they are reported to the police. Acquaintance rates for assaults known to the police range from 65 to 80 percent, and several studies based on police files have estimated that 20 percent take place within the immediate family (Curtis, 1974; President’s Commission, 1967; Pittman and Handy, 1964). Reported rapes, on the other hand, rarely are family affairs: Amir’s study (1971) attributed only 2.5 percent to related parties, while Curtis (1974) attributed 7 percent. Descriptions or positive identifications of offenders are important to the criminal justice system, for criminal acts are more likely to generate arrests if the “circle of investigation” can be decreased by attaching a name or label to a suspect. The circle of investigation defines a pool of possible offenders who must be considered in designating a set of plausible suspects. Any information such as the age, race, or sex of the offender tightens the circle of persons who must be sorted through to identify likely suspects. A national victimization survey conducted for the Law Enforcement Assistance Adminis- tration (1975) indicated that only about one-third of all personal crimes of violence were committed by persons who were known to the victim and could be identified (presuma-
  • 11. bly) immediately. Thus, in two-thirds of personal crimes of violence the circle of investi- gation is fairly large and the task of identifying an assailant is rather arduous. In these instances the cooperation of victims and witnesses is crucial since even simple informa- tion about the physical characteristics of offenders aids in reducing the circle of investi- gation and identifying suspects.3 If a crime were committed by someone known to the victim, the circle of investiga- tion would encompass only one person, and the job of the police would be reduced to finding the suspect. This may account for the extremely high clearance rate for homicide even though victims themselves are not in a position to shed much light on the matter. I Many murder suspects even fail to flee the scene of the-crime, due perhaps to the bond between the parties. A Chicago murder analysis (Chicago Police Department, 1970) revealed a high incidence of what local police call “smoking gun” homicides: 35 percent of all murders during the 196% 1969 period were cleared when responding patrol officers arrested a suspect at the site (much the same obtains in Philadelphia-see Rubenstein, 1973:343). An additional 20 percent were cataloged as “known but flown” cases, those in which the suspect was not present but proved immediately identifiable by witnesses, neighbors, bystanders, or family members. The frequency of murder, fortunately, is relatively low. Crimes other than homicide
  • 12. in which virtually no information is available to the police (or at least almost none is recorded by police) are much more common. In a recent study of the disposition of cases by the police in Rochester, New York, conducted for the Police Foundation, Bloch and Bell (1976) judged the utility of suspect and property identifications contained in police reports on burglary and robbery cases. They found descriptions of suspects nonexistent in 68 percent of all burglary reports, but in only 3 percent of robbery reports. (This does not mean that information on robbers was all that good-the modal category for those data was “vague.“) Thus, for many common crimes the amount of information available in police records may be so low as to render unrealistic the expectation that offenders will be identified, arrested, and brought to trial. 222 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES INFORMATION: NEW DATA Previous research on the information problem faced by participants in the criminal justice system has been based largely on data extracted from local police files. Given the irregular and often unreliable nature of that data, it is remarkable how regular the emerging picture remains. Victimization surveys now being conducted by the Census Bureau provide a new
  • 13. perspective on these same problems. Victim surveys were designed to bypass the organ- izational and procedural difficulties involved in collecting reliable national crime data through police departments (see Wolfgang, 1963). Surveys acquire, directly from vic- tims, reports of events that .can be analyzed with some confidence (for a description of these surveys and a critical appraisal of their methodology see Skogan, 1976~; National Academy of Sciences, 1976). While published official reports based on these data have emphasized the frequency of various types of victimization (e.g., Law Enforcement Assistance Administration, 1975 and 1976), the surveys themselves gather extensive data on detailed characteristics of incidents and victims’ recollections of their perpetrators. These data can be analyzed to reveal under what circumstances useful information appears to be available to the criminal justice system, and the frequency with which information about the very existence of criminal events remains hidden from the police and the courts. The data analyzed in Table 1 were collected through interviews with a national panel of approximately 132,000 people. They were questioned every six months about events occurring during the previous six months for periods between February 1973 and June 1974 to produce estimates of the yearly distribution of crime for calendar 1973 (for a description, see Argana, 1975). The data are analyzed here to uncover “who knows
  • 14. what” about criminals that might be useful to police, improve apprehension rates, and promote the deterrent consequences of that activity. Table 1 explores the distribution of “don’t know” responses to a series of questions asked of all victims uncovered in the survey about their perpetrators. This allows us to judge the frequency with which descriptions of offenders are potentially available to the police.4 Like research based on police files, the victim data indicate that the most important distinction between crimes is the duration of personal contact between the parties. Interpersonal violence (assaults and rapes) and robberies are characterized by high levels of information at the descriptive level. 5 About 95 percent of the victims of those crimes were willing to assess the sex and race of their assailants, somewhat fewer hazarded an estimate (choosing among a series of ranges) of their ages. Purse snatchings and picked pockets, on the other hand, are personal thefts characterized by brief inter- changes of information; only about 50 percent of the victims of those crimes have much of a tale to tell the police if they report the event to them. When we examine property crimes, the paucity of information potentially available to the criminal justice system is apparent. In the case of burglary, only about 5 percent of the victims have anything concrete to tell the police about “whodunit,” and the figure is only marginally better for auto theft.
  • 15. Table 1 also tells us the distribution of positive identifications of suspects as op- posed to simple descriptions of perpetrators. The analysis is based on a series of ques- tions probing victim-offender relationships. 6 It categorizes incidents into those in which victims do not even know what the relationship might be, so slight is their information; Information, Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the Limits of Police Productivity TABLE 1 223 DESCRIPTIONS AND IDENTIFICATIONS’ Information Interpersonal Personal Vehicle Violence* Robbery Theft3 Burglary Larceny Theft Percentage of Victims Able to Describe Perpetrator’s: Sex 96.8 %.l 53.4 5.4 4.3 7.6 Race 96.0 93.9 52.5 4.8 4.2 7.0 Age 90.5 92.1 51.0 4.8 4.1. 7.1 Percentage of Victims Who:
  • 16. Don’t Know Relationship 3.2 3.6 46.0 90.5 92.9 91.8 Know Relationship- Is a Stranger 53.8 78.6 49.0 8.5 5.6 5.8 Some Identification Possible 43.0 17.8 5.0 1.0 1.5 2.4 (N of cases) (3.777) (1,023) (512) (5,789) (19,601) (1,198) ‘Based on all regular and series incidents from the National Crime Panel for reference year 1973; computations by the authors. The figures reported here are refined and differ slightly from those previously reported. *Includes crimes classified by the FBI as assault (simple and aggravated) and rape. %cludes purse snatchings and pocket picking. cases in which they know enough to know they did not know the other party; and those in which there is a chance that some identification might be made (offenders are de- scribed as “casual acquaintances,” etc.). To the extent to which positive identifications realistically are required to solve crimes, the data in Table 1 do not speak well to the potential ability of the criminal justice system to solve crimes and deter criminals. While descriptions abound for certain personal crimes, identifications do not. Only in interper- sonal violence, which we have seen includes a considerable number of nonstranger assaults and some rapes, can victims positively identify a substantial number of of- fenders. The frequency of descriptions in robberies is unmatched by identifications, for
  • 17. most robberies are attributed to strangers. Among property crimes, the situation appears hopeless. This hopelessness reflects our reading of how the police do their work. The fact of policing-as opposed to reconstructions of police work depicted by the media and in detective novels-is that investigatory follow-up work, the gathering of physical evi- dence, and the ferreting out of criminals through detective work, p1ay.a relatively unim- portant role in identifying and apprehending offenders. In Bloch and Bell’s (1976) study in Rochester, only about 5 percent of burglaries were solved through follow-up investiga- tions, and most of those came from interviews with victims and new witnesses. There, 224 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES only 0.1 percent of all burglaries were solved by physical evidence, and an additional 0.3 percent by the recovery of traceable property (Bloch and Bell. 1976:45). Of the few burglaries that were solved, most were either cleared by arrests at the scene based on information supplied by victims and (mostly) witnesses, or by obtaining confessions from persons later arrested for something else. As the RAND Corporation concluded in a recent analysis of the criminal investigation process (Greenwood et al., 1975:ix):
  • 18. The single most important determinant of whether or not a case will be solved is the information the victim supplies to the immediately responding patrol officer. If informa- tion that uniquely identifies the perpetrator is not presented at the time the crime is reported, the perpetrator, by and large. will not be subsequently identified. These data suggest that “unique identifications” are rarely available. INFORMATION: UNREPORTED CRIME While duration of contact and other determinants of the perceptual acuity of the victims of crime shape the kind and accuracy of the information they potentially can offer the criminal justice system, even this potential is not fully realized. Two major social and organizational processes -citizen crime-reporting and police incident-record- ing-mediate the flow of information between victim and system. It is widely known that the police do not record, or record accurately, all of the complaints that are brought to their attention (Seidman and Couzens, 1974). That source of error in crime statistics is beyond the scope of this article, but see Skogan (1976b). Victimization surveys, on the other hand, were designed to reveal something of the dimensions of “the dark figure of unreported crime” by probing for the frequency and rationale for nonreporting. What those data reveal is substantial amounts of citizen nonreporting.
  • 19. even in major crime categories (Skogan, 1976a). Nonreporting may serve to shield from police attention a large amount of potential strategic intelligence about the distribution of crime (useful for planning, manpower allocation, and the like), and it may also protect individual offenders from the “long arm of the law.” It has also led us to overestimate seriously the extent to which the police are able to solve crimes in the United States since measures of “certainty” of punishment in most empirical research on deterring crime have been ratios of the number of arrests for a type of crime (e.g., larceny) divided by the number of crimes of that type officially known to the police. Assuming that unreported crimes would be unlikely to generate additional arrests if they were known to the police,7 then to the extent that crimes are underreported our estimation of the capacity of police to make arrests (certainty) is erroneously inflated. Table 2 summarizes National Crime Panel estimates of citizen reporting practices for the major types of crime measured by the survey. The data indicate that over 67 percent of all attempted and successful auto theft was reported to the police (or they were on the scene of the crime), while the comparable figure was about 50 percent for robbery and only about 22 percent for simple property larceny. The failure of many or most crimes to come to the attention of the police in the first place clearly sets severe upper limits on their ability to solve an appreciable portion of
  • 20. them, given current patterns of citizen reporting and police activity. When we examine the next filter in the flow of information to the police, the ability Information. Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the Limits of Police Productivity TABLE 2 225 NONREPORTING AND THE FLOW OF INFORMATION' Crime Percentage Reported Percentage Reported Percentage Reported? and Know Race of and Positive or Police on Scene Offender identification N Interpersonal Violence Robbery Personal Theft Burglary Larceny Vehicle Theft 40. I 38.1 16.9 3.777 49.6 43.6 7.4 1,023 31.6 18.9 1.4 512
  • 21. 45.8 2.6 0.2 5.789 22.5 I.0 0.3 19.603 67.2 3.5 0.5 1.198 ‘Computed by the authors from the 1973 National Crime Panel regular and series incidents. *A small number of “don’t know” responses to the question on reporting to the police are treated as nonre- porters in this analysis. of victims to pass on some description of the offender in crimes that are reported, we find further attenuation in the ability of the police to ensure high levels of certainty of arrest. The effect is not large for interpersonal violence, for most of those crimes that are reported to the police carry with them some modicum of information. In the case of robbery, and especially purse snatching, the effect of nonreporting and a lack of descrip- tive information each make serious inroads into the solvability of offenses, however. Only 19 percent of all personal thefts were reported to the police and carried with them some information about the offender. Again, in the case of property offenses only an extremely small number of cases admit of much useful information for the criminal justice system. The most realistic column of Table 2, that which isolates reported crimes in which some positive identification might be made, indicates that almost all of the cases that might come to the attention of the police can be solved only with some difficulty. Only
  • 22. interpersonal violence, crimes that are characterized by victim- offender relationships of some duration and often take place within acquaintanceship networks, have the potential to yield many positive identifications. Identifications could be made only in about 7 percent of all robberies, 1 percent of personal thefts, and even fewer burglaries. The data in Tables 1 and 2 indicate that there are severe structural obstacles to the solution of large numbers of serious crimes by the police. The obstacles are “structural” in the sense that they rest on enduring human patterns of cognition and behavior under conditions of uncertainty and stress. Offenders in many of these events are seen only fleetingly, often under bad conditions. and estimates of such personal attributes as age are doubtlessly difficult under any circumstances. Research by Buckhout (1974) has 226 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES TABLE 3 NATIONAL APPREHENSION RATIOS (1973) Crime’ Survey’ Apprehension Ratioss UCR Arrests* UCR Incidents3 Incidents UCR Survey Forcible Rape
  • 23. Aggravated Assault Robbery Vehicle Theft Burglary 25,720 51,000 153,000 0.50 0.17 208,100 416.270 1,313,180 0.50 0.16 127,530 382,680 1,214,884 0.33 0.10 155,800 923,600 1,330,470 0.17 0.12 434,000 2,540,OOO 7,818,026 0.17 0.06 ‘Following standard UCR (Uniform Crime Reports) definitions, these are crimes that generally are compatible between the UCR and LEAA’s (Law Enforcement Assistance Administration) victim surveys. Robbery and burglary counts include both household and commercial survey data; FBI conventions regarding incident and victimization counting rules are used here. Personal theft is excluded here for lack of good comparable UCR figures see Skogan (19766). *Uniform Crime Report, 1973, Table 24; FBI 100 percent estimates for U.S. population. Wniform Crime Report, 1973, pp. I l-29; FBI 100 percent estimates for U.S. population. 4Estimated U.S. totals calculated from unpublished tabulations supplied by the Census Bureau (incident series) from the 1973 reference year National Victim Survey data. SEstimated U.S. totals computed by dividing UCR arrests by UCR, U.S. estimates, and by survey incident estimates for 1973. documented the well-known unreliability of eyewitness testimony. These problems are
  • 24. not likely to go away, or even to change very dramatically in magnitude. These data thus may be seen as setting some rough upper limits on the extent to which society can deal effectively with major crimes. The ability of the criminal justice system to solve, by arrest, crimes with a positive identification is high, but those crimes are a very small portion of all crimes. Effective and innovative police work may extend the ability of the system to apprehend criminals some distance into the pool of crimes with suspect descriptions, but, given the limited resources and the great number of cases coming to the attention of the police daily, such sleuthing never will extend very far. Thus, even crimes that do come to the attention of the police will include a great number (perhaps 20 to 90 percent) of successful predations. When we add to that number the vast pool of events that go unreported in the first place, it is clear that the certainty of arrest for specific crimes is quite low indeed. Table 3 attempts to estimate more realistic certainty levels for various types of crimes. It presents both official and survey data on crimes relevant to the computation of “apprehension ratios,” or the ratio of arrests to crimes. Such ratios typically are used to measure certainty of arrest or police effectiveness in a jurisdiction (Hatry, 1975) and serve as indicators of the certainty component of deterrence models. Table 3 indicates that the use of officially reported crime in the denominator of
  • 25. such measures greatly enhances the apparent certainty of arrest in each category, for those data underestimate Information, Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the Limits of Police Productivity 227 the frequency of crime in the population. The ratio of arrests to officially known crime in the aggravated assault category reaches nearly 1:2, while the ratio of arrests to the National Crime Panel’s estimate of the number of aggravated assaults in the United States is closer to 1:61/4. By this measure, the certainty of arrest for assault (a crime involving serious physi- cal injury or the use of a dangerous weapon) is much lower than previously calculated. A difference of the same magnitude can be noted in the forcible rape category and (less dramatically) for robbery and burglary. The apprehension ratio for burglary in particular is extraordinarily small when we calculate it as the ratio of arrests to survey-measured crime.8 These figures, in fact, raise some question whether deterrence processes have ever “been given a chance” in this country. Much academic research on deterrence has concluded that certainty of punishment, not severity of punishment, is the key to effec- tive crime reduction (see Antunes and Hunt, 1973). The low apprehension rates pre- sented here indicate that the holes in the preventive net cast by
  • 26. the criminal justice system are so large that we should not be surprised that the threat of criminal sanction appears to have so little impact.g POLICE ORGANIZATION AS AN INFORMATION- GATHERING STRATEGY The criminal justice system will depend on the development of dramatically more effective means of coping with many types of crime to raise the certainties of arrest significantly. Only by extending the arm of the law rather deeply into the pool of currently unidentified offenders will such inroads be made. It thus is useful to examine policing strategies as organizational devices for gathering information. Such strategies are numerous. Patrol officers cruise looking for “activity,” and attempt to arrive on the scene rapidly after the reporting of a crime. Technicians gather fingerprints and other physical evidence. Detectives assemble photographs of suspicious persons and descrip- tions of their methods of operation. While these activities reflect conventional wisdom about the effective organization of policing, recent studies have begun to challenge their efficacy. Reiss has pointed out that arrest productivity in general is low, and that when arrests are made, they are likely to be made by patrol officers rather than detectives. In an analysis of 1965 Washington, D.C. data, Reiss found the 87 percent of arrests for Part I Index crimes (crimes against
  • 27. persons) were made by the patrol division (1971: 104). He believes that the reason for this is that “arrests do not result from investigation by a specialized division of the department, such as the detective division, but rather from the routine activity of patrol as it responds to citizen calls for assistance” (1971: 105). Similarly, Greenwood et al. (1975) sharply questioned the productivity of most detective work. They found that little time is in fact devoted to the investigation of most serious crimes, and that most investi- gatory effort is spent in essentially administrative activity. The detective practice of reinterviewing victims, duplicating in many instances the work of patrol officers, does not expand the amount of information available in most cases. As Greenberg et al., found in their study of robbery and other crimes, “unless relevant field information had been obtained at the crime scene by the responding [patrol] officer, if the offender had not been apprehended, the chances of the case being solved at the detective level were minimal” (1977:Xx). 228 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES Greenwood et al., (1975) also argue that most physical evidence is unproductive, and that fingerprinting as it is currently organized rarely itself leads to arrests. They conclude that these problems are in part structural, relating less to the quality of police
  • 28. work (they found that neither training nor the organization of detective work much affected these matters) than to the nature of the task at hand, and that certain crimes. “which constitute the majority, are difficult, if not impossible, to solve, regardless of the efforts expended by police” (Greenwood et al., 1975:65). Or, as Conklin and Bittner (1973) noted while traveling with burglary detectives, some “evidence” of any sort was present in only one-third of all cases. Repetto’s study of residential burglaries in Boston indicated similar low investigative productivity levels+nly 2.0 percent of the cases were solved by detective follow-up investigation (1974:50). Other research has begun to challenge the assumptions underlying police patrol strategies: that the visible presence of patrol cars on the streets prevents crimes from occurring, and that when crimes do occur, police officers will be able to either observe them in progress or arrive on the scene rapidly enough to gather fast-dissipating informa- tion. The preventive effect of patrol is difficult to assess, and it is becoming apparent that the Kansas City South Patrol Experiment (Kelling et al., 1974) has provided neither a definitive nor generalizable answer to that question (Larson, 1975; Davis and Knowles, 1975). Data gathered from victims in the national crime survey indicate that there is an upper limit to the number of crimes that potentially can be observed by police patrols, and that very few crimes currently are being interrupted in progress by passing patrols.
  • 29. However, the vulnerability of certain crimes to police intervention, which is shaped by the information available to the criminal justice system, is not as severely constrained as the potential for positive eyewitness identification. Although apprehension is much more problematic when that information is not available, crimes that are visible or in which a rapid “hue and cry” may be raised provide opportunities for intervention that may raise certainty levels if appropriate policing strategies are adopted. Table 4 presents 1973 National Crime Panel data on the location of criminal of- fenses, categorized to shed some light on the extent to which those incidents were likely to occur in public places where they could be observed either by bystanders or passing police patrols. Incidents in Table 4 are classified as taking place (1) in a home, (2) in a public building, or (3) on a street, field, park, or “near home” (and, of course. some fell in the ubiquitous “other” category). Assuming that those that took place on a street, etc., were most visible to police patrols, we can see in Table 4 that the potential for police intervention varies considerably from crime to crime. Robbery, more than assault and rape,‘O takes place in public places, as do many larcenies and vehicle thefts. Rapes in particular tend to occur in the home (almost 30 percent of the total re- ported in the 1973 survey). a place legally and culturally almost completely shielded from official view (Stinchcombe, 1963). In between fall many
  • 30. personal thefts and larcenies (purse snatchings and the like) that occur in shops or office buildings, but not in places highly visible to official police. It is most likely that private security guards, along with controlled access and egress from buildings and employee training in vigilance, would have the highest deterrent payoff in such cases. The large number of robberies that take place on the street (visible to the police) or in public buildings (visible to bystanders able to raise a rapid hue and cry), combined with the high amount of descriptive information available in such crimes, may recommend them for special attention by criminal justice planners. Policies to increase police or public surveillance of the streets, reduce neigh- Information, Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the Limits of Police Productivity TABLE 4 229 VULNERABILITY OF CRIMES TO PATROL’ Crime At In a On Street or 9% of Police Home Building* Near Home Other “On Scene”3 Total Assault 11.0 23.4 54.2. 11.4 3.7 Forcible Rape 29.3 7.8 45.5 17.4 1.4
  • 31. Robbery 10.9 18.8 64.8 5.5 1.6 Personal Theft 2.1 47.7 39.3 10.8 0.7 Larceny 4.8 27.0 60.0 8.2 0.3 Burglary 96.3 3.7 0.0 0.0 0.4 Vehicle Theft 0.0 1.5 94.8 2.8 1.8 ‘Based on all regular and series incidents in the 1973 National Crime Panel data. Incident N'S are the same as previous tables. *The “in a building” category combines events occurring in schools, hotels, office buildings, and personal crimes occurring in commercial establishments. 3Percentage of incidents in each type of crime, regardless of the place ofoccurrence, in which the police were present or arrived at the scene without being called. borhood anonymity, and encourage bystander reporting to the police may have substan- tial payoffs in raising certainty rates for robbery. It is important to note, however, that these visibility figures probably again set important upper bounds on the solvability of crimes. As Rubinstein has noted (1973:339-40), the ordinary routines of police work virtually preclude the extension of patrol intervention into private or even semipublic places: For the policeman, the determining factor of any crime’s importance is its setting. He defines the location of all crimes by the deceptively simple distinction between “inside” and “outside.” [Olutside means any location a patrolman can be reasonably expected to see while on patrol. . . . Any outside crime is an afront to the patrolman’s notion of
  • 32. himself as a guardian of his territory, an occurrence which suggests to his superiors that he was not doing his job properly. Thus, for example (p. 339), “If a burglar breaks into a building through a rear door or cuts a hole in a roof, his act is considered an inside crime, because the patrolman had no chance to notice anything amiss, even if he was patrolling alertly.” And (p. 341), “The majority of all murders and aggravated assaults occur inside and therefore they are not ‘on’ the patrolman.” The result is that relatively few crimes currently are being interrupted in progress. l1 As Table 4 also indicates, in no major category were as many as 4 percent of crimes so described by victims in the 1973 survey. Robbery was typical of personal crimes: in that year only 1.6 percent were in progress when the police arrived or the police arrived before they could be summoned. For larceny and burglary similar percentages were 230 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES vanishingly small. The data on the visibility of crimes presented in Table 4 indicate that these figures potentially could become much larger, however. Many crimes, including some with currently low apprehension rates, frequently occur on the street or in other public places where police and citizen activities might have
  • 33. more productive conse- quences if they were effectively organized.12 SHOULD ONLY “GOOD” ARRESTS BE COUNTED? Thus far we have focused on the problem of identifying and arresting offenders. However, all arrests are not equally productive. Some arrests result in a guilty plea or conviction, but many do not. Measures of arrest productivity must look beyond the simple counting of arrests. Making an arrest is one thing; making an arrest that will result in an indictment and conviction is something else entirely. In some senses a better measure of arrest productivity is the ratio of arrests resulting in conviction to crimes known to the police. This reflects, at least in part, the diligence with which police have conducted investigations and built cases. In a related suggestion, Hatry (1975) has pro- posed as one measure of productivity the number of arrests that survive the first judicial screening per police man-year. He argues that simply counting arrests without regard to quality, particularly if used as a measure of the performance of a department or of individual officers within a department, may produce perverse effects; specifically, po- lice “may be encouraged to make excessive, unreasonable, or at least marginal arrests” (Hatry, 1975: 103). Whatever the specific measure, attempts to gauge arrest productivity must reflect the quality of those arrests; the way in which the police do their job in investigating a case has a profound effect on the eventual
  • 34. disposition of those who are arrested. As Jacob has noted (1978:173), “The quality of police work determines, to a considerable extent, what the courts can do with those whom the police arrest.” Of course, as police are quick to point out, these productivity measures also reflect the influence of other actors such as prosecutors, judges, witnesses, and juries, over whom the police have little or no control. Still, considering productivity from the per- spective of arrests that result in conviction has much to recommend it. While over three-fourths of all Index crimes are not cleared by arrest, there also is considerable slippage out of the justice system after an arrest has been made. An analysis of data from Kansas City (Pate, Bowers, and Parks, 1976) revealed that of those persons arrested for Part I Index crimes half were not indicted, and thus were simply released. Of those who were indicted, only one-third were eventually convicted or pleaded guilty. Two-thirds, the bulk of those indicted, had charges dropped by the prosecutor, were dismissed by the judge, or were found not guilty. Given that such a small portion of offenses is solved by arrest, the substantial attrition (over 80 percent) within the prosecution and court system of the cases of those few who are arrested is disturbing. This means that the certainty of conviction for many types of crime is virtually nil, and police productivity measured in terms of “good” arrests (i.e., those that hold up in court and result in a
  • 35. conviction or guilty plea) is similarly low. The data reported by Pate and his associates indicate that the Kansas City Police Department, widely respected as one of the best in the country, manages one felony conviction per 14,720 patrol hours, or about one con- viction per seven work years!13 Similar results have been reported for Washington D.C. Analyzing data from the Information. Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the Limits of Police Productivity 231 Prosecutor’s Management Information System (PROMIS), researchers from the Institute for Law and Social Research found tl~ it only about 9 percent of commercial robberies were cleared by an arrest. I4 Furthermore, about half of those arrested dropped out of the system; the result was that only about 5 percent of felonious commercial robberies reported to the police resulted in either a felony or misdemeanor conviction or guilty plea. The figures for commercial burglaries were even worse. Only about 4 percent of commercial burglaries known to police resulted in an arrest, and over half of those arrested dropped out of the system; thus, about 2 percent of the commercial burglaries known to police resulted in a conviction or guilty plea of some kind. A second analysis of Washington, D.C. PROMIS data reported
  • 36. the curious finding that arrest productivity in the Washington police primarily results from the actions of a small portion of the force; the department’s arrest productivity appears to be concen- trated in a small subset of the force (Forst, Luclanovic, and Cox, 1977). The analysis revealed that 46 percent of the department’s sworn personnel made no arrests at all in l974; a total of 63 percent made no arrest resulting in a guilty plea or conviction. Forst and his associates found that over half of all arrests that resulted in a guilty plea or conviction were produced by 368 officers, about 8 percent of the department’s sworn personnel. Why these officers were so productive while most of the force was notably unproductive is unknown. The limited data in the PROMIS files allow Forst and his colleagues to point to some interesting possibilities, but a definite answer is impossible. They believe that arrests succeed in court only when the arresting officer presents the prosecutor with strong physical evidence and good witnesses. Only 32 percent of the department’s 1974 robbery arrests resulted in a conviction (or guilty plea), but “the number of convictions per 100 robbery arrests was 60 percent higher when tangible evidence was recovered than when it was not, and it was 40 percent higher when the MPD secured at least two lay witnesses than when they did not” (Forst, Lucianovic, and Cox, 1977:24). Thus, although both the RAND study of detectives (Greenwood et al., 1975) and the Rochester team policing study (Bloch and
  • 37. Bell, 1976) found that officers seldom gathered physical evidence, and that when they did it did not prove helpful in solving cases, neither of those studies followed cases after arrest to learn their fate in the courts. Had they done so, the role of tangible evidence might have taken on more importance in their analysis. The extent to which blame for post-arrest attrition should be apportioned among police, courts, prosecutors, victims, and witnesses is unclear. However, an analysis of the Washington PROMIS data by Cannavale and Falcon (1976) provides some indication of a few of the things that can go wrong. They found a serious problem caused by interrogation practices used by police in field settings. Investigating why a substantial number of witnesses. could not be located by either follow-up detective investigators or the prosecutor’s office, Cannavale and Falcon found that this was often the result of witnesses being interrogated by police within earshot of the offender. In these circum- stances they tended to give false names and addresses to avoid the possibility of retribu- tion at the hands of the offender. Of course when they did this, witnesses could not be contacted to appear in court, thus allowing the offender to go free in many instances.15 The research also brought to light major problems within the prosecutor’s office in the management of cases and witnesses. A program was devised to modify police field interrogation practices, and the prosecutor’s office totally
  • 38. revamped its previously er- ratic witness management system. These changes should reduce somewhat the 23 per- 232 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES cent of cases that were dropped by the prosecutor’s office because witnesses were classified as unfindable or uncooperative. Of course, some victims and witnesses truly are uncooperative. A study by the Vera Institute (1976) of the 100,739 felony arrests made by the New York City police in 1971 found that only about 5 percent of those arrests resulted in felony prison sentences. In about half of the cases involving a victim there was a prior relationship between the victim and the defendant, and in many of these cases the victim refused to press charges. WHAT IS TO BE DONE? In this article we have pointed out some unpleasant empirical generalizations about the operation of police in local communities. The capacity of police to solve most kinds of crime is low. Most Part I offenses known to the police are nor solved. When crimes are solved and arrests made, it is almost always due to extensive information supplied to police by witnesses and victims as to the identity and where-
  • 39. abouts of the offender. The amount of information about the offender that could be supplied by victims and witnesses is a function of the type of crime and the circumstances in which it occurs. For many ordinary crimes the amount of information that can be supplied police is extremely slight. Most arrests are made by patrol officers: the arrest productivity of detectives is very low. The capacity of police and prosecutors to successfully prosecute those who are arrested is low. Most of those arrested for Part I offenses will eventually have charges dropped or the case dismissed. In the face of these grim realities what, if anything, can be done? Could the police do better if they tried? Would increased efforts by citizens improve arrest productivity? We believe that although a dramatic improvement in arrest rates is unlikely (and indeed may be impossible), there are a number of promising ways by which police and communities may be able to effect real gains in arrest productivity. Our approach is based on two premises: (1) the importance of citizens as co-producers of police outputs and (2) the crucial importance of information as the basis for solving crimes and arresting offenders. As Kelling and his associates concluded:
  • 40. Assigning the police full responsibility for the maintenance of order, the prevention of crime and the apprehension of criminals constitutes far too great a burden on far too few. Primary responsibility rests with families, the community and its individual mem- bers. The police can only facilitate and assist members of the community in the mainte- nance of order, and no more. (1974:533) Information. Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the Limits of Police Productivity 233 The solution of crimes is the shared responsibility of both the citizens and the police in a community; both are in a very real sense co-producers of police services.r6 The role of the police in solving crime has long been taken for granted; the notion that citizens also play an important role in this process is somewhat more novel. Citizens are impor- tant because most information about crime, including the fact that it has occurred, comes from citizens who are victims or witnesses. Because the success of the police in making arrests is so dependent on the actions of witnesses and victims, citizens share responsibility for police productivity. l7 Reiss underscored this when he pointed out (1967: 105) that, “the capacity of the police to solve any crime is severely limited by citizens, partly owing to the fact that there is no feasible way to
  • 41. solve most crimes except by securing the cooperation of citizens to link a person to the crime.” Convincing the public that this is true may require a considerable effort. Popular films, novels, and television dramas generally portray crime as a matter that will be dealt with effectively by the police. As a result, the public has formed certain expectations about crime that underemphasize the role of victims and witnesses and overestimate the efficacy of the police. As Sanders wryly pointed out (1977: lOO), because they have seen it on television, burglary victims expect that police will dust for fingerprints, and when prints are found they believe that “if you send the prints to the FBI, they’ll send you the name, photo- graph, and address of the burglar.” Police departments must convince the public that the realities of policing differ from the sometimes flattering images depicted in the mass media, and that crimes can be solved only when citizens report them to the police and assist in identifying the offender. To be sure, police departments contribute to the solution of crimes, but communi- ties must be aware that, realistically, the police acring on their own have a very limited impact. The recent response-time study by the Kansas City Police Department (1977) dramatically underscores this point. The researchers found that the average victim did not contact police until more than six minutes after the incident occurred. They esti- mated however that reporting crimes within two minutes of
  • 42. completion would increase arrests 10 percent. Further, if burglaries observed by victims or witnesses were reported within one minute, the probability of making a response-related arrest would improve 40 percent. It is ironic that for all the public concern expressed in recent years about improving police response time, as far as improving on-scene arrests goes, the average amount of delay introduced by citizens is so great that it virtually precludes the transla- tion of reductions in police response time into increased arrest productivity.18 The police cannot be everywhere; in fact the police “discover” very few crimes in progress. Communities must rely on their citizens to call police to the scene of a crime and, if the offender has already fled, to provide as complete a description as possible of the incident, the offender, and any missing property. We believe that community aware- ness campaigns should stress the importance of those witnessing a crime (or suspicious behavior) reporting the incident to police and making notes on the description of sus- pects, getaway car, etc., to improve the quality of information they can provide the police. Some communities have undertaken citizen “anticrime” programs, but the focus of many of these campaigns has been limited to crime deterrence through such tactics as personal self-defense courses or installing better door and window locks. Our approach to the crime of burglary would not ignore household “hardening” measures, but it would
  • 43. also stress the importance of vigilant and concerned neighbors and the necessity of preparing an inventory and description of major items of property in each household. 234 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES Citizens need to know that burglars are generally apprehended only when their activities are noticed by neighbors who alert the authorities or when police are able to trace stolen property based on information provided by the victim. Once citizens have been prodded into reporting crimes and information about crime, the focus of responsibility shifts from citizens to the police. It is possible that some improvement in arrest productivity may result from modest changes in the way police go about their work. In policing, innovations and proposed innovations to enhance produc- tivity abound. We will not undertake a systematic review of suggested police innova- tions; that is beyond the scope of this article. lg However, we do wish to consider some selected examples to illustrate how the ideas we have developed can serve as a frame- work for thinking about innovation and arrest productivity. We believe that the key to greater arrest productivity lies in increasing the amount and quality of information available to the police, preserving (i.e., not losing) that infor- mation, and efficiently making use of the information. Police
  • 44. practices must be scrutin- ized in terms of the way they affect the acquisition and use of information. The initial police response to citizen reports is made by patrol officers. Most arrests are made either by patrol officers or by detectives using information that was recorded by patrol officers. The way patrol officers do their job has important consequences. If the investigating officer does not ask appropriate questions about the incident, witnesses may not volunteer and important information will be omitted. Or if information is not recorded accurately and in some comprehensible format, any follow-up investigation will be im- peded. For this reason many police departments have begun to revise their training proce- dures to emphasize the importance of preliminary investigation in accurately gathering relevant information while events are still fresh in the minds of witnesses. This position is summarized by Chief George Hart of the Oakland Police Department who argues that, “we must reevaluate our traditional thinking concerning the role of the patrol officer in the investigative process, and we must give very careful attention to our training and record- ing function to ensure that maximum attention is given to those investigative elements of information that have been shown to be useful in the solution of crimes” (1977:~~). In connection with revised training, many police departments (Greenwood et al., 1975:83) are restructuring their incident report forms so that
  • 45. patrol officers routinely record on a standardized form information about offenders and their characteristics.20 It is expected that this will increase the amount of information recorded by patrol officers, and, because of the standardized format, the information gathered in the initial investiga- tion should be more accessible to follow-up investigators and police planners. However, as Greenberg et al. (1977) point out, implementing these ideas in the investigation pro- cess involves some serious trade-offs; it is not true that more information necessarily is more productive. In some circumstances, the use of lengthy, preceded incident forms may be counterproductive. Greenberg and his associates at Stanford Research Institute agree that information is essential to apprehension and prosecution, but they are pessi- mistic about the way in which this notion has been implemented in some departments where investigating officers must wade through long, general lists of questions and preceded investigation forms. They believe that the “main objective of patrol-to ensure the safety of the victim and quickly ascertain what information can be derived to hasten the offender’s apprehension-can be thwarted by undue delay in running over a list of data that are likely to be useless” (1977:xxii). Greenberg and his colleagues argue that the key to enhanced productivity lies in collecting only that information that is likely to
  • 46. lnformation, Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the Limits of Police Productivity ‘235 be useful in identifying and apprehending an offender. They have developed and tested specialized investigation forms for robbery that minimize the amount of information collected but maximize the likelihood that it will be useful information (Greenberg et al., 1977). In earlier research Greenberg, Yu, and Lang (1973), developed a similar special- ized form and decision model for investigating burglaries. A replication testing this burglary model in some thirty police departments currently is being conducted by the Police Executive Research Forum (Farmer, 1978). Another area of police department improvement involves liaison between patrol officers and detectives. As Bloch and Bell (1976) note, relations between patrol officers and detectives are frequently characterized more by a spirit of hostility and mistrust rather than a spirit of cooperation. Unless the patrol officer sees an opportunity to personally make an arrest, the preliminary investigation is often perfunctory. Detective investigators, in turn, tend to ignore preliminary investigation reports and begin anew, duplicating work already done. The goal of the Rochester Team Policing experiment was to improve productivity by improving patrol detective liaison. In the Rochester experi- ment, instead of dividing patrol officers into geographically based units and relegating detectives to a single, centralized detective division, detectives
  • 47. were paired with patrol officers to form police teams; each team was assigned to a specific geographic area. Teams had their own offices and were responsible for most police services within their areas on a twenty-four-hour basis. Team commanders were responsible for both patrol and investigative operations. Both Rochester police officials and outside evaluators judged the experiment to be a success (Bloch and Bell, 1976); detectives were able to use preliminary investigation reports much more productively, and their follow-up investiga- tion developed significantly greater additional information. The result was a substantial increase in arrest productivity.*’ The Rochester experiment also developed two management techniques that were credited with improving productivity. One was centralized case management, which allowed a team commander to assign specific investigative tasks rather than entire cases, to specific patrol officers or detectives. This allowed work to proceed very quickly, and also allowed work to go round the clock when needed. The second management tech- nique was “early case closure.” This recognized that some crimes are unsolvable- information is scant, there are no leads, and prospects for identifying the offender are miniscule. As Greenwood et al. showed (1975:65), regardless of the amount of effort expended by investigators, cases like this will not be solved. Under early case closure “hopeless” cases are identified as quickly as possible on the
  • 48. basis of a number of explicit solvability factors and they are officially closed. This allows investigators to concentrate on those cases where there is at least a reasonable chance of solving the case and making an arrest.22 A final area of productivity enhancement that we wish to discuss is that of compu- terized data analysis and data management systems. These systems, which are coming into use in many police departments, potentially can improve the capacity of police departments to make use of the voluminous data that they routinely gather. Some police departments are using their data systems to follow cases through the prosecution and trial phase of the judicial system. Proper use of a data analysis system allows police departments to keep track of the number of “good” (i.e., prosecutable) arrests they are making and pinpoint problems of departmental practices needing improvement (Canna- vale and Falcon, 1976). 236 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES Other possible applications include computerized M.O. (modus operandi, or, method of operation) files, computerized fingerprint files, and computer-assisted mug shot systems. However, the practical utility of existing systems ‘designed to apply com- puters to these problems is open to question. Greenberg et al.
  • 49. (1977), found the compu- terized M.O. systems, fingerprint systems, and physical description inventories they examined to be of little value in identifying offenders. Among the factors they felt were responsible for this failure were the often low quality of information entered into the systems and the fact that few offenders seem to specialize in a single type of street crime. Although an automated, national fingerprint search capability would be particu- larly helpful (Greenwood et al., 1975; Greenberg, Yu, and Lang, 1973), it appears that the availability of any operational and practical system is still some years in the future. The examples we have reviewed are only a few of many proposals for enhancing police productivity and improving the likelihood of identifying and apprehending felons. We have presented these examples as illustrations of the kinds of innovation being tried by a number of police departments. Unfortunately, as Antunes and Hunt (1973) pointed out, while the most effective deterrent policy may be one that increases the certainty of apprehension as much as possible, neither criminal justice researchers nor police admin- istrators know how this might be accomplished or what it might cost. We have no specific remedies to propose. Our purpose has been to focus attention on the capacity of the police to identify and apprehend offenders. We have shown that the probability of apprehension varies according to the type of crime and the circumstances surrounding
  • 50. the crime. For crimes of profit, apprehension probabilities are very low, and the reason for this is the paucity of information about the identity of the offender. Our analysis of victim survey data indicates that, while there is much room for improvement, the infor- mation problem is real and fixes discernable limits on police productivity. We believe that scholars and police administrators must forthrightly address the productivity problem and shift their attention to apprehension strategies and tactics that involve the public and that are formed within the information acquisition and utilization framework we have discussed. Crime is a problem for which there are no easy solutions. But we believe that communities can, and should, ameliorate the problem by gradually improving their capacity to identify and apprehend offenders. Given our present state of ignorance about these matters, there is a need for a major effort to identify potentially helpful programs and innovations, and to rigorously evaluate their usefulness in a variety of community settings. This, combined with programs that involve the public in the fight against crime, holds the key to improving the currently dismal situation. ACKNOWLEDGEMENT Wesley G. Skogan was supported in part during the development of this paper by a Visiting Fellowship grant (76NI-994032) from the National institute of Law Enforcement and Criminal
  • 51. Justice, Law Enforcement Assistance Administration, and by the Center for Urban Affairs at Northwestern University’s Research Agreement Program grant (77Nl-994018) from the same source. George E. Antunes was partially supported by a grant (5T32-MH-15222-02) from the Na- tional Institute of Mental Health to the Workshop in Political Theory and Policy Analysis at Indiana University. Points of view or opinions stated here are those of the authors and do not represent the official position of the United States Department of Justice or the National Institute of Mental Health. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 1978 meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association. Information. Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the Limits of Police Productivity 237 NOTES In this regard, Greenwood et al.. note (1975:7) that “it may be obvious at the outset that the crime is not likely to be solved. The police view in these situations is that a distraught victim deserves attention. regardless of the eventual result.” Indeed, one study (Ward, Ward, and Feely, 197564-65). points out that within the set of events classified as robbery, clearance rates are systematically lower for unarmed compared with armed robbery. The study attributes this difference to the substantially lower amount of information available in unarmed robberies that, as a group, are briefer in
  • 52. duration than those in which a weapon is employed. A substantial proportion of arrests for serious crime occurs because a victim or witness is able to supply police with the name and address of the offender (Greenwood et al., 1975:68). Similar findings are reported by Isaacs (l%7). Folk (1971), Feeney et al. (1973). Conklin (1972). and Conklin and Bittner (1973). Of course, the total amount of information available undoubtedly includes additional information provided by witnesses who were not victims. The National Victim Survey focuses on informa- tion recalled by those who were victims. We have no data on the amount of information available from respondents who witnessed crimes other than those in which they were victimized. There may be an important amount of untapped information available from nonvictim witnesses that might improve arrest productivity. Unfortunately, we know of no victim-survey data that ad- dress this point, and thus we must leave the question unresolved. That this is something worth considering in the design of future surveys is suggested by the Kansas City response-time study that found that about 9 percent of crimes reported to police were reported by witnesses who were not victims (Kansas City Police Department, 1977). Assaults and rapes have been combined here for methodological reasons. Validity research by the Census Bureau indicates that rapes are often “confused” with assaults by the survey instru- ment (Turner, 1972). Also, only 158 rapes were uncovered by
  • 53. the 1973 national survey, although this weights to over 150,000 for estimation pruposes. Validity research by the Census Bureau indicates that the survey figures for assault substantially underestimate the amount of nonstranger assault in the population, for those events often are not recalled to the interviewer (Turner, 1972). The resistance of unreported crimes to solution (had they in fact been reported to police) is suggested by the explanations citizens give for not reporting incidents to the police. The most frequent reason is that “nothing can be done” and the second most frequent rationale is that “the police wouldn’t want to be bothered.” Both of these responses suggest citizen awareness that less serious offenses that lack clear evidence as to the identity of the perpetrator are very unlikely to be solved. For further discussion of the nonreporting phenomenon see Skogan (1976~). However, it should be noted that even if the probability of arrest for any given criminal act is low. if one commits such acts regularly, the probability of being arrested at least once is much higher. Take as a starting point Greenwood’s finding (1970:24) that New York police solved by arrest four of every one hundred burglaries; this is a fairly low (but not unusual) probability of apprehension. Repetto (1974:21) found in his sample of burglars that it was common for them to make at least two hits per week. If we allow a burglar two weeks off for vacation, this would still amount to one hundred burglaries per year. Applying the appropriate formula (Owen, 1970:229),
  • 54. we find that the probability of no arrest (if the probability of arrest is 0.04 and one hundred burglaries are committed during a year) is 0.016; subtracting from 1 .O we find that the probability of being arrested at least once during the year is 0.984! This implies that while the police do not solve many burglaries, they eventually catch most burglars. Our suspicion is that this estimate is far too high for professional burglars. Nationally, the median age of those arrested for burglary is 17. If, as Repetto suggests (1974:24), professional burglars are older (25+) and more skilled than burglars below the age of 18, then the probability of arrest is not likely to be uniform across all classes of burglars. We believe that the probability of arrest is higher for younger, less experienced burglars, and much lower for experienced professionals. This would mean that the probability of a professional being arrested in any given year would be substantially lower than the 0.984 computed in our example. 9 For a thorough review of the substantive findings and methodological questions raised by the research literature on the deterrent effect of criminal sanctions see Blumstein, Cohen, and Nagin, 1978. 238 WESLEY Cl. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES lo We examine rape separately here because of the relatively large amount of “at home” crime it accounts for.
  • 55. I’ A study of the Los Angeles Police Department estimated that, by chance, every police patrol officer would pass the scene of a burglary about once every three months, and that of a robbery in progress about once every fourteen years (Institute for Defense Analysis, 1%7: 12). However, due to the fact that most burglaries (particularly of residences) offer few tangible clues that might alert passing officers, the likelihood of detection and intervention for that crime is low. I2 In addition, the use of alarms may effectively convert many crimes not occurring in public places from “inside” crimes to “outside” crimes for which police officers can be held accountable. Early Robbery Warning systems (Eliot, Strack, and Witter, 1975), for example, link silent alarms in commercial establishments directly to patrol cars, thus decreasing response time while effec- tively moving the crime “outdoors.” Traditional burglar alarm systems also may reduce re- sponse time and increase arrest rates (Conklin and Bittner, 1973), and they surely have the same effect as preventive patrol-to scatter intruders. I3 A specialized unit in the Kansas City Department that spent most of its time staked out at locations thought to be likely robbery or burglary targets did somewhat better, but even it managed only one felony conviction per work year (Pate, Bowers, and Parks, 1976:98). I* Computed by the authors from data presented by the Institute for Law and Social Research (1977: IO).
  • 56. I5 Cannavale and Falcon (1976) also found that the major determinant of successful prosecution was the number of prosecution witnesses. This implies that in cases where there are multiple witnesses, a loss of witnesses that is insufficient to justify dropping the case may seriously jeopardize chances of obtaining a conviction. Conversely, retaining all possible witnesses max- imizes the probability of a guilty plea or conviction. is As Gam et al., (1976) point out, co-productivity is a general feature in the production of all social services; it should not be construed as somehow unique to the provision of police services. I7 Citizens may also be thought of as sharing with offenders responsibility for the production of crime. Many citizen actions, such as leaving the keys in a car, encourage crime, while other actions, such as getting and using good locks in a home, discourage crime. I8 It should be noted that, at least in Kansas City, police response time is very good; obviously, this may not be true of departments in other communities. According to data collected in the response-time study (Kansas City Police Department, 1977) the average time to receive a call and dispatch a patrol unit is under three minutes. More importantly, the study revealed that the amount of time consumed in this process is primarily determined by the reporting style of those calling the police; there is little likelihood that this time can be reduced significantly. Once a patrol unit is dispatched, the amount of time it takes to reach
  • 57. the scene of a crime is almost entirely a function of the physical distance between the scene of the crime and the location of the patrol unit at the time of dispatch. This time could be reduced somewhat by increasing the number of patrol units, thus reducing the average distance to be traveled, but this would sharply escalate costs. (In departments staffing patrol cars with two officers, this could be accomplished by switching to one-officer patrol units.) In general, however, because of citizen delay in report- ing, police in Kansas City are racing to the scene of a crime long after there is any reasonable chance of making an arrest at or near the scene of the crime. Unless some way can be found to substantially reduce the time between the conclusion of a victimization incident and notification of police, the speed of police response will have only a limited effect on arrest productivity. If citizen delay in reporting could be reduced, the generally rapid police response time in Kansas City means that there is at least some hope of effecting an on- scene arrest. In other jurisdictions increased arrest productivity may require improvement in both the citizen and the police compo- nents of response time. [email protected] For a general overview of the innovation and police productivity literature, see the report of the Advisory Group on Productivity in Law Enforcement (1973). Many of the proposals it reviews have to do with productivity in areas other than the identification and apprehension of offenders (e.g., non-crime related calls for service, preventive patrol, etc.), but others clearly bear on the question of arrest productivity.
  • 58. *O For a discussion of a variety of specific techniques intended to improve the information gathering activities of patrol officers conducting investigations see Gay, Schell, and Schack (1977:117-19). Information, Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the Limits of Police Productivity 239 *I Not all innovations produce such felicitous results. A demonstration program in Wilmington intended to enhance patrol productivity by separating calls for service from deterrence-oriented patrol succeeded in raising the arrest productivity of patrol officers, but it had such a negative effect on detective investigations that the overall arrest productivity of the force declined (Tien, Simon, and Larson, 1978). 2* This notion of deciding on the basis of a set of objective criteria that a case is unlikely to be solved by follow-up investigation and should be dropped was pioneered by Greenberg, Yu, and Lang (1973) in their work on burglary investigation. REFERENCES Advisory Group on Productivity in Law Enforcement (1973). Opportunities for improving produc- tivity in police services. Washington, DC: National Commission on Productivity. Amir, M. (1971). Patterns in forcible rape. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • 59. Antunes, G. E., and Hunt, A. L. (1973). The impact of certainty and severity of punishment on levels of crime in American states: An extended analysis. Journal of criminal law, criminology ond police science, 64:48&93. Argana, M. (1975). Development of a national victimization survey. In Emilio Viano (ed.) Victimol- ogy: A New Focus, Vol. Ill. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books, pp. 171-79. Bloch, P. B., and Bell, J. (1976). Managing investigations: The Rochester system. Washington, DC: The Police Foundation. Blumstein, A.; Cohen, J.; and Nagin, D., eds. (1978). Deterrence and incapacitation: Estimating the effects of criminal sanctions on crime rates. Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences. Buckhout, R. (1974). Eyewitness testimony. [email protected] American, 231:23-31. Cannavale, F., and Falcon, W. (1976). Improving witness cooperation. Washington, DC: National Institute of Law Enforcement and Criminal Justice. Chicago Police Department (1970). Murder analysis, 1965- 1969. Chicago: Chicago Police Depart- ment. Conklin, J., and Bittner, E. (1973). Burglary in a suburb. Criminology, 11:206-32. Conklin, J. (1972). Robbery and the criminal justice system.
  • 60. Philadelphia: J.B. Lippincott. Curtis, L. A. (1974). Criminal violence: National patterns and behavior. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Davis, E. M., and Knowles, L. (1975). An evaluation of the Kansas City Preventive Patrol Experi- ment. Police Chief, pp. 22-27. Ehrlich, I. (1975). The deterrent effect of capital punishment: A question of life and death. Ameri- can economic review, 65~397-417. Eliot, W.A.; Strack, J.R.; and Witter, A.E. (1975). Early warning robbery reduction projects: An assessment of performance. McLean, VA: The MITRE Corporation. Farmer, M. (1978). Personal communication. Federal Bureau of Investigation (1976). Uniform Crime Report, 1976. Washington, DC: U.S. Gov- ernment Printing Gflice. Feeney, F. et al. (1973). The prevenrion and control of robbery. (Five volumes). Davis, CA: Center on Administration of Criminal Justice, University of California at Davis. Folk, J. (1971). Municipal detective systems: A quantitative approach. Cambridge, MA: Operations Research Center, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Forst, B.; Lucianovic, J.; and Cox, S. (1977). What happens after arrest? A court perspective of
  • 61. police operations in the District of Columbia. Washington, DC: Institute for Law and Social Research. 240 WESLEY G. SKOGAN and GEORGE E. ANTUNES Gartt, H.; Flax. M.; Springer, M.; and Taylor, J. (1976). Models for indicator development: A framework for policy analysis. Washington, DC: The Urban Institute. Gay, W.; Schell, T.; and Schack, S. (1977). Improving patrol productivity: Volume 1. Routine patrol. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Greenberg, B.; Yu, 0.; and Lang, K. (1973). Enhancement of the investigativefunction, Volume I. NTIS # PB-222 895. Menlo Park, CA: Stanford Research Institute. Greenberg, B. et al. (1977). Felony investigation decision model: An analysis of investigative elements of information. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Greenwood, P. (1970). An analysis of the apprehension activities of the New York City Police Department. New York: The New York City RAND Institute. Greenwood. P.W.; Chaiken, J.M.; Petersilia, J.; and Prusoff, L. (1975). The criminal investigation process; Volume III: Observations and analysis. Santa Monica, CA: The RAND Corporation.
  • 62. Hart, G. (1977). Foreword. In B. Greenberg et al., Felony investigation decision model: An analysis of investigative elements of information. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Hatry, H.P. (1975). Wrestling with police crime control measurement. In J. L. Wolfe and J. A. Heaphy (eds.) Readings on productivity in policing. Washington, DC: The Police Foundation, 86 128. Institute for Defense Analysis (1967). Task force report: Science and technology, a report to the President’s Commission on Law Enforcement and the Administration of Justice. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Institute for Law and Social Research (1977). Expanding the perspective of crime data: Perfor- mance implications for policymakers. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. lsaacs, H. (1%7). A study of communications, crimes, and arrests in a metropolitan police depart- ment. Appendix B of Institute for Defense Analyses, Task force report: Science and technology, a report to the President’s Commission on Law Enforcement and the Administration of Justice. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office, pp. 88-106. Jacob, H. (1978). Justice in America: Courts, lawyers, and the judicial process, 3rd ed. Boston: Little, Brown & Co. Kansas City Police Department (1977). Response time analysis; Vol. II. Analysis. Kansas City MO:
  • 63. Board of Police Commissioners. Kelling, G.; and Pate, T.; Dieckman, D.; and Brown, C.E. (1974). The Kansas City Preventive Patrol Experiment: A technical report. Washington, DC: The Police-Foundation. Larson, R. C. (1975). What happened to patrol operations in Kansas City. Journal of criminal justice. 31267-97. Law Enforcement Assistance Administration (1975). Criminal victimization in the United States: 1973 advance report. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice. - (1976). Criminal victimization in the United States: A comparison of 1973 and 1974jindings. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice. McClintock, F.H., and Gibson, E. (1960). Robbery,in London. London: Macmillan. McDonald, J. M. (1975). Armed robbery: Offenders and their victims. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. National Academy of Sciences (1976). Surveying crime: Report of the Panel for rhe Evaluation of Crime Surveys. Washington, DC: Committee on National Statistics, National Academy of Sciences. Owen, G. (1970). Mathematics for the social and management sciences: Finite mathematics. Phila- delphia: W.B. Saunders.
  • 64. Pate, T.; Bowers, R.; and Parks, R. (1976). Three approaches to criminal apprehension in Kansas City: An evaluation report. Washington, DC: The Police Foundation. Information, Apprehension and Deterrence: Exploring the Limits of Police Productivity 241 Pittman, D. J., and Handy, W. (1964). Patterns in criminal aggravated assault. Journ ofcriminal law,, criminology and police science, 551462-70. President’s Commission on Law Enforcement and Administration of Justice (1%7). Task force report: Crime and its assessment. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Reiss, A. (1971). The police and the public. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Repetto. T. A. (1974). Residential crime. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger Publishing Co. Rubinstein, J. (1973). City police. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. Sagalyn, A. (1971). The crime ofrobbery in the United States. Washington, DC: Law Enforcement Assistance Administration, U.S. Department of Justice. Sanders, W. (1977). Detective work: A study of criminal investigations. New York: Free Press. Seidman. D., and Couzens, M. (1975). Getting the crime rate
  • 65. down: Political pressure and crime reporting. Law and society review, 8:457-93. Skogan, W. G. (197Q). Citizen reporting of crime: Some National Panel data. Criminology, 1353% 49. - (1976b). Crime and crime rates. In W. G. Skogan (ed.) Sample surveys of the victims of crime. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger Publishing Co., pp. 105-20. - (1976~) Sample surveys of the victims of crime. Review of public data use, 4:23-28. - (1976d). The victims of crime: Some National Survey findings. In A. L. Guenther (ed.) Criminal behavior and social systems. Chicago: Rand McNally, pp. 131-48. Stinchcombe, A. L. (1963). Institutions of privacy in the determination of police practices. Ameri- can journal of sociology, 69: 150-60. Tien. J.; Simon, J.; and Larson, R. (1978). An alternative approach in police patrol: The Wilming ton splif-force experiment. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Turner, A. (1972). San Jose methods test of known crime victims. Washington, DC: Law Enforce- ment Assistance Administration. Vera Institute of Justice (1976). Felony arrests: Their prosecution and disposition in New York Cify’s courts. New York: Vera Institute of Justice.
  • 66. Ward, R.; Ward, T.; and Feeley, J. (1975). Police robbery control manual. Washington, DC: National Institute of Law Enforcement and Criminal Justice. Wolfgang, M. E. (1958). Patterns in criminal homicide. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. - ( 1963). Uniform crime reports: A critical appraisal. Universiw of Pennsylvania law review, 11:708-38. Citation: 13 Am. J. Police 149 1994 Content downloaded/printed from HeinOnline (http://heinonline.org) Wed Jul 8 16:55:15 2015 -- Your use of this HeinOnline PDF indicates your acceptance of HeinOnline's Terms and Conditions of the license agreement available at http://heinonline.org/HOL/License -- The search text of this PDF is generated from uncorrected OCR text. American Journal of Police Vol. XIII, No. 3 1994 149 THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN EVIDENCE, DETECTIVE EFFORT, AND THE DISPOSITION OF BURGLARY AND ROBBERY INVESTIGATIONS
  • 67. Steven G. Brandl University of Wisconsin - Milwaukee James Frank University of Cincinnati The police detective is commonly cast by the media and de- tective fiction as a highly skilled and intelligent investigator who, through the use of complex and extraordinary means, is capable of solving virtually any crime (Klockars, 1985; see also Garofalo, 1981). The public, in turn, has largely accepted this portrayal as an accurate reflection of reality (Wilson, 1975). Further, the police organization reflects and perpetuates this institutionalized belief by dividing the in- vestigative task among detectives, who specialize in investigating crimes, and patrol officers, who perform the countless other tasks asso- ciated with police work. With the appearance of empirical inquiries into the operations of the criminal investigation process, the capabilities of detectives in investigating crimes have been directly questioned. However, not only have these studies been relatively few in number, but they have also produced contradictory findings. For example, Greenwood, Chaiken, and Petersilia (1977), explain that "detective work is not
  • 68. characterized by hard work leading to case solutions . . ." rather, "the cases that get cleared are primarily the easy ones to solve" (p. 112). Thus, "any justi- fication for the work of [detectives] must lie in areas other than crime solution" (p. 141). On the other hand, Eck (1983) argues that "the work of... detectives is extremely important with respect to the subsequent making of follow-up arrests" (p. 243). As a result of the divergent 150 American Journal of Police Vol. XIII, No. 3 1994 findings, definitive conclusions concerning the contributions of detec- tives in solving crimes cannot be drawn. The analyses reported in this study are intended to cast addi- tional light on the debate concerning the extent to which detective ef- fort contributes to the identification and apprehension of criminals. Specifically, we test hypotheses that link burglary and robbery follow- up investigation arrests to the amount of time spent on follow- up in- vestigations and the strength of the evidence associated with the cases. The study addresses some shortcomings evident in the prior research and provides some empirical evidence where speculation has
  • 69. gone be- fore. In so doing, the study represents an extension of previous re- search. PREVIOUS RESEARCH In the first study to examine the contribution of detectives in solving crimes, Isaacs (1967) analyzed data on 1,905 crimes reported to two of 15 Los Angeles Police Department field divisions in January 1966. Isaacs found that of the 1,905 crimes, 336 (18%) resulted in an actual arrest while 146 (8%) resulted in "other clearances" (e.g., victim refused to prosecute, unfounded). Of the 336 cases that resulted in an arrest, only 27 (8%) were as a result of an arrest by a detective. Based on the limited statistical analyses performed on the data, the author concluded that "[o]ne of the most significant characteristics of a case, affecting its chances of being cleared, is whether the suspect was named in the crime report by the victim" (p. 96). For example, of the 1,905 crimes, 1,556 (82%) involved suspects who were not named in the initial crime report. Of these 1,556 cases, 1,375 (88%) were not cleared. Conversely, of the 1,905 crimes, 349 (18%) involved suspects who were named in the crime report. Of these cases, only 48 (14%)
  • 70. were not cleared. Greenwood (1970), in an analysis of burglary, robbery, and grand larceny crimes that were reported to several precincts of the New York City Police Department during the first six months of 1967, also found that when a suspect could be named by the victim, the chances of an arrest were significantly greater compared to when only a descrip- tion was provided, or other evidence was available. For example, the analyses showed that approximately nine percent of reported robberies resulted in an on-scene arrest while the remaining 91 percent were ini- American Journal of Police Vol. XIII, No. 3 1994 151 tially unsolved. Of the unsolved cases, the victim could name a suspect in about two percent and could describe the suspect in 67 percent. For those cases with a named suspect, 46 percent resulted in an arrest, while only two percent of the cases with a description resulted in an ar- rest. Once again, circumstances-namely, the victim providing the suspect's name-were portrayed as determining the outcome of the case.
  • 71. The study conducted by Greenwood, Chaiken, and Petersilia of the RAND Corporation in 1977 supported, and elaborated on, the conclusions of Isaacs (1967) and Greenwood (1970). Their results were derived from analyses of primarily two sets of data. First, based on an analysis of the computer-readable case assignment file main- tained by the Kansas City (MO) Police Department, the researchers provided insight into how detectives spend their working time. In comparing the amount of time spent on uncleared cases with the amount of time spent on cleared cases, they found that the "time spent on uncleared cases [was] significantly higher than time spent prior to clearance on cleared cases with no initial patrol arrest" (p. 115).1 On the basis of this comparison, they conclude that there are basically two types of investigations: "Some investigations are lengthy but do not re- sult in a clearance. Others lead to a clearance after a small amount of work but entail substantial efforts by the investigator once the crime is cleared" (p. 116). Thus, "the cases that get cleared are primarily the easy ones to solve" (p. 112). Second, a post hoc analysis of cleared crimes (homicide, ag- gravated assault, felony morals, robbery, burglary, theft, auto theft, and forgery/fraud) from five police departments (Berkeley, Long
  • 72. Beach, Los Angeles, Miami, and Washington D.C.) 2 provided a means by which to describe the method in which cleared cases were solved. Greenwood et al. (1977) found that about 22 percent (38 of 172) of the cleared crimes resulted from an arrest by a patrol officer at the scene of the crime. In another 44 percent (76 of 172), the perpetrator was "known" when the crime report was first taken and, as described by the researchers, "the main job for the investigator in these cases [was] to locate and apprehend the perpetrator, and to assemble evidence ade- quate to charge him" (p. 135). The remaining 34 percent (58 of 172) of all cleared crimes were cleared primarily as a result of "simple routine actions" of detectives (e.g., showing mug shot books, acting on precise and detailed informant tips). According to Greenwood et al. (1977), only about three percent of these cleared crimes could be attributed to 152 American Journal of Police Vol. XIII, No. 3 1994 "special actions" of detectives (e.g., preparation of investigative bul- letins, matching latent prints to inked prints in a file organized by M.O.).
  • 73. Like Isaacs (1967) and Greenwood (1970), Greenwood et al. (1977) explain that the overwhelming majority of solved crimes are solved because of arrests by patrol officers or because the identity of the perpetrator is known when the crime report reached the detective. "If information that uniquely identifies the perpetrator is not presented at the time the crime is reported, the perpetrator, by and large, will not be subsequently identified" (Greenwood, Chaiken, and Petersilia, 1976:65). Thus, "it is not true in general that the greater the investiga- tive effort devoted to a crime the more likely it will be cleared" (p. 64). Eck (1983) provides contrast with Isaacs (1967), Greenwood (1970), and Greenwood et al. (1977) by emphasizing, or at least not minimizing, the importance of detective effort in solving crimes. Us- ing data collected from three police departments (DeKalb Co., GA, St. Petersburg, FL, and Wichita, KS), he examined activities performed by detectives and patrol officers to produce crime and/or suspect informa- tion, and the relationship between this information and a suspect being arrested. The study focused on burglaries and robberies. Eck found that certain types of information produced during the preliminary in-
  • 74. vestigation (i.e., information pertaining to the suspect) were predictive of whether or not an arrest was made during the follow-up investigation as were some of the activities performed by detectives during the fol- low-up investigation (e.g., record checks, informant interviews). As such, Eck provides evidence to suggest that circumstances of criminal incidents as well as efforts of detectives contribute to the making of ar- rests during follow-up investigations. On the basis of his analyses, Eck (1983) proposed what he re- ferred to as the "triage hypothesis" (see also Eck, 1979). According to this hypothesis, cases can be grouped into three categories depending on the strength of the evidence (i.e., favorable circumstances) associ- ated with the case. The hypothesis predicts that, first, cases with weak evidence will rarely conclude with an arrest, regardless of the inves- tigative effort or time allocated to them. Second, cases with moderate evidence, where there are sufficient leads to suggest that the cases could be solved, will often result in an arrest but only if effort is allo- cated to them. Third, cases with strong evidence, where the suspect has been positively identified, will usually result in an arrest, even with only minimal investigative effort. Accordingly, based on this
  • 75. hypothe- American Journal of Police Vol. XIII, No. 3 1994 153 sis, circumstances determine the outcome of the cases in the first and third group and thus, for these cases, the probability of an arrest does not increase with more time spent. However, effort determines the out- come of the cases in the second group and as a result, for these cases, the probability of an arrest does increase with more time spent on the investigation. Therefore, like Greenwood et al. (1977), Eck implies that, in general, the likelihood of an arrest may not increase with more effort devoted to the case. However, Eck (1983) suggests that this re- lationship--that more time spent on an investigation increases the probability of an arrest--does hold true for cases with moderate suspect information. And, as hypothesized by Eck (1979), it is cases in this second category that have the most time devoted to them. There are several issues that need to be considered when judging the overall validity of the conclusions offered by the previous research. First, Isaacs (1967) and some of the analyses performed by Greenwood et al. (1977:97-119) used whether cases were "cleared" as