SlideShare una empresa de Scribd logo
1 de 27
Descargar para leer sin conexión
Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1002092
Why Inexperienced Investors Do Not Learn:
They Do Not Know Their Past Portfolio Performance
Markus Glaser and Martin Weber∗
Final Version, Finance Research Letters 4(4), 203-216
Winner of the “Ross Best Paper Award” for
the best paper published in “Finance Research Letters” in 2007
Abstract
Recently, researchers have gone a step further from just documenting biases of individual investors.
More and more studies analyze how experience affects decisions and whether biases are eliminated
by trading experience and learning. A necessary condition to learn is that investors actually know
what happened in the past and that the views of the past are not biased. We contribute to the
above mentioned literature by showing why learning and experience go hand in hand. Inexperienced
investors are not able to give a reasonable self-assessment of their own past realized stock portfolio
performance which impedes investors’ learning ability. Based on the answers of 215 online broker
investors to an internet questionnaire, we analyze whether investors are able to correctly estimate
their own realized stock portfolio performance. We show that investors are hardly able to give a correct
estimate of their own past realized stock portfolio performance and that experienced investors are
better able to do so. In general, we can conclude that we find evidence that investor experience
lessens the simple mathematical error of estimating portfolio returns, but seems not to influence their
“behavioral” mistakes pertaining to how good (in absolute sense or relative to other investors) they
are.
Keywords: Return Estimation, Portfolio Return, Perceived Returns, Self-Assessment, Better Than Aver-
age Effect, Overconfidence, Financial Education, Financial Literacy, Learning, Experience
JEL Classification Code: D8, G1
∗Markus Glaser is from the Lehrstuhl f¨ur Bankbetriebslehre at the Business School, Universit¨at Mannheim, L 5, 2, 68131
Mannheim. E-Mail: glaser@bank.BWL.uni-mannheim.de. Martin Weber is from the Lehrstuhl f¨ur Bankbetriebslehre at the
Business School, Universit¨at Mannheim, L 5, 2, 68131 Mannheim and CEPR, London. E-Mail: weber@bank.BWL.uni-
mannheim.de. We would like to thank an anonymous referee for helpful comments. This paper was in parts written while
Markus Glaser was visiting the Swedish Institute for Financial Research (SIFR) in Stockholm whose support is gratefully
acknowledged. Financial Support from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) is also gratefully acknowledged.
1
Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1002092
1 Introduction
Recently, researchers have gone a step further from just documenting biases of individual
investors. More and more studies analyze how experience affects decisions and whether
biases are eliminated by trading experience and learning. Consider, for example, one of
the most extensively studied biases of individual investors, the disposition effect. Feng and
Seasholes (2005) analyze the disposition effect, the investor’s reluctance to realize losses
and his propensity to realize gains, and find that experience eliminates the reluctance to
realize losses. Seru, Shumway, and Stoffman (2007) analyze 22 million trades from more
than one million individuals in Finland from 1995 to 2003 and also find that the disposition
effect falls, and performance improves, as investors become more experienced. Dhar and
Zhu (2006) use demographic and socioeconomic variables as proxies for investor literacy,
and find empirical evidence that wealthier individuals exhibit a lower disposition effect.
Weber and Welfens (2007) present empirical and experimental evidence that learning
attenuates the magnitude of the disposition effect. Consistent with the studies above,
trading frequency also tends to reduce the disposition effect. Kaustia and Kn¨upfer (2007)
document a strong link between personal experience with IPOs and future subscriptions.
Greenwood and Nagel (2007) find that around the peak of the stock market bubble in
the year 2000, mutual funds run by inexperienced managers were more heavily invested
in technology stocks. Nicolosi, Peng, and Zhu (2007) also present evidence that individual
investors learn from past trading experience.
A necessary condition to learn is that investors actually know what happened in the past
and that the views of the past are not biased. We contribute to the above mentioned lit-
erature by showing why learning and experience go hand in hand. Inexperienced investors
2
are not able to give a reasonable self-assessment of their own past realized stock portfolio
performance which impedes investors’ learning ability.
Based on the answers of 215 online broker investors to an internet questionnaire we
analyze whether investors are able to correctly estimate their own realized stock portfolio
performance. Portfolio returns are calculated with the help of the stock portfolio positions
of this investor group over the years preceding the questionnaire. Furthermore, we compare
their perceived performance percentile with the actual performance percentile. Moreover,
we analyze determinants of the cross-sectional heterogeneity in a regression analysis.
The first focus of our paper is on the absolute difference between estimated and realized
performance. A potential difference is presumably mainly driven by lack of knowledge or
mathematical skills. No behavioral factors (should) come into play in this mathematical
exercise. Thus, it is intuitive that expertise might play a role in explaining potential
heterogeneity across investors.
Furthermore, we analyze whether investors overestimate their past realized performance
and their performance relative to other investors. We thus also contribute to the literature
on overconfidence. The facets of overconfidence usually studied in the literature are (see
Glaser, N¨oth, and Weber (2004), Glaser and Weber (2007), or Moore and Healy (2007)):
1. overestimation of one’s actual performance,
2. overplacement of one’s performance relative to others, also called the better than
average effect, and
3. excessive precision in one’s belief, also called miscalibration.
Empirical and experimental studies show that these facets of overconfidence are hardly
3
correlated (see Glaser and Weber (2007) and Moore and Healy (2007) and the references
cited therein). The first two facets of overconfidence can be regarded as a psychological
foundation of differences of opinion models in finance (see Hong and Stein (2007)) while the
last facet resembles the way overconfidence is modeled in finance with investors inferring
a higher signal-to-noise ratio in market news than is statistically appropriate with the
consequence of too tight prediction intervals (see, for example, the models by Odean
(1998) and Gervais and Odean (2001), and the survey by Glaser, N¨oth, and Weber (2004)).
By analyzing the determinants of the first two out of the three above mentioned facets
of overconfidence, we contribute to the still scarce literature on the demographics of
overconfidence in the spirit of Bhandari and Deaves (2006).
Our main results can be summarized as follows. Investors are hardly able to give a cor-
rect estimate of their own past realized stock portfolio performance over the past four
years. The correlation coefficient between return estimates and realized returns is not dis-
tinguishable from zero. Furthermore, people overrate themselves. On average, investors
think, that they are better than others. The correlation between self ratings and actual
performance is also not distinguishable from zero. High past realized stock portfolio per-
formance does not make investors overconfident in the sense that they rate themselves
as better than other investors. In other words, investors who think that they had above
average performance actually did not have above average performance in the past. In-
vestors with higher stock market investment experience and higher past portfolio returns
are better able to estimate their past realized stock portfolio performance.
The rest of the paper is organized as follows. In Section 2, we present the data sets
analyzed and the design of the study. In Section 3, we analyze the correlation between
return estimates and actual past realized portfolio returns. Results on the correlation
4
between perceived performance percentile and actual performance percentile are presented
in Section 4. Section 5 contains regression results on the determinants of cross-sectional
heterogeneity in the answers provided. The last section summarizes and discusses the
results and concludes.
2 Data Sets and the Design of the Study
This study is based on the combination of several data sets. The main data set consists
of 563,104 buy and sell transactions of 3,079 individual investors from a German online
broker in the period from January 1997 to mid April 2001. We considered all investors
who trade via the internet, had opened their account prior to January 1997, had at least
one transaction in 1997, and have an e-mail-address. The second data set consists of sev-
eral demographic and other self-reported information (age, gender, investment strategy,
investment experience), that was collected by the online broker at the time each investor
opened her or his account. The third data set consists of the answers to an online ques-
tionnaire (see Glaser and Weber (2005) and the next section for details). Data on the
securities traded are obtained from Datastream, our fourth data source. In August and
September 2001, our investor sample received an email from the online broker with a link
to an online questionnaire. 215 investors answered the questionnaire.1
Glaser and Weber
(2007) show that there is no indication of a sample selection bias. The results of this paper
are based on parts of this questionnaire and will be discussed in the following sections.
We calculate the monthly gross portfolio performance of each investor making the follow-
ing simplifying assumptions: We assume that all stocks are bought and sold at the end of
1See Glaser and Weber (2005) for details about this questionnaire.
5
the month and we ignore intra-month trading. Barber and Odean (2000) show that these
simplifying assumptions do not bias the measurement of portfolio performance. The gross
portfolio return Rht of investor h in month t is calculated as follows:
Rht =
Sht
i=1
wihtRit with wiht =
Pitniht
Sht
i=1
Pitniht
(1)
Rit is the return of stock i in month t, Sht is the number of type of stocks held by individual
h in month t, Pit is the price of stock i at the beginning of month t, and niht is the number
of stocks of company i held by investor h in month t. wiht is the beginning-of-month-t
market value of the holding of stock i of investor h divided by the beginning-of-month-t
market value of the whole stock portfolio of investor h.
Table 1 shows the results for all investors and the subgroup of respondents to the ques-
tionnaire. The cross-sectional distribution of the monthly gross returns is similar to the
results in Barber and Odean (2000), Table IV, p. 791. We observe a large cross-sectional
variation in the performance across investors. When we exclude investors with stock po-
sitions in 12 or fewer months, we find gross returns between −16% and +24% per month.
On average, investors underperform relevant benchmarks. For example, the arithmetic
average monthly return of the German blue chip index DAX from January 1997 to March
2001 is 2.02% whereas the mean gross monthly return of investors in our data set is 0.54%.
Furthermore, parametric and non-parametric tests show that the distribution of monthly
returns is not significantly different in the two groups. Thus, there is no indication of a
sample selection bias.2
2Glaser and Weber (2007) show that this is also true for all other variables used later in the present paper.
6
3 Do Investors Know Their Past Portfolio Returns?
In this section, we present survey evidence on investors’ ability to give an estimate of their
own past realized stock portfolio performance. We asked the investors to give an estimate
of their portfolio performance in the past (from January 1997 to December 2000):
Please try to estimate your past performance of your stock portfolio at your
online broker. Please estimate the return of your stock portfolio from January
1997 to December 2000:
[Answer] percent per year on average.
Table 2 presents the results. 210 of 215 investors who answered at least one question
answered the question presented above. The investors think, on average, that their own
realized stock portfolio performance from January 1997 to December 2000 was about 15
% per year. There is a large variation in the answers to this questions. The answers range
from −50% to +120%.
Figure 1 plots the realized portfolio returns versus return estimates of the individual
investors who answered the questionnaire (variables are winsorized at the 10 percent level).
The correlation coefficient between return estimates and realized returns is −0.0471 (p =
0.5203). This complete lack of correlation might seem extremely surprising. But another
study that uses a design similar to ours documents exactly the same findings. Owhoso and
Weickgenannt (2007) investigate the extent to which auditors’ ratings of self-perceived
abilities correspond with their actual performance, and whether these perceptions are
influenced by audit experience and effectiveness when conducting audits within their
domain of specialization. 144 industry-specialized audit seniors and managers reviewed
7
two sets of audit working paper cases. At the end of the review, the auditors rated their
ability to perform an audit in their domain. One result is that there is no significant
positive correlation between auditors’ self-perceived abilities and actual performance.
The difference between return estimates and realized returns is positive (mean and median
are higher than 10 percentage points per year, see also Table 3). The difference is highly
significantly positive (p < 0.0001). This finding is consistent with Figure 1 which shows
that most dots lie below the 45-degree line. Thus, many investors believe that they made
money although they did not. This finding is consistent with psychological evidence that
people overstate past performance in a variety of tasks (see Dunning, Heath, and Suls
(2004), Moore and Healy (2007), and Owhoso and Weickgenannt (2007)).
Why is there no correlation between realized portfolio returns and return estimates? One
interpretation is that investors do not have a good understanding of the concept “return”.
Another explanation is the way the online broker presents returns. Usually, the online
broker presents gains and losses (with the buying price as the reference point) for every
stock in the portfolio separately which makes it difficult to estimate the monthly or yearly
stock portfolio performance. The broker also presents the total value of the portfolio,
day by day. However, still, it is hard to calculate the performance of the portfolio when
investors are continuously buying and selling stocks. When stocks are bought every month
with additional money from, say, a cash account the stock portfolio value can increase
although the average stock had negative returns. The information that should be relevant
to judge own stock selection ability, the own past realized stock portfolio performance, is
not calculated by the online broker.
8
The results in this subsection are related to the experimental literature which shows that
individuals in general are poor at recalling price changes when compared to recalling
prices. Andreassen (1988) finds in an experiment that errors recalling price changes were
significantly larger than those made in recalling prices. He argues that subjects pay greater
attention to prices than to price changes. This result is in line with Glaser, Langer,
Reynders, and Weber (2007) who show that a group of students has bigger problems
stating return forecasts for financial time series when compared to price forecasts.
Table 3 also shows that experienced investors are better able to estimate their past realized
stock portfolio performance. The difference between the perceived return and the actual
return is significantly lower for investors with more than 5 years of investment experience.3
Furthermore, the percentage of investors who estimate at least the right sign of their
past realized portfolio performance is higher for experienced investors. Moreover, more
experienced investors are reasonably close with their estimates (see the lines in the table
which show the number of investors who are less than 5 percentage points or 10 percentage
points wrong). These findings might explain why the studies mentioned in the Introduction
find that experienced investors make better decisions.
These results are supported by Amromin and Sharpe (2006). They examine answers to the
following question: “Thinking about a diversified portfolio of stocks, what would you guess
was the average annual return earned over the past 10 years?” from the Michigan Survey
of Consumer Attitudes, conducted by the Survey Research Center (SRC) at the University
of Michigan. In particular, they calculate the absolute value of the recall error, i.e. the
difference between recalled and actual 10-year market returns, and regress this difference
3We use a cutoff of 5 years as this is the median level of experience so that we obtain two groups of approximately equal
size.
9
on demographic and stock ownership characteristics. They find that the accuracy of a
respondent’s recall of past returns improves with both wealth and education, as well as
other indicators of financial market knowledge.
To summarize, the main result of this section is that investors are hardly able to give a cor-
rect estimate of their own past realized stock portfolio performance and that experienced
investors are better able to do so.
4 Self-Rating and Actual Performance
Furthermore, we asked the following question to analyze investors’ self-ratings and their
relation with actual performance.
What percentage of customers of your discount brokerage house had higher
returns than you in the four-year period from January 1997 to December 2000?
(Please give a number between 0 % and 100 %)
[Answer] percent of other customers had higher returns than I did.
Table 4 presents the results. The mean is 46.99 indicating a slight better than average
effect. This number is significantly different from 50 (p = 0.0335, Wilcoxon signed-rank
test).4
We are thus able to confirm prior literature on the better than average effect
(Taylor and Brown (1988), Svenson (1981)). One reason for the finding that this number
is so close to 50 might be that about about 30 % of all investors classify themselves as
4Note that in Table 3 the results are slightly different as we show results for the subgroup of respondents for which we
have data on investment experience in that table.
10
average, i.e. state 50 as an answer.5
Figure 2 plots the self-ratings in percentiles versus actual return percentiles of the individ-
ual investors who answered the questionnaire. Such a graph is often used in the literature
(see for example Ackerman, Beier, and Bowen (2002)). The figure shows that there is no
relation between the self-ratings in percentiles and actual return percentiles. The corre-
lation between the self-ratings and actual percentiles is −0.0110 (p = 0.8810) which is
not significantly distinguishable from zero. We are thus able to confirm prior research
which shows that a correlation between self-ratings in percentiles and objective measures
in percentiles is not existent (see Larrick, Burson, and Soll (2007) for further references
and Dunning, Heath, and Suls (2004) for a recent survey).
Furthermore, the difference between the actual return percentile of the respective investor
and the self-assessed percentile is positive on average (this difference is positive if an
investor thinks, for example, that only 25% of the other investors had higher portfolio
returns in the past even though 30 % of the investors in the sample actually had higher
returns). Thus, investors overestimate their relative position in terms of return percentiles.
Moreover, high returns in the past do not lead to high overconfidence as measured by
perceived percentile in our questionnaire at the end of the sample period. Thus, we do not
find support for the learning-to-be-overconfident hypothesis (Gervais and Odean (2001)),
i.e. a high degree of overconfidence as a result of past investment success. We argue, that
one reason is, that investors do not know their past realized stock portfolio performance
5Furthermore, recent studies show that the better than average effect is not as universal as was previously documented
in the literature (see Moore (2007), Moore and Cain (2007), and Moore and Small (2007)). Thus, our small better than
average effect is not a puzzle.
11
as was presented in the previous section.6
Note, however, that Gervais and Odean (2001)
model the third of the manifestations of overconfidence mentioned in the Introduction
while we are analyzing the first two facets in this paper.
However, there is also a further interpretation of these findings. We find that the correla-
tion between investors’ self-assessed absolute performance and their self-assessed relative
performance is 0.2704 (with a p-value of 0.0002). Therefore, investors are somehow con-
sistent in their answers. This raises the question “which” returns are actually relevant
for overconfidence. It is possible that not the actual realized returns are relevant for the
learning-to-be-overconfident hypothesis but the perceived realized returns. It is possible
that investors “feel overconfident” even without knowing the true performance, by simply
allowing their overblown beliefs of own realized portfolio returns to influence their view
of returns relative to others. Thus, it is intriguing that actual returns are uncorrelated
with the self-perceived ranking, but perceived returns are. To summarize, investors who
believe they have done well in the absolute sense, also believe they have done better than
others. Which returns are actually more relevant for overconfidence is a question for future
research.
6These results do not contradict the studies of Statman, Thorley, and Vorkink (2006) or Glaser and Weber (2008).
These studies find that returns over the past 6 months positively influence trading activity which is consistent with the
learning-to-be-overconfident hypothesis. Statman, Thorley, and Vorkink (2006) find, however, that returns with lags larger
than 6 do not influence trading volume anymore. In connection with the findings presented in this study, we can conclude
that learning-to-be-overconfident stories are more appropriate for the effects of past returns over shorter horizons than the
four year horizon which we analyze in the present study.
12
5 Which Investors are Able to Correctly Estimate Their Past
Realized Portfolio Performance?
Table 5 presents cross-sectional regression results on the determinants of the absolute
difference between return estimates and realized returns (Regressions (1) and (2)), the
difference between return estimates and realized returns (Regressions (3) and (4)), the
absolute difference between perceived and actual return percentile (Regressions (5) and
(6)), and the difference between actual and perceived return percentile (Regressions (7)
and (8)) as dependent variables and stock market investment experience, a gender dummy
variable, age, a mutual fund investor dummy, a warrant trader dummy variable, a high
risk dummy, the logarithm of mean monthly stock portfolio value, the time-series average
of the monthly stock portfolio performance of an investor, the logarithm of the standard
deviation of monthly stock portfolio performance as a measure of portfolio risk, and the
logarithm of number of stocks in portfolio.7
Stock market investment experience, gender
and the high risk dummy are based on a voluntary self-report made by investors at the
time the respective account was opened. This information was not updated afterwards by
the online broker. The dependent variables and the monthly stock portfolio performance
are winsorized at the 10 percent level. The table reports standardized beta coefficients
(except for the intercept). Robust p-values are in parentheses.
Regression (1) shows that stock market investment experience has a significantly negative
effect on the absolute difference between return estimates and realized returns at the 1
percent level. This finding can be interpreted in the way that investors learn how to bet-
7We use the natural logarithm of variables that are positively skewed. Tests show, that we thus avoid problems like
non-normality, non-linearity, and heteroscedasticity in the cross-sectional regression analysis. See Spanos (1986), chapter
21, especially, pp. 455-456, Davidson and MacKinnon (1993), chapter 14, and Atkinson (1985), pp. 80-81.
13
ter judge their own past realized stock portfolio performance over time. Stock portfolio
performance is also negatively related to the absolute difference between return estimates
and realized returns. In other words, the lower the returns the worse investors are when
judging their realized returns. There are several interpretations of this result. On the one
hand, investors may look at their portfolio less often when returns are negative and, as
a consequence, they do not know how bad they have actually performed. On the other
hand, it is possible, that investors do not want to admit that they have performed pretty
badly. This is consistent with psychological studies showing that people often neglect bad
outcomes or unfavorable experience (see Dunning, Heath, and Suls (2004) for a survey).
Karlsson, Loewenstein, and Seppi (2005), for example, present related evidence that in-
vestors check the value of their portfolios more frequently in rising markets but “put their
heads in the sand” when markets are flat or falling. This finding is therefore sometimes
called the “Ostrich Effect”. Furthermore, Table 2 shows that only less than 5 percent
of our investors think they had negative returns in the past while more than 25 percent
actually had negative returns in the past. Thus, somehow mechanically, investors with
high past returns are closer to their self-assessment on average. This is why we re-run
the regression without stock portfolio performance as explanatory variable (see Regres-
sion (2)). The results are similar. Stock market experience remains highly significantly
negative at the 5 percent level.8
Furthermore, portfolio risk has a positive effect on the absolute difference between return
8All results are similar when we use a winsorization at the 2 percent or 5 percent level. For example, when variables
are winsorized at the 5 percent level, the beta coefficient for experience is -0.185 with a p-value of 0.018 in Regression 1
and -0.171 (p-value = 0.041) in Regression 2. When we use quantile regressions, the significance of the experience variable
in Regression 1 is even stronger (p-value = 0.003). In Regression 2, the experience variable remains significant at the 10
percent level.
14
estimates and realized returns. This result is intuitive. The higher the standard deviation
of returns, the more difficult is it to calculate the past realized stock portfolio performance.
All the other variables are not robustly related to the dependent variable. Regression (3)
shows that experienced investors and mutual fund investors are less likely to overesti-
mate their past realized portfolio performance. However, this effect is not significant in
Regression (4) anymore.
Regressions (5) to (8) present the determinants of the (absolute) difference between per-
ceived and actual percentile. Compared to Regressions (1) to (4), the adjusted R-squared
values are quite low. Furthermore, we do not find a robust influence of our explanatory
variables on the (absolute) difference between perceived and actual percentile.
To summarize, we find that experience helps in calculating the own past realized portfolio
performance. This task should be mainly driven by skills that are enhanced by invest-
ment experience. In contrast, the other measures analyzed in Regressions (3) to (8) are
closely related to the manifestations of overconfidence mentioned in the Introduction,
especially overestimation of one’s actual performance and overplacement of one’s perfor-
mance relative to others. The regression analysis in this part is exploratory in the spirit
of Bhandari and Deaves (2006) who analyze the demographics of overconfidence. We had
no ex ante hypothesis of the effect of expertise (and the other variables) on these over-
confidence measures. Our analysis shows that our explanatory variables are not related
to these overconfidence measures. In general, we can conclude that we find evidence that
investor experience lessens the simple mathematical error of estimating portfolio returns,
but seems not to influence their “behavioral” mistakes pertaining to how good (in absolute
or relative sense) they are.
15
6 Summary, Discussion, and Conclusion
Based on the answers of 215 online broker investors to an internet questionnaire we
analyze whether investors are able to correctly estimate their own realized stock portfolio
performance. Furthermore, we compare their perceived performance percentile with the
actual performance percentile. Moreover, we analyze determinants of the cross-sectional
heterogeneity in a regression analysis. The main findings can be summarized as follows.
Investors are hardly able to give a correct estimate of their own past realized stock portfolio
performance. Experienced investors are better able to do so. Furthermore, people overrate
themselves. On average, investors think, that they are better than others. Moreover, the
correlation between self ratings and actual performance is not distinguishable from zero.
We find that investors do not have a good understanding of the concept “return”. This
result is consistent with other studies. Parts of our results can be explained by psycholog-
ical reasons (such as the negative influence of past portfolio performance on the absolute
difference between return estimates and realized returns). However, this is only one part
of the story. We also find that stock market investment experience has a positive influ-
ence on the quality of estimates of past realized stock portfolio returns. This is consistent
with other studies that document a positive effect of financial education on behavior. As
investors are increasingly encouraged or even forced to invest for their own retirement
savings, a good understanding of returns is essential. Future research should further in-
vestigate why people have problems dealing with returns and how these problems can be
mitigated.
16
References
Ackerman, P. L., M. E. Beier, and K. R. Bowen, 2002, “What we really know about our
abilities and our knowledge,” Personality and Individual Differences, 33(4), 587–605.
Amromin, G., and S. Sharpe, 2006, “From the Horse’s Mouth: Gauging Conditional Ex-
pected Stock Returns from Investor Surveys,” Working paper, Federal Reserve Bank of
Chicago.
Andreassen, P. B., 1988, “Explaining the Price-Volume Relationship: The Difference be-
tween Price Changes and Changing Prices,” Organizational Behavior and Human De-
cision Processes, 41(3), 371–389.
Atkinson, A., 1985, Plots, Transformations, and Regression. Clarendon Press, Oxford.
Barber, B. M., and T. Odean, 2000, “Trading Is Hazardous to Your Wealth: The Common
Stock Investment Performance of Individual Investors,” Journal of Finance, 55(2), 773–
806.
Bhandari, G., and R. Deaves, 2006, “The Demographics of Overconfidence,” Journal of
Behavioral Finance, 7(1), 5–11.
Davidson, R., and J. G. MacKinnon, 1993, Estimation and Inference in Econometrics.
Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Dhar, R., and N. Zhu, 2006, “Up Close and Personal: Investor Sophistication and the
Disposition Effect,” Management Science, 52(5), 726–740.
Dunning, D., C. Heath, and J. M. Suls, 2004, “Flawed Self-Assessment: Implications for
Health, Education, and the Workplace,” Psychological Science in the Public Interest,
5(3), 69–106.
17
Feng, L., and M. S. Seasholes, 2005, “Do Investor Sophistication and Trading Experience
Eliminate Behavioral Biases in Financial Markets?,” Review of Finance, 9(3), 305–351.
Gervais, S., and T. Odean, 2001, “Learning to Be Overconfident,” Review of Financial
Studies, 14(1), 1–27.
Glaser, M., T. Langer, J. Reynders, and M. Weber, 2007, “Framing Effects in Stock
Market Forecasts: The Difference Between Asking for Prices and Asking for Returns,”
Review of Finance, 11(2), 325–357.
Glaser, M., M. N¨oth, and M. Weber, 2004, “Behavioral Finance,” in Blackwell Handbook
of Judgment and Decision Making, ed. by D. J. Koehler, and N. Harvey. Blackwell,
Malden, Mass., pp. 527–546.
Glaser, M., and M. Weber, 2005, “September 11 and Stock Return Expectations of Indi-
vidual Investors,” Review of Finance, 9(2), 243–279.
, 2007, “Overconfidence and Trading Volume,” Geneva Risk and Insurance Review,
32(1), 1–36.
, 2008, “Which Past Returns Affect Trading Volume?,” Journal of Financial Mar-
kets, forthcoming.
Greenwood, R., and S. Nagel, 2007, “Inexperienced Investors and Bubbles,” Working
paper.
Hong, H., and J. C. Stein, 2007, “Disagreement and the Stock Market,” Journal of Eco-
nomic Perspectives, 21(2), 109–128.
Karlsson, N., G. Loewenstein, and D. Seppi, 2005, “The “Ostrich Effect”: Selective At-
tention to Information about Investments,” Working paper.
18
Kaustia, M., and S. Kn¨upfer, 2007, “Do Investors Learn from Personal Experience? Evi-
dence from IPO Subscriptions,” Working paper, Helsinki School of Economics.
Larrick, R. P., K. A. Burson, and J. B. Soll, 2007, “Social comparison and confidence:
When thinking you’re better than average predicts overconfidence (and when it does
not),” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102(1), 76–94.
Moore, D. A., 2007, “Not so above average after all: When people believe they are worse
than average and its implications for theories of bias in social comparison,” Organiza-
tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102(1), 42–58.
Moore, D. A., and D. M. Cain, 2007, “Overconfidence and underconfidence: When and why
people underestimate (and overestimate) the competition,” Organizational Behavior
and Human Decision Processes, 103(2), 197–213.
Moore, D. A., and P. J. Healy, 2007, “The Trouble With Overconfidence,” Working pa-
per, Carnegie Mellon University - David A. Tepper School of Business and Ohio State
University - Department of Economics.
Moore, D. A., and D. A. Small, 2007, “Error and Bias in Comparative Judgment: On
Being Both Better and Worse Than We Think We Are,” Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 92(6), 972–989.
Nicolosi, G., L. Peng, and N. Zhu, 2007, “Do Individual Investors Learn from Their
Trading Experience?,” Working paper.
Odean, T., 1998, “Volume, Volatility, Price, and Profit When All Traders Are Above
Average,” Journal of Finance, 53(6), 1887–1934.
Owhoso, V., and A. Weickgenannt, 2007, “Auditors’ self-perceived abilities in conducting
domain audits,” Critical Perspectives on Accounting, forthconming.
19
Seru, A., T. Shumway, and N. Stoffman, 2007, “Learning By Trading,” Working paper,
University of Michigan.
Spanos, A., 1986, Statistical foundations of econometric modelling. Cambridge University
Press, Cambridge.
Statman, M., S. Thorley, and K. Vorkink, 2006, “Investor Overconfidence and Trading
Volume,” Review of Financial Studies, 19(4), 1531–1565.
Svenson, O., 1981, “Are we all less risky and more skillful than our fellow drivers?,” Acta
Psychologica, 47(2), 143–148.
Taylor, S. S., and J. D. Brown, 1988, “Illusion and well being: A social psychology per-
spective on mental health,” Psychological Bulletin, 103(2), 193–210.
Weber, M., and F. Welfens, 2007, “An Individual Level Analysis of the Disposition Effect:
Empirical and Experimental Evidence,” Working paper, University of Mannheim.
20
Table 1: Cross-Sectional Distribution of Percentage Monthly Gross Portfolio Returns
This table shows the cross-sectional distribution of the monthly gross returns of our investor sample (all
investors and the subgroup of respondents to the questionnaire). Gross monthly portfolio performance of
each investor was calculated making the following simplifying assumptions: We assume that all stocks are
bought and sold at the end of the month and we ignore intra-month trading. The gross portfolio return
Rht of investor h in month t is calculated as follows:
Rht =
Sht
i=1
wihtRit with wiht =
Pitniht
Sht
i=1
Pitniht
Rit is the return of stock i in month t, Sht is the number of type of stocks held by individual h in month
t, Pit is the price of stock i at the beginning of month t, and niht is the number of stocks of company i
held by investor h in month t. wiht is the beginning-of-month-t market value of the holding of stock i of
investor h divided by the beginning-of-month-t market value of the whole stock portfolio of investor h.
Time period is January 1997 to March 2001. Investors with 12 or less portfolio return observations are
excluded from the sample. The table also shows the arithmetic monthly return of the German blue chip
index DAX from January 1997 to March 2001 and the number of investors with more than 12 portfolio
return observations in our 51 month sample period. Parametric and non-parametric tests show that the
distribution of monthly returns is not significantly different in the two groups.
All Respondents to
investors the questionnaire
Mean 0.54% 0.30%
Minimum −16.02% −10.73%
1st percentile −5.83% −8.15%
5th percentile −2.99% −3.96%
10th percentile −1.90% −2.11%
25th percentile −0.49% −0.58%
Median 0.57% 0.53%
75th percentile 1.50% 1.40%
90th percentile 2.75% 2.52%
95th percentile 3.92% 3.42%
99th percentile 7.80% 6.06%
Maximum 23.81% 7.09%
DAX (arithmetic monthly return) 2.02% 2.02%
Number of households 2,793 195
(91% of 3,079) (91% of 215)
21
Table 2: Return Estimates
We asked the investors to give an estimate of their portfolio performance in the past (from January 1997
to December 2000):
Please try to estimate your past performance of your stock portfolio at your online broker.
Please estimate the return of your stock portfolio from January 1997 to December 2000:
[Answer] percent per year on average.
This table presents the answers to this question (mean, median, standard deviation, skewness, kurtosis,
minimum, maximum, and various percentiles).
Number of observations 210
Mean 14.93 %
Standard deviation 13.11 %
Skewness 2.01
Kurtosis 24.33
Minimum −50 %
1st percentile −15 %
5th percentile 0 %
10th percentile 5 %
25th percentile 10 %
Median 15 %
75th percentile 20 %
90th percentile 27 %
95th percentile 35 %
99th percentile 41 %
Maximum 120 %
22
Table 3: Return Estimates, Self-Assessments, and Experience
This table presents mean, median, the number of observations as well as the p-value of a Wilcoxon
test (null hypothesis: value is equal to 0) of the absolute return difference, the difference between the
perceived return and the actual return, the absolute performance percentile difference and the difference
between actual performance percentile and the perceived performance percentile for all investors (with
stock market investment experience variable available) and investors with low (less than 5 years) and
high (more than 5 years) stock market investment experience. 5 years is the median experience level
in our data set so that we obtain two groups of approximately equal size. See Sections 3 and 4 for
details. Furthermore, the table shows the number of cases and the percentage of cases in which the sign
of the past return assessment was correct. Moreover, the table shows the number of investors who are
reasonably close with their estimates (see the lines which show the number of investors who are less than
5 percentage points or 10 percentage points wrong). Variables are winsorized at the 10 percent level. *
indicates significance at 10%; ** indicates significance at 5%; *** indicates significance at 1%.
All investors Low investment High investment p-value
(with experience experience experience (Mann-Whitney)
variable available) (less than 5 years) (more than 5 years) (difference in
experience groups)
Absolute return Mean 20.96 23.68 18.73 0.098*
difference Median 17.84 21.01 16.71
Observations 142 64 78
Different from 0 p < 0.0001*** p < 0.0001*** p < 0.0001***
(Wilcoxon)
Correct sign 87 37 50
Wrong sign 55 27 28
Percent correct 61.27% 57.81% 64.10%
Less than 5 percentage 35 13 22
points wrong
Less than 10 percentage 48 18 30
points wrong
Perceived return Mean 11.61 13.18 10.32 0.42
-actual return Median 13.85 14.13 12.41
Observations 142 64 78
Different from 0 p < 0.0001*** p < 0.0001*** p < 0.0001***
(Wilcoxon)
Absolute percentile Mean 25.33 25.31 25.35 0.99
difference Median 26.00 23.00 27.00
Observations 140 62 78
Different from 0 p < 0.0001*** p < 0.0001*** p < 0.0001***
(Wilcoxon)
Actual percentile Mean 4.44 5.61 3.50 0.73
-perceived percentile Median 4.00 3.50 4.00
Observations 140 62 78
Different from 0 0.0485** 0.103 0.2328
(Wilcoxon)
23
Table 4: Self-Ratings
We asked the investors to answer the following question:
What percentage of customers of your discount brokerage house had higher returns than
you in the four-year period from January 1997 to December 2000? (Please give a number
between 0 % and 100 %)
[Answer] percent of other customers had higher returns than I did.
This table presents the answers to this question (mean, median, standard deviation, skewness, kurtosis,
minimum, maximum, and various percentiles).
Number of observations 212
Mean 46.99
Standard deviation 19.33
Skewness 0.04
Kurtosis 2.87
Minimum 2
1st percentile 5
5th percentile 15
10th percentile 20
25th percentile 30
Median 50
75th percentile 60
90th percentile 70
95th percentile 80
99th percentile 90
Maximum 95
24
Table5:DeterminantsoftheDifferenceBetweenReturnEstimatesandRealizedReturnsandBetweenPerceivedandActualReturn
Percentile:Cross-SectionalRegressions
Thistablepresentscross-sectionalregressionresultsonthedeterminantsoftheabsolutedifferencebetweenreturnestimatesandrealizedreturns(Regressions(1)and(2)),
thedifferencebetweenreturnestimatesandrealizedreturns(Regressions(3)and(4)),theabsolutedifferencebetweenperceivedandactualreturnpercentile(Regressions
(5)and(6)),andthedifferencebetweenactualandperceivedreturnpercentile(Regressions(7)and(8))asdependentvariablesandstockmarketinvestmentexperience,a
genderdummyvariable(thevariableissetequalto1iftheinvestorismale),age,amutualfundinvestordummy(thevariableissetequalto1iftheinvestortradesfunds
atleastonceinthetimeperiodfromJanuary1997untilApril2001),awarranttraderdummyvariable(thevariableissetequalto1iftheinvestortradeswarrantsat
leastonceinthetimeperiodfromJanuary1997untilApril2001),ahighriskdummy(thevariableissetequalto1iftheinvestorclassifiesherorhisinvestmentstrategy
ashighrisk),thelogarithmofmeanmonthlystockportfoliovalue,thetime-seriesaverageofthemonthlystockportfolioperformanceofaninvestor,thelogarithmof
thestandarddeviationofmonthlystockportfolioperformanceasameasureofportfoliorisk,andthelogarithmofnumberofstocksinportfolio.Investmentexperience
isreportedwithinfiveranges,wherethetoprangeismorethan15years.Intheregressionsweusethemidpointofeachrangeand17.5yearsforthetoprange.The
dependentvariablesandthemonthlystockportfolioperformancearewinsorizedatthe10percentlevel.Thetablereportsstandardizedbetacoefficients(exceptforthe
intercept).Robustp-valuesareinparentheses.*indicatessignificanceat10%;**indicatessignificanceat5%;***indicatessignificanceat1%.
AbsolutedifferenceDifferenceAbsolutedifferenceDifference
betweenbetweenbetweenbetween
returnestimateandreturnestimateandactualpercentileactualpercentile
realizedreturnrealizedreturnandperceivedpercentileandperceivedpercentile
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)
Stockmarketinvestmentexperience-0.202-0.186-0.088-0.057-0.040-0.036-0.088-0.056
(inyears)(0.010)***(0.029)**(0.051)*(0.519)(0.646)(0.691)(0.179)(0.543)
Gender-0.074-0.0560.0110.047-0.032-0.0280.0280.056
(Dummy;men=1)(0.523)(0.619)(0.557)(0.686)(0.722)(0.749)(0.225)(0.596)
Age-0.014-0.0600.038-0.051-0.096-0.1070.027-0.047
(0.854)(0.538)(0.490)(0.622)(0.403)(0.344)(0.737)(0.637)
Mutualfundinvestor-0.102-0.034-0.07580.058-0.051-0.033-0.0740.041
(Dummy)(0.197)(0.694)(0.076)*(0.539)(0.579)(0.712)(0.196)(0.676)
Warranttrader-0.063-0.0290.0310.0990.0460.057-0.0440.026
(Dummy)(0.434)(0.759)(0.417)(0.311)(0.647)(0.574)(0.447)(0.785)
Highriskinvestmentstrategy0.031-0.054-0.036-0.2010.0280.0050.0158-0.131
(basedonself-report;dummy)(0.717)(0.460)(0.427)(0.026)**(0.780)(0.960)(0.784)(0.189)
ln(stockportfoliovalue)0.0370.062-0.0250.0241-0.236-0.226-0.0180.043
(inEUR;time-seriesaverageperinvestor)(0.771)(0.645)(0.704)(0.879)(0.136)(0.150)(0.857)(0.800)
Stockportfolioperformance-0.467-0.919-0.126-0.813
(time-seriesaverageperinvestor)(0.000)***(0.000)***(0.232)(0.000)***
ln(portfoliorisk)0.2210.231-0.048-0.027-0.011-0.0080.0570.079
(standarddeviationofmonthlyportfolioreturns)(0.063)*(0.044)**(0.351)(0.839)(0.918)(0.943)(0.385)(0.505)
ln(numberofstocksinportfolio)-0.151-0.1750.012-0.0360.0720.0650.1460.100
(time-seriesaverageperinvestor)(0.231)(0.230)(0.882)(0.825)(0.628)(0.658)(0.150)(0.498)
Constant55.60652.04214.8005.35259.10157.77214.235-0.679
(0.000)***(0.001)***(0.039)**(0.820)(0.000)***(0.000)***(0.327)(0.983)
Observations121121121121119119119119
AdjustedR-squared0.3250.1100.8280.0000.0020.0000.6290.000
25
Figure 1: Return Estimates and Realized Returns
This figure plots return estimates versus realized portfolio returns of the individual investors who answered
the questionnaire. Furthermore, the figure shows a 45-degree line. Variables are winsorized at the 10
percent level.
-30
-20
-10
0
10
20
30
40
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40
Return estimates (% p.a.)
Realizedreturns(%p.a.)
26
Figure 2: Self-Ratings in Percentiles and Actual Percentiles
This figure plots the self-ratings in percentiles versus actual percentiles of the individual investors who
answered the questionnaire.
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Self-rating (past returns in percentiles)
Actualpercentile
27

Más contenido relacionado

La actualidad más candente

A research study on investors behaviour regarding choice of asset allocation ...
A research study on investors behaviour regarding choice of asset allocation ...A research study on investors behaviour regarding choice of asset allocation ...
A research study on investors behaviour regarding choice of asset allocation ...
SubmissionResearchpa
 

La actualidad más candente (9)

Purely Procedural Preferences - Beyond Procedural Equity and Reciprocity
Purely Procedural Preferences - Beyond Procedural Equity and ReciprocityPurely Procedural Preferences - Beyond Procedural Equity and Reciprocity
Purely Procedural Preferences - Beyond Procedural Equity and Reciprocity
 
A research study on investors behaviour regarding choice of asset allocation ...
A research study on investors behaviour regarding choice of asset allocation ...A research study on investors behaviour regarding choice of asset allocation ...
A research study on investors behaviour regarding choice of asset allocation ...
 
Investor psychology and asset pricing
Investor psychology and asset pricingInvestor psychology and asset pricing
Investor psychology and asset pricing
 
behavioral finance:theories, issues and challenges
behavioral finance:theories, issues and challenges behavioral finance:theories, issues and challenges
behavioral finance:theories, issues and challenges
 
Firm Level Investment Demand
Firm Level Investment Demand Firm Level Investment Demand
Firm Level Investment Demand
 
Stock Screen White Paper
Stock Screen White PaperStock Screen White Paper
Stock Screen White Paper
 
FACTORS AFFECTING INVESTMENT BEHAVIOUR AMONG YOUNG PROFESSIONALS
FACTORS AFFECTING INVESTMENT BEHAVIOUR AMONG YOUNG PROFESSIONALSFACTORS AFFECTING INVESTMENT BEHAVIOUR AMONG YOUNG PROFESSIONALS
FACTORS AFFECTING INVESTMENT BEHAVIOUR AMONG YOUNG PROFESSIONALS
 
Impact of corporate vc investment motivation on startup valuation
Impact of corporate vc investment motivation on startup valuationImpact of corporate vc investment motivation on startup valuation
Impact of corporate vc investment motivation on startup valuation
 
Factors affecting the investors decision making
Factors affecting the investors decision makingFactors affecting the investors decision making
Factors affecting the investors decision making
 

Destacado

Destacado (16)

English class activity 2
English class activity 2English class activity 2
English class activity 2
 
Horison singkawang
Horison singkawangHorison singkawang
Horison singkawang
 
99
9999
99
 
When Intentions Go Public
When Intentions Go PublicWhen Intentions Go Public
When Intentions Go Public
 
Operaciones 1°e
Operaciones 1°eOperaciones 1°e
Operaciones 1°e
 
911 management la importancia de contar con buenos gerentes y sus habilidades...
911 management la importancia de contar con buenos gerentes y sus habilidades...911 management la importancia de contar con buenos gerentes y sus habilidades...
911 management la importancia de contar con buenos gerentes y sus habilidades...
 
20151229 112
20151229 11220151229 112
20151229 112
 
Your future is participation and here’s why
Your future is participation and here’s whyYour future is participation and here’s why
Your future is participation and here’s why
 
Biased Shorts: Short sellers’ Disposition Effect and Limits to Arbitrage
Biased Shorts: Short sellers’ Disposition Effect and Limits to ArbitrageBiased Shorts: Short sellers’ Disposition Effect and Limits to Arbitrage
Biased Shorts: Short sellers’ Disposition Effect and Limits to Arbitrage
 
Using EEG when usability testing
Using EEG when usability testingUsing EEG when usability testing
Using EEG when usability testing
 
Introduction to Image Processing - Short Course - Part I
Introduction to Image Processing - Short Course - Part IIntroduction to Image Processing - Short Course - Part I
Introduction to Image Processing - Short Course - Part I
 
Bad hibernation-rooted
Bad hibernation-rootedBad hibernation-rooted
Bad hibernation-rooted
 
Case Study: Upgrade Strategies for PeopleSoft Financials and Supply Chain 9.1
Case Study: Upgrade Strategies for PeopleSoft Financials and Supply Chain 9.1Case Study: Upgrade Strategies for PeopleSoft Financials and Supply Chain 9.1
Case Study: Upgrade Strategies for PeopleSoft Financials and Supply Chain 9.1
 
FILUM Annelida
FILUM Annelida FILUM Annelida
FILUM Annelida
 
Shelley Kuipers: The rise of participation and why it’s better for business
Shelley Kuipers: The rise of participation and why it’s better for businessShelley Kuipers: The rise of participation and why it’s better for business
Shelley Kuipers: The rise of participation and why it’s better for business
 
La etica profesional
La etica profesionalLa etica profesional
La etica profesional
 

Similar a Why Inexperienced Investors Do Not Learn: They Do Not Know Their Past Portfolio Performance

Electronic copy available at httpssrn.comabstract=1629786.docx
Electronic copy available at httpssrn.comabstract=1629786.docxElectronic copy available at httpssrn.comabstract=1629786.docx
Electronic copy available at httpssrn.comabstract=1629786.docx
SALU18
 
Empirical Methods In Accounting And Finance.docx
Empirical Methods In Accounting And Finance.docxEmpirical Methods In Accounting And Finance.docx
Empirical Methods In Accounting And Finance.docx
4934bk
 
HONR 4000 Report - Alyssa Hofstetter
HONR 4000 Report - Alyssa HofstetterHONR 4000 Report - Alyssa Hofstetter
HONR 4000 Report - Alyssa Hofstetter
Alyssa Hofstetter
 
Fund flows and performance berk green
Fund flows and performance berk greenFund flows and performance berk green
Fund flows and performance berk green
bfmresearch
 
Can fundmanagerspickstocks baker
Can fundmanagerspickstocks bakerCan fundmanagerspickstocks baker
Can fundmanagerspickstocks baker
bfmresearch
 
A Tale of Two InvestorsEstimating Optimism and Overconfid.docx
A Tale of Two InvestorsEstimating Optimism and Overconfid.docxA Tale of Two InvestorsEstimating Optimism and Overconfid.docx
A Tale of Two InvestorsEstimating Optimism and Overconfid.docx
ransayo
 
The value investing-requiem rebirth or reincarnation
The value investing-requiem rebirth or reincarnationThe value investing-requiem rebirth or reincarnation
The value investing-requiem rebirth or reincarnation
Ying wei (Joe) Chou
 
Accounting Research Center, Booth School of Business, Universi.docx
Accounting Research Center, Booth School of Business, Universi.docxAccounting Research Center, Booth School of Business, Universi.docx
Accounting Research Center, Booth School of Business, Universi.docx
nettletondevon
 
Performance of trades and stocks of fund managers pinnuck
Performance of trades and stocks of fund managers pinnuckPerformance of trades and stocks of fund managers pinnuck
Performance of trades and stocks of fund managers pinnuck
bfmresearch
 
How corporate diversification affects excess value and excess profitability
How corporate diversification affects excess value and excess profitabilityHow corporate diversification affects excess value and excess profitability
How corporate diversification affects excess value and excess profitability
Alexander Decker
 

Similar a Why Inexperienced Investors Do Not Learn: They Do Not Know Their Past Portfolio Performance (20)

Electronic copy available at httpssrn.comabstract=1629786.docx
Electronic copy available at httpssrn.comabstract=1629786.docxElectronic copy available at httpssrn.comabstract=1629786.docx
Electronic copy available at httpssrn.comabstract=1629786.docx
 
Empirical Methods In Accounting And Finance.docx
Empirical Methods In Accounting And Finance.docxEmpirical Methods In Accounting And Finance.docx
Empirical Methods In Accounting And Finance.docx
 
HONR 4000 Report - Alyssa Hofstetter
HONR 4000 Report - Alyssa HofstetterHONR 4000 Report - Alyssa Hofstetter
HONR 4000 Report - Alyssa Hofstetter
 
The effects of psychology on individual investors behaviors
The effects of psychology on individual investors behaviorsThe effects of psychology on individual investors behaviors
The effects of psychology on individual investors behaviors
 
Artical
ArticalArtical
Artical
 
Fund flows and performance berk green
Fund flows and performance berk greenFund flows and performance berk green
Fund flows and performance berk green
 
Can fundmanagerspickstocks baker
Can fundmanagerspickstocks bakerCan fundmanagerspickstocks baker
Can fundmanagerspickstocks baker
 
Information Asymmetries & Portfolio Concentration
Information Asymmetries & Portfolio ConcentrationInformation Asymmetries & Portfolio Concentration
Information Asymmetries & Portfolio Concentration
 
kilu assy.docx
kilu assy.docxkilu assy.docx
kilu assy.docx
 
A FINANCIAL STATEMENT ANALYSIS OF COMPANIES WITH DIFFERENT OWNERSHIP CONCENTR...
A FINANCIAL STATEMENT ANALYSIS OF COMPANIES WITH DIFFERENT OWNERSHIP CONCENTR...A FINANCIAL STATEMENT ANALYSIS OF COMPANIES WITH DIFFERENT OWNERSHIP CONCENTR...
A FINANCIAL STATEMENT ANALYSIS OF COMPANIES WITH DIFFERENT OWNERSHIP CONCENTR...
 
A Tale of Two InvestorsEstimating Optimism and Overconfid.docx
A Tale of Two InvestorsEstimating Optimism and Overconfid.docxA Tale of Two InvestorsEstimating Optimism and Overconfid.docx
A Tale of Two InvestorsEstimating Optimism and Overconfid.docx
 
The value investing-requiem rebirth or reincarnation
The value investing-requiem rebirth or reincarnationThe value investing-requiem rebirth or reincarnation
The value investing-requiem rebirth or reincarnation
 
DIss_paper
DIss_paperDIss_paper
DIss_paper
 
Accounting Research Center, Booth School of Business, Universi.docx
Accounting Research Center, Booth School of Business, Universi.docxAccounting Research Center, Booth School of Business, Universi.docx
Accounting Research Center, Booth School of Business, Universi.docx
 
IJSRED-V2I3P71
IJSRED-V2I3P71IJSRED-V2I3P71
IJSRED-V2I3P71
 
Performance of trades and stocks of fund managers pinnuck
Performance of trades and stocks of fund managers pinnuckPerformance of trades and stocks of fund managers pinnuck
Performance of trades and stocks of fund managers pinnuck
 
Mega Essay
Mega EssayMega Essay
Mega Essay
 
Abstract
AbstractAbstract
Abstract
 
Impact of marketing on fianancial instrument
Impact of marketing on fianancial instrumentImpact of marketing on fianancial instrument
Impact of marketing on fianancial instrument
 
How corporate diversification affects excess value and excess profitability
How corporate diversification affects excess value and excess profitabilityHow corporate diversification affects excess value and excess profitability
How corporate diversification affects excess value and excess profitability
 

Más de Trading Game Pty Ltd

Age-Related Physiological Changes and Their Clinical Significance
Age-Related Physiological Changes and Their Clinical SignificanceAge-Related Physiological Changes and Their Clinical Significance
Age-Related Physiological Changes and Their Clinical Significance
Trading Game Pty Ltd
 

Más de Trading Game Pty Ltd (20)

2019 In Review
 2019 In Review 2019 In Review
2019 In Review
 
Bullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their Lives
Bullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their LivesBullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their Lives
Bullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their Lives
 
Analyzing the Bitcoin Ponzi Scheme Ecosystem
Analyzing the Bitcoin Ponzi Scheme EcosystemAnalyzing the Bitcoin Ponzi Scheme Ecosystem
Analyzing the Bitcoin Ponzi Scheme Ecosystem
 
Age-Related Physiological Changes and Their Clinical Significance
Age-Related Physiological Changes and Their Clinical SignificanceAge-Related Physiological Changes and Their Clinical Significance
Age-Related Physiological Changes and Their Clinical Significance
 
A Brief Introduction to the Basics of Game Theory
A Brief Introduction to the Basics of Game TheoryA Brief Introduction to the Basics of Game Theory
A Brief Introduction to the Basics of Game Theory
 
Primer on Managed Futures
Primer on Managed FuturesPrimer on Managed Futures
Primer on Managed Futures
 
S&P500 Panic Attacks
S&P500 Panic AttacksS&P500 Panic Attacks
S&P500 Panic Attacks
 
How Biases Affect Investor Behaviour
How Biases Affect Investor BehaviourHow Biases Affect Investor Behaviour
How Biases Affect Investor Behaviour
 
Avoid News
Avoid NewsAvoid News
Avoid News
 
The Psychology and Neuroscience of Financial Decision Making
The Psychology and Neuroscience of Financial Decision MakingThe Psychology and Neuroscience of Financial Decision Making
The Psychology and Neuroscience of Financial Decision Making
 
ARE CEOS PAID FOR PERFORMANCE? Evaluating the Effectiveness of Equity Incentives
ARE CEOS PAID FOR PERFORMANCE? Evaluating the Effectiveness of Equity IncentivesARE CEOS PAID FOR PERFORMANCE? Evaluating the Effectiveness of Equity Incentives
ARE CEOS PAID FOR PERFORMANCE? Evaluating the Effectiveness of Equity Incentives
 
People
PeoplePeople
People
 
Female Fund Managers 2016
Female Fund Managers 2016Female Fund Managers 2016
Female Fund Managers 2016
 
The science of cycology: Failures to understand how everyday objects work
The science of cycology: Failures to understand how everyday objects workThe science of cycology: Failures to understand how everyday objects work
The science of cycology: Failures to understand how everyday objects work
 
SPIVA Australian Scorecard
SPIVA Australian ScorecardSPIVA Australian Scorecard
SPIVA Australian Scorecard
 
The Rise of Behavioural Economics
The Rise of Behavioural EconomicsThe Rise of Behavioural Economics
The Rise of Behavioural Economics
 
Golf buddies aer_may_2016 2
Golf buddies aer_may_2016 2Golf buddies aer_may_2016 2
Golf buddies aer_may_2016 2
 
What’s Going On in Your Brain?”
What’s Going On in Your Brain?” What’s Going On in Your Brain?”
What’s Going On in Your Brain?”
 
How creativity-works-in-the-brain-report
How creativity-works-in-the-brain-reportHow creativity-works-in-the-brain-report
How creativity-works-in-the-brain-report
 
JP Morgan - Guide to the Markets
JP Morgan - Guide to the MarketsJP Morgan - Guide to the Markets
JP Morgan - Guide to the Markets
 

Último

Call Girls in Yamuna Vihar (delhi) call me [🔝9953056974🔝] escort service 24X7
Call Girls in  Yamuna Vihar  (delhi) call me [🔝9953056974🔝] escort service 24X7Call Girls in  Yamuna Vihar  (delhi) call me [🔝9953056974🔝] escort service 24X7
Call Girls in Yamuna Vihar (delhi) call me [🔝9953056974🔝] escort service 24X7
9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
MASTERING FOREX: STRATEGIES FOR SUCCESS.pdf
MASTERING FOREX: STRATEGIES FOR SUCCESS.pdfMASTERING FOREX: STRATEGIES FOR SUCCESS.pdf
MASTERING FOREX: STRATEGIES FOR SUCCESS.pdf
Cocity Enterprises
 

Último (20)

Q1 2024 Conference Call Presentation vF.pdf
Q1 2024 Conference Call Presentation vF.pdfQ1 2024 Conference Call Presentation vF.pdf
Q1 2024 Conference Call Presentation vF.pdf
 
Famous No1 Amil Baba Love marriage Astrologer Specialist Expert In Pakistan a...
Famous No1 Amil Baba Love marriage Astrologer Specialist Expert In Pakistan a...Famous No1 Amil Baba Love marriage Astrologer Specialist Expert In Pakistan a...
Famous No1 Amil Baba Love marriage Astrologer Specialist Expert In Pakistan a...
 
falcon-invoice-discounting-unlocking-prime-investment-opportunities
falcon-invoice-discounting-unlocking-prime-investment-opportunitiesfalcon-invoice-discounting-unlocking-prime-investment-opportunities
falcon-invoice-discounting-unlocking-prime-investment-opportunities
 
Bhubaneswar🌹Ravi Tailkes ❤CALL GIRLS 9777949614 💟 CALL GIRLS IN bhubaneswar ...
Bhubaneswar🌹Ravi Tailkes  ❤CALL GIRLS 9777949614 💟 CALL GIRLS IN bhubaneswar ...Bhubaneswar🌹Ravi Tailkes  ❤CALL GIRLS 9777949614 💟 CALL GIRLS IN bhubaneswar ...
Bhubaneswar🌹Ravi Tailkes ❤CALL GIRLS 9777949614 💟 CALL GIRLS IN bhubaneswar ...
 
Turbhe Fantastic Escorts📞📞9833754194 Kopar Khairane Marathi Call Girls-Kopar ...
Turbhe Fantastic Escorts📞📞9833754194 Kopar Khairane Marathi Call Girls-Kopar ...Turbhe Fantastic Escorts📞📞9833754194 Kopar Khairane Marathi Call Girls-Kopar ...
Turbhe Fantastic Escorts📞📞9833754194 Kopar Khairane Marathi Call Girls-Kopar ...
 
Significant AI Trends for the Financial Industry in 2024 and How to Utilize Them
Significant AI Trends for the Financial Industry in 2024 and How to Utilize ThemSignificant AI Trends for the Financial Industry in 2024 and How to Utilize Them
Significant AI Trends for the Financial Industry in 2024 and How to Utilize Them
 
Premium Call Girls Bangalore Call Girls Service Just Call 🍑👄6378878445 🍑👄 Top...
Premium Call Girls Bangalore Call Girls Service Just Call 🍑👄6378878445 🍑👄 Top...Premium Call Girls Bangalore Call Girls Service Just Call 🍑👄6378878445 🍑👄 Top...
Premium Call Girls Bangalore Call Girls Service Just Call 🍑👄6378878445 🍑👄 Top...
 
Female Russian Escorts Mumbai Call Girls-((ANdheri))9833754194-Jogeshawri Fre...
Female Russian Escorts Mumbai Call Girls-((ANdheri))9833754194-Jogeshawri Fre...Female Russian Escorts Mumbai Call Girls-((ANdheri))9833754194-Jogeshawri Fre...
Female Russian Escorts Mumbai Call Girls-((ANdheri))9833754194-Jogeshawri Fre...
 
Call Girls in Yamuna Vihar (delhi) call me [🔝9953056974🔝] escort service 24X7
Call Girls in  Yamuna Vihar  (delhi) call me [🔝9953056974🔝] escort service 24X7Call Girls in  Yamuna Vihar  (delhi) call me [🔝9953056974🔝] escort service 24X7
Call Girls in Yamuna Vihar (delhi) call me [🔝9953056974🔝] escort service 24X7
 
Collecting banker, Capacity of collecting Banker, conditions under section 13...
Collecting banker, Capacity of collecting Banker, conditions under section 13...Collecting banker, Capacity of collecting Banker, conditions under section 13...
Collecting banker, Capacity of collecting Banker, conditions under section 13...
 
Fixed exchange rate and flexible exchange rate.pptx
Fixed exchange rate and flexible exchange rate.pptxFixed exchange rate and flexible exchange rate.pptx
Fixed exchange rate and flexible exchange rate.pptx
 
Call Girls Howrah ( 8250092165 ) Cheap rates call girls | Get low budget
Call Girls Howrah ( 8250092165 ) Cheap rates call girls | Get low budgetCall Girls Howrah ( 8250092165 ) Cheap rates call girls | Get low budget
Call Girls Howrah ( 8250092165 ) Cheap rates call girls | Get low budget
 
Dubai Call Girls Deira O525547819 Dubai Call Girls Bur Dubai Multiple
Dubai Call Girls Deira O525547819 Dubai Call Girls Bur Dubai MultipleDubai Call Girls Deira O525547819 Dubai Call Girls Bur Dubai Multiple
Dubai Call Girls Deira O525547819 Dubai Call Girls Bur Dubai Multiple
 
MASTERING FOREX: STRATEGIES FOR SUCCESS.pdf
MASTERING FOREX: STRATEGIES FOR SUCCESS.pdfMASTERING FOREX: STRATEGIES FOR SUCCESS.pdf
MASTERING FOREX: STRATEGIES FOR SUCCESS.pdf
 
Business Principles, Tools, and Techniques in Participating in Various Types...
Business Principles, Tools, and Techniques  in Participating in Various Types...Business Principles, Tools, and Techniques  in Participating in Various Types...
Business Principles, Tools, and Techniques in Participating in Various Types...
 
Certified Kala Jadu, Black magic specialist in Rawalpindi and Bangali Amil ba...
Certified Kala Jadu, Black magic specialist in Rawalpindi and Bangali Amil ba...Certified Kala Jadu, Black magic specialist in Rawalpindi and Bangali Amil ba...
Certified Kala Jadu, Black magic specialist in Rawalpindi and Bangali Amil ba...
 
Lion One Corporate Presentation May 2024
Lion One Corporate Presentation May 2024Lion One Corporate Presentation May 2024
Lion One Corporate Presentation May 2024
 
20240419-SMC-submission-Annual-Superannuation-Performance-Test-–-design-optio...
20240419-SMC-submission-Annual-Superannuation-Performance-Test-–-design-optio...20240419-SMC-submission-Annual-Superannuation-Performance-Test-–-design-optio...
20240419-SMC-submission-Annual-Superannuation-Performance-Test-–-design-optio...
 
CBD Belapur((Thane)) Charming Call Girls📞❤9833754194 Kamothe Beautiful Call G...
CBD Belapur((Thane)) Charming Call Girls📞❤9833754194 Kamothe Beautiful Call G...CBD Belapur((Thane)) Charming Call Girls📞❤9833754194 Kamothe Beautiful Call G...
CBD Belapur((Thane)) Charming Call Girls📞❤9833754194 Kamothe Beautiful Call G...
 
cost-volume-profit analysis.ppt(managerial accounting).pptx
cost-volume-profit analysis.ppt(managerial accounting).pptxcost-volume-profit analysis.ppt(managerial accounting).pptx
cost-volume-profit analysis.ppt(managerial accounting).pptx
 

Why Inexperienced Investors Do Not Learn: They Do Not Know Their Past Portfolio Performance

  • 1. Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1002092 Why Inexperienced Investors Do Not Learn: They Do Not Know Their Past Portfolio Performance Markus Glaser and Martin Weber∗ Final Version, Finance Research Letters 4(4), 203-216 Winner of the “Ross Best Paper Award” for the best paper published in “Finance Research Letters” in 2007 Abstract Recently, researchers have gone a step further from just documenting biases of individual investors. More and more studies analyze how experience affects decisions and whether biases are eliminated by trading experience and learning. A necessary condition to learn is that investors actually know what happened in the past and that the views of the past are not biased. We contribute to the above mentioned literature by showing why learning and experience go hand in hand. Inexperienced investors are not able to give a reasonable self-assessment of their own past realized stock portfolio performance which impedes investors’ learning ability. Based on the answers of 215 online broker investors to an internet questionnaire, we analyze whether investors are able to correctly estimate their own realized stock portfolio performance. We show that investors are hardly able to give a correct estimate of their own past realized stock portfolio performance and that experienced investors are better able to do so. In general, we can conclude that we find evidence that investor experience lessens the simple mathematical error of estimating portfolio returns, but seems not to influence their “behavioral” mistakes pertaining to how good (in absolute sense or relative to other investors) they are. Keywords: Return Estimation, Portfolio Return, Perceived Returns, Self-Assessment, Better Than Aver- age Effect, Overconfidence, Financial Education, Financial Literacy, Learning, Experience JEL Classification Code: D8, G1 ∗Markus Glaser is from the Lehrstuhl f¨ur Bankbetriebslehre at the Business School, Universit¨at Mannheim, L 5, 2, 68131 Mannheim. E-Mail: glaser@bank.BWL.uni-mannheim.de. Martin Weber is from the Lehrstuhl f¨ur Bankbetriebslehre at the Business School, Universit¨at Mannheim, L 5, 2, 68131 Mannheim and CEPR, London. E-Mail: weber@bank.BWL.uni- mannheim.de. We would like to thank an anonymous referee for helpful comments. This paper was in parts written while Markus Glaser was visiting the Swedish Institute for Financial Research (SIFR) in Stockholm whose support is gratefully acknowledged. Financial Support from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) is also gratefully acknowledged. 1
  • 2. Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1002092 1 Introduction Recently, researchers have gone a step further from just documenting biases of individual investors. More and more studies analyze how experience affects decisions and whether biases are eliminated by trading experience and learning. Consider, for example, one of the most extensively studied biases of individual investors, the disposition effect. Feng and Seasholes (2005) analyze the disposition effect, the investor’s reluctance to realize losses and his propensity to realize gains, and find that experience eliminates the reluctance to realize losses. Seru, Shumway, and Stoffman (2007) analyze 22 million trades from more than one million individuals in Finland from 1995 to 2003 and also find that the disposition effect falls, and performance improves, as investors become more experienced. Dhar and Zhu (2006) use demographic and socioeconomic variables as proxies for investor literacy, and find empirical evidence that wealthier individuals exhibit a lower disposition effect. Weber and Welfens (2007) present empirical and experimental evidence that learning attenuates the magnitude of the disposition effect. Consistent with the studies above, trading frequency also tends to reduce the disposition effect. Kaustia and Kn¨upfer (2007) document a strong link between personal experience with IPOs and future subscriptions. Greenwood and Nagel (2007) find that around the peak of the stock market bubble in the year 2000, mutual funds run by inexperienced managers were more heavily invested in technology stocks. Nicolosi, Peng, and Zhu (2007) also present evidence that individual investors learn from past trading experience. A necessary condition to learn is that investors actually know what happened in the past and that the views of the past are not biased. We contribute to the above mentioned lit- erature by showing why learning and experience go hand in hand. Inexperienced investors 2
  • 3. are not able to give a reasonable self-assessment of their own past realized stock portfolio performance which impedes investors’ learning ability. Based on the answers of 215 online broker investors to an internet questionnaire we analyze whether investors are able to correctly estimate their own realized stock portfolio performance. Portfolio returns are calculated with the help of the stock portfolio positions of this investor group over the years preceding the questionnaire. Furthermore, we compare their perceived performance percentile with the actual performance percentile. Moreover, we analyze determinants of the cross-sectional heterogeneity in a regression analysis. The first focus of our paper is on the absolute difference between estimated and realized performance. A potential difference is presumably mainly driven by lack of knowledge or mathematical skills. No behavioral factors (should) come into play in this mathematical exercise. Thus, it is intuitive that expertise might play a role in explaining potential heterogeneity across investors. Furthermore, we analyze whether investors overestimate their past realized performance and their performance relative to other investors. We thus also contribute to the literature on overconfidence. The facets of overconfidence usually studied in the literature are (see Glaser, N¨oth, and Weber (2004), Glaser and Weber (2007), or Moore and Healy (2007)): 1. overestimation of one’s actual performance, 2. overplacement of one’s performance relative to others, also called the better than average effect, and 3. excessive precision in one’s belief, also called miscalibration. Empirical and experimental studies show that these facets of overconfidence are hardly 3
  • 4. correlated (see Glaser and Weber (2007) and Moore and Healy (2007) and the references cited therein). The first two facets of overconfidence can be regarded as a psychological foundation of differences of opinion models in finance (see Hong and Stein (2007)) while the last facet resembles the way overconfidence is modeled in finance with investors inferring a higher signal-to-noise ratio in market news than is statistically appropriate with the consequence of too tight prediction intervals (see, for example, the models by Odean (1998) and Gervais and Odean (2001), and the survey by Glaser, N¨oth, and Weber (2004)). By analyzing the determinants of the first two out of the three above mentioned facets of overconfidence, we contribute to the still scarce literature on the demographics of overconfidence in the spirit of Bhandari and Deaves (2006). Our main results can be summarized as follows. Investors are hardly able to give a cor- rect estimate of their own past realized stock portfolio performance over the past four years. The correlation coefficient between return estimates and realized returns is not dis- tinguishable from zero. Furthermore, people overrate themselves. On average, investors think, that they are better than others. The correlation between self ratings and actual performance is also not distinguishable from zero. High past realized stock portfolio per- formance does not make investors overconfident in the sense that they rate themselves as better than other investors. In other words, investors who think that they had above average performance actually did not have above average performance in the past. In- vestors with higher stock market investment experience and higher past portfolio returns are better able to estimate their past realized stock portfolio performance. The rest of the paper is organized as follows. In Section 2, we present the data sets analyzed and the design of the study. In Section 3, we analyze the correlation between return estimates and actual past realized portfolio returns. Results on the correlation 4
  • 5. between perceived performance percentile and actual performance percentile are presented in Section 4. Section 5 contains regression results on the determinants of cross-sectional heterogeneity in the answers provided. The last section summarizes and discusses the results and concludes. 2 Data Sets and the Design of the Study This study is based on the combination of several data sets. The main data set consists of 563,104 buy and sell transactions of 3,079 individual investors from a German online broker in the period from January 1997 to mid April 2001. We considered all investors who trade via the internet, had opened their account prior to January 1997, had at least one transaction in 1997, and have an e-mail-address. The second data set consists of sev- eral demographic and other self-reported information (age, gender, investment strategy, investment experience), that was collected by the online broker at the time each investor opened her or his account. The third data set consists of the answers to an online ques- tionnaire (see Glaser and Weber (2005) and the next section for details). Data on the securities traded are obtained from Datastream, our fourth data source. In August and September 2001, our investor sample received an email from the online broker with a link to an online questionnaire. 215 investors answered the questionnaire.1 Glaser and Weber (2007) show that there is no indication of a sample selection bias. The results of this paper are based on parts of this questionnaire and will be discussed in the following sections. We calculate the monthly gross portfolio performance of each investor making the follow- ing simplifying assumptions: We assume that all stocks are bought and sold at the end of 1See Glaser and Weber (2005) for details about this questionnaire. 5
  • 6. the month and we ignore intra-month trading. Barber and Odean (2000) show that these simplifying assumptions do not bias the measurement of portfolio performance. The gross portfolio return Rht of investor h in month t is calculated as follows: Rht = Sht i=1 wihtRit with wiht = Pitniht Sht i=1 Pitniht (1) Rit is the return of stock i in month t, Sht is the number of type of stocks held by individual h in month t, Pit is the price of stock i at the beginning of month t, and niht is the number of stocks of company i held by investor h in month t. wiht is the beginning-of-month-t market value of the holding of stock i of investor h divided by the beginning-of-month-t market value of the whole stock portfolio of investor h. Table 1 shows the results for all investors and the subgroup of respondents to the ques- tionnaire. The cross-sectional distribution of the monthly gross returns is similar to the results in Barber and Odean (2000), Table IV, p. 791. We observe a large cross-sectional variation in the performance across investors. When we exclude investors with stock po- sitions in 12 or fewer months, we find gross returns between −16% and +24% per month. On average, investors underperform relevant benchmarks. For example, the arithmetic average monthly return of the German blue chip index DAX from January 1997 to March 2001 is 2.02% whereas the mean gross monthly return of investors in our data set is 0.54%. Furthermore, parametric and non-parametric tests show that the distribution of monthly returns is not significantly different in the two groups. Thus, there is no indication of a sample selection bias.2 2Glaser and Weber (2007) show that this is also true for all other variables used later in the present paper. 6
  • 7. 3 Do Investors Know Their Past Portfolio Returns? In this section, we present survey evidence on investors’ ability to give an estimate of their own past realized stock portfolio performance. We asked the investors to give an estimate of their portfolio performance in the past (from January 1997 to December 2000): Please try to estimate your past performance of your stock portfolio at your online broker. Please estimate the return of your stock portfolio from January 1997 to December 2000: [Answer] percent per year on average. Table 2 presents the results. 210 of 215 investors who answered at least one question answered the question presented above. The investors think, on average, that their own realized stock portfolio performance from January 1997 to December 2000 was about 15 % per year. There is a large variation in the answers to this questions. The answers range from −50% to +120%. Figure 1 plots the realized portfolio returns versus return estimates of the individual investors who answered the questionnaire (variables are winsorized at the 10 percent level). The correlation coefficient between return estimates and realized returns is −0.0471 (p = 0.5203). This complete lack of correlation might seem extremely surprising. But another study that uses a design similar to ours documents exactly the same findings. Owhoso and Weickgenannt (2007) investigate the extent to which auditors’ ratings of self-perceived abilities correspond with their actual performance, and whether these perceptions are influenced by audit experience and effectiveness when conducting audits within their domain of specialization. 144 industry-specialized audit seniors and managers reviewed 7
  • 8. two sets of audit working paper cases. At the end of the review, the auditors rated their ability to perform an audit in their domain. One result is that there is no significant positive correlation between auditors’ self-perceived abilities and actual performance. The difference between return estimates and realized returns is positive (mean and median are higher than 10 percentage points per year, see also Table 3). The difference is highly significantly positive (p < 0.0001). This finding is consistent with Figure 1 which shows that most dots lie below the 45-degree line. Thus, many investors believe that they made money although they did not. This finding is consistent with psychological evidence that people overstate past performance in a variety of tasks (see Dunning, Heath, and Suls (2004), Moore and Healy (2007), and Owhoso and Weickgenannt (2007)). Why is there no correlation between realized portfolio returns and return estimates? One interpretation is that investors do not have a good understanding of the concept “return”. Another explanation is the way the online broker presents returns. Usually, the online broker presents gains and losses (with the buying price as the reference point) for every stock in the portfolio separately which makes it difficult to estimate the monthly or yearly stock portfolio performance. The broker also presents the total value of the portfolio, day by day. However, still, it is hard to calculate the performance of the portfolio when investors are continuously buying and selling stocks. When stocks are bought every month with additional money from, say, a cash account the stock portfolio value can increase although the average stock had negative returns. The information that should be relevant to judge own stock selection ability, the own past realized stock portfolio performance, is not calculated by the online broker. 8
  • 9. The results in this subsection are related to the experimental literature which shows that individuals in general are poor at recalling price changes when compared to recalling prices. Andreassen (1988) finds in an experiment that errors recalling price changes were significantly larger than those made in recalling prices. He argues that subjects pay greater attention to prices than to price changes. This result is in line with Glaser, Langer, Reynders, and Weber (2007) who show that a group of students has bigger problems stating return forecasts for financial time series when compared to price forecasts. Table 3 also shows that experienced investors are better able to estimate their past realized stock portfolio performance. The difference between the perceived return and the actual return is significantly lower for investors with more than 5 years of investment experience.3 Furthermore, the percentage of investors who estimate at least the right sign of their past realized portfolio performance is higher for experienced investors. Moreover, more experienced investors are reasonably close with their estimates (see the lines in the table which show the number of investors who are less than 5 percentage points or 10 percentage points wrong). These findings might explain why the studies mentioned in the Introduction find that experienced investors make better decisions. These results are supported by Amromin and Sharpe (2006). They examine answers to the following question: “Thinking about a diversified portfolio of stocks, what would you guess was the average annual return earned over the past 10 years?” from the Michigan Survey of Consumer Attitudes, conducted by the Survey Research Center (SRC) at the University of Michigan. In particular, they calculate the absolute value of the recall error, i.e. the difference between recalled and actual 10-year market returns, and regress this difference 3We use a cutoff of 5 years as this is the median level of experience so that we obtain two groups of approximately equal size. 9
  • 10. on demographic and stock ownership characteristics. They find that the accuracy of a respondent’s recall of past returns improves with both wealth and education, as well as other indicators of financial market knowledge. To summarize, the main result of this section is that investors are hardly able to give a cor- rect estimate of their own past realized stock portfolio performance and that experienced investors are better able to do so. 4 Self-Rating and Actual Performance Furthermore, we asked the following question to analyze investors’ self-ratings and their relation with actual performance. What percentage of customers of your discount brokerage house had higher returns than you in the four-year period from January 1997 to December 2000? (Please give a number between 0 % and 100 %) [Answer] percent of other customers had higher returns than I did. Table 4 presents the results. The mean is 46.99 indicating a slight better than average effect. This number is significantly different from 50 (p = 0.0335, Wilcoxon signed-rank test).4 We are thus able to confirm prior literature on the better than average effect (Taylor and Brown (1988), Svenson (1981)). One reason for the finding that this number is so close to 50 might be that about about 30 % of all investors classify themselves as 4Note that in Table 3 the results are slightly different as we show results for the subgroup of respondents for which we have data on investment experience in that table. 10
  • 11. average, i.e. state 50 as an answer.5 Figure 2 plots the self-ratings in percentiles versus actual return percentiles of the individ- ual investors who answered the questionnaire. Such a graph is often used in the literature (see for example Ackerman, Beier, and Bowen (2002)). The figure shows that there is no relation between the self-ratings in percentiles and actual return percentiles. The corre- lation between the self-ratings and actual percentiles is −0.0110 (p = 0.8810) which is not significantly distinguishable from zero. We are thus able to confirm prior research which shows that a correlation between self-ratings in percentiles and objective measures in percentiles is not existent (see Larrick, Burson, and Soll (2007) for further references and Dunning, Heath, and Suls (2004) for a recent survey). Furthermore, the difference between the actual return percentile of the respective investor and the self-assessed percentile is positive on average (this difference is positive if an investor thinks, for example, that only 25% of the other investors had higher portfolio returns in the past even though 30 % of the investors in the sample actually had higher returns). Thus, investors overestimate their relative position in terms of return percentiles. Moreover, high returns in the past do not lead to high overconfidence as measured by perceived percentile in our questionnaire at the end of the sample period. Thus, we do not find support for the learning-to-be-overconfident hypothesis (Gervais and Odean (2001)), i.e. a high degree of overconfidence as a result of past investment success. We argue, that one reason is, that investors do not know their past realized stock portfolio performance 5Furthermore, recent studies show that the better than average effect is not as universal as was previously documented in the literature (see Moore (2007), Moore and Cain (2007), and Moore and Small (2007)). Thus, our small better than average effect is not a puzzle. 11
  • 12. as was presented in the previous section.6 Note, however, that Gervais and Odean (2001) model the third of the manifestations of overconfidence mentioned in the Introduction while we are analyzing the first two facets in this paper. However, there is also a further interpretation of these findings. We find that the correla- tion between investors’ self-assessed absolute performance and their self-assessed relative performance is 0.2704 (with a p-value of 0.0002). Therefore, investors are somehow con- sistent in their answers. This raises the question “which” returns are actually relevant for overconfidence. It is possible that not the actual realized returns are relevant for the learning-to-be-overconfident hypothesis but the perceived realized returns. It is possible that investors “feel overconfident” even without knowing the true performance, by simply allowing their overblown beliefs of own realized portfolio returns to influence their view of returns relative to others. Thus, it is intriguing that actual returns are uncorrelated with the self-perceived ranking, but perceived returns are. To summarize, investors who believe they have done well in the absolute sense, also believe they have done better than others. Which returns are actually more relevant for overconfidence is a question for future research. 6These results do not contradict the studies of Statman, Thorley, and Vorkink (2006) or Glaser and Weber (2008). These studies find that returns over the past 6 months positively influence trading activity which is consistent with the learning-to-be-overconfident hypothesis. Statman, Thorley, and Vorkink (2006) find, however, that returns with lags larger than 6 do not influence trading volume anymore. In connection with the findings presented in this study, we can conclude that learning-to-be-overconfident stories are more appropriate for the effects of past returns over shorter horizons than the four year horizon which we analyze in the present study. 12
  • 13. 5 Which Investors are Able to Correctly Estimate Their Past Realized Portfolio Performance? Table 5 presents cross-sectional regression results on the determinants of the absolute difference between return estimates and realized returns (Regressions (1) and (2)), the difference between return estimates and realized returns (Regressions (3) and (4)), the absolute difference between perceived and actual return percentile (Regressions (5) and (6)), and the difference between actual and perceived return percentile (Regressions (7) and (8)) as dependent variables and stock market investment experience, a gender dummy variable, age, a mutual fund investor dummy, a warrant trader dummy variable, a high risk dummy, the logarithm of mean monthly stock portfolio value, the time-series average of the monthly stock portfolio performance of an investor, the logarithm of the standard deviation of monthly stock portfolio performance as a measure of portfolio risk, and the logarithm of number of stocks in portfolio.7 Stock market investment experience, gender and the high risk dummy are based on a voluntary self-report made by investors at the time the respective account was opened. This information was not updated afterwards by the online broker. The dependent variables and the monthly stock portfolio performance are winsorized at the 10 percent level. The table reports standardized beta coefficients (except for the intercept). Robust p-values are in parentheses. Regression (1) shows that stock market investment experience has a significantly negative effect on the absolute difference between return estimates and realized returns at the 1 percent level. This finding can be interpreted in the way that investors learn how to bet- 7We use the natural logarithm of variables that are positively skewed. Tests show, that we thus avoid problems like non-normality, non-linearity, and heteroscedasticity in the cross-sectional regression analysis. See Spanos (1986), chapter 21, especially, pp. 455-456, Davidson and MacKinnon (1993), chapter 14, and Atkinson (1985), pp. 80-81. 13
  • 14. ter judge their own past realized stock portfolio performance over time. Stock portfolio performance is also negatively related to the absolute difference between return estimates and realized returns. In other words, the lower the returns the worse investors are when judging their realized returns. There are several interpretations of this result. On the one hand, investors may look at their portfolio less often when returns are negative and, as a consequence, they do not know how bad they have actually performed. On the other hand, it is possible, that investors do not want to admit that they have performed pretty badly. This is consistent with psychological studies showing that people often neglect bad outcomes or unfavorable experience (see Dunning, Heath, and Suls (2004) for a survey). Karlsson, Loewenstein, and Seppi (2005), for example, present related evidence that in- vestors check the value of their portfolios more frequently in rising markets but “put their heads in the sand” when markets are flat or falling. This finding is therefore sometimes called the “Ostrich Effect”. Furthermore, Table 2 shows that only less than 5 percent of our investors think they had negative returns in the past while more than 25 percent actually had negative returns in the past. Thus, somehow mechanically, investors with high past returns are closer to their self-assessment on average. This is why we re-run the regression without stock portfolio performance as explanatory variable (see Regres- sion (2)). The results are similar. Stock market experience remains highly significantly negative at the 5 percent level.8 Furthermore, portfolio risk has a positive effect on the absolute difference between return 8All results are similar when we use a winsorization at the 2 percent or 5 percent level. For example, when variables are winsorized at the 5 percent level, the beta coefficient for experience is -0.185 with a p-value of 0.018 in Regression 1 and -0.171 (p-value = 0.041) in Regression 2. When we use quantile regressions, the significance of the experience variable in Regression 1 is even stronger (p-value = 0.003). In Regression 2, the experience variable remains significant at the 10 percent level. 14
  • 15. estimates and realized returns. This result is intuitive. The higher the standard deviation of returns, the more difficult is it to calculate the past realized stock portfolio performance. All the other variables are not robustly related to the dependent variable. Regression (3) shows that experienced investors and mutual fund investors are less likely to overesti- mate their past realized portfolio performance. However, this effect is not significant in Regression (4) anymore. Regressions (5) to (8) present the determinants of the (absolute) difference between per- ceived and actual percentile. Compared to Regressions (1) to (4), the adjusted R-squared values are quite low. Furthermore, we do not find a robust influence of our explanatory variables on the (absolute) difference between perceived and actual percentile. To summarize, we find that experience helps in calculating the own past realized portfolio performance. This task should be mainly driven by skills that are enhanced by invest- ment experience. In contrast, the other measures analyzed in Regressions (3) to (8) are closely related to the manifestations of overconfidence mentioned in the Introduction, especially overestimation of one’s actual performance and overplacement of one’s perfor- mance relative to others. The regression analysis in this part is exploratory in the spirit of Bhandari and Deaves (2006) who analyze the demographics of overconfidence. We had no ex ante hypothesis of the effect of expertise (and the other variables) on these over- confidence measures. Our analysis shows that our explanatory variables are not related to these overconfidence measures. In general, we can conclude that we find evidence that investor experience lessens the simple mathematical error of estimating portfolio returns, but seems not to influence their “behavioral” mistakes pertaining to how good (in absolute or relative sense) they are. 15
  • 16. 6 Summary, Discussion, and Conclusion Based on the answers of 215 online broker investors to an internet questionnaire we analyze whether investors are able to correctly estimate their own realized stock portfolio performance. Furthermore, we compare their perceived performance percentile with the actual performance percentile. Moreover, we analyze determinants of the cross-sectional heterogeneity in a regression analysis. The main findings can be summarized as follows. Investors are hardly able to give a correct estimate of their own past realized stock portfolio performance. Experienced investors are better able to do so. Furthermore, people overrate themselves. On average, investors think, that they are better than others. Moreover, the correlation between self ratings and actual performance is not distinguishable from zero. We find that investors do not have a good understanding of the concept “return”. This result is consistent with other studies. Parts of our results can be explained by psycholog- ical reasons (such as the negative influence of past portfolio performance on the absolute difference between return estimates and realized returns). However, this is only one part of the story. We also find that stock market investment experience has a positive influ- ence on the quality of estimates of past realized stock portfolio returns. This is consistent with other studies that document a positive effect of financial education on behavior. As investors are increasingly encouraged or even forced to invest for their own retirement savings, a good understanding of returns is essential. Future research should further in- vestigate why people have problems dealing with returns and how these problems can be mitigated. 16
  • 17. References Ackerman, P. L., M. E. Beier, and K. R. Bowen, 2002, “What we really know about our abilities and our knowledge,” Personality and Individual Differences, 33(4), 587–605. Amromin, G., and S. Sharpe, 2006, “From the Horse’s Mouth: Gauging Conditional Ex- pected Stock Returns from Investor Surveys,” Working paper, Federal Reserve Bank of Chicago. Andreassen, P. B., 1988, “Explaining the Price-Volume Relationship: The Difference be- tween Price Changes and Changing Prices,” Organizational Behavior and Human De- cision Processes, 41(3), 371–389. Atkinson, A., 1985, Plots, Transformations, and Regression. Clarendon Press, Oxford. Barber, B. M., and T. Odean, 2000, “Trading Is Hazardous to Your Wealth: The Common Stock Investment Performance of Individual Investors,” Journal of Finance, 55(2), 773– 806. Bhandari, G., and R. Deaves, 2006, “The Demographics of Overconfidence,” Journal of Behavioral Finance, 7(1), 5–11. Davidson, R., and J. G. MacKinnon, 1993, Estimation and Inference in Econometrics. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Dhar, R., and N. Zhu, 2006, “Up Close and Personal: Investor Sophistication and the Disposition Effect,” Management Science, 52(5), 726–740. Dunning, D., C. Heath, and J. M. Suls, 2004, “Flawed Self-Assessment: Implications for Health, Education, and the Workplace,” Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5(3), 69–106. 17
  • 18. Feng, L., and M. S. Seasholes, 2005, “Do Investor Sophistication and Trading Experience Eliminate Behavioral Biases in Financial Markets?,” Review of Finance, 9(3), 305–351. Gervais, S., and T. Odean, 2001, “Learning to Be Overconfident,” Review of Financial Studies, 14(1), 1–27. Glaser, M., T. Langer, J. Reynders, and M. Weber, 2007, “Framing Effects in Stock Market Forecasts: The Difference Between Asking for Prices and Asking for Returns,” Review of Finance, 11(2), 325–357. Glaser, M., M. N¨oth, and M. Weber, 2004, “Behavioral Finance,” in Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making, ed. by D. J. Koehler, and N. Harvey. Blackwell, Malden, Mass., pp. 527–546. Glaser, M., and M. Weber, 2005, “September 11 and Stock Return Expectations of Indi- vidual Investors,” Review of Finance, 9(2), 243–279. , 2007, “Overconfidence and Trading Volume,” Geneva Risk and Insurance Review, 32(1), 1–36. , 2008, “Which Past Returns Affect Trading Volume?,” Journal of Financial Mar- kets, forthcoming. Greenwood, R., and S. Nagel, 2007, “Inexperienced Investors and Bubbles,” Working paper. Hong, H., and J. C. Stein, 2007, “Disagreement and the Stock Market,” Journal of Eco- nomic Perspectives, 21(2), 109–128. Karlsson, N., G. Loewenstein, and D. Seppi, 2005, “The “Ostrich Effect”: Selective At- tention to Information about Investments,” Working paper. 18
  • 19. Kaustia, M., and S. Kn¨upfer, 2007, “Do Investors Learn from Personal Experience? Evi- dence from IPO Subscriptions,” Working paper, Helsinki School of Economics. Larrick, R. P., K. A. Burson, and J. B. Soll, 2007, “Social comparison and confidence: When thinking you’re better than average predicts overconfidence (and when it does not),” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102(1), 76–94. Moore, D. A., 2007, “Not so above average after all: When people believe they are worse than average and its implications for theories of bias in social comparison,” Organiza- tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102(1), 42–58. Moore, D. A., and D. M. Cain, 2007, “Overconfidence and underconfidence: When and why people underestimate (and overestimate) the competition,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 103(2), 197–213. Moore, D. A., and P. J. Healy, 2007, “The Trouble With Overconfidence,” Working pa- per, Carnegie Mellon University - David A. Tepper School of Business and Ohio State University - Department of Economics. Moore, D. A., and D. A. Small, 2007, “Error and Bias in Comparative Judgment: On Being Both Better and Worse Than We Think We Are,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(6), 972–989. Nicolosi, G., L. Peng, and N. Zhu, 2007, “Do Individual Investors Learn from Their Trading Experience?,” Working paper. Odean, T., 1998, “Volume, Volatility, Price, and Profit When All Traders Are Above Average,” Journal of Finance, 53(6), 1887–1934. Owhoso, V., and A. Weickgenannt, 2007, “Auditors’ self-perceived abilities in conducting domain audits,” Critical Perspectives on Accounting, forthconming. 19
  • 20. Seru, A., T. Shumway, and N. Stoffman, 2007, “Learning By Trading,” Working paper, University of Michigan. Spanos, A., 1986, Statistical foundations of econometric modelling. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Statman, M., S. Thorley, and K. Vorkink, 2006, “Investor Overconfidence and Trading Volume,” Review of Financial Studies, 19(4), 1531–1565. Svenson, O., 1981, “Are we all less risky and more skillful than our fellow drivers?,” Acta Psychologica, 47(2), 143–148. Taylor, S. S., and J. D. Brown, 1988, “Illusion and well being: A social psychology per- spective on mental health,” Psychological Bulletin, 103(2), 193–210. Weber, M., and F. Welfens, 2007, “An Individual Level Analysis of the Disposition Effect: Empirical and Experimental Evidence,” Working paper, University of Mannheim. 20
  • 21. Table 1: Cross-Sectional Distribution of Percentage Monthly Gross Portfolio Returns This table shows the cross-sectional distribution of the monthly gross returns of our investor sample (all investors and the subgroup of respondents to the questionnaire). Gross monthly portfolio performance of each investor was calculated making the following simplifying assumptions: We assume that all stocks are bought and sold at the end of the month and we ignore intra-month trading. The gross portfolio return Rht of investor h in month t is calculated as follows: Rht = Sht i=1 wihtRit with wiht = Pitniht Sht i=1 Pitniht Rit is the return of stock i in month t, Sht is the number of type of stocks held by individual h in month t, Pit is the price of stock i at the beginning of month t, and niht is the number of stocks of company i held by investor h in month t. wiht is the beginning-of-month-t market value of the holding of stock i of investor h divided by the beginning-of-month-t market value of the whole stock portfolio of investor h. Time period is January 1997 to March 2001. Investors with 12 or less portfolio return observations are excluded from the sample. The table also shows the arithmetic monthly return of the German blue chip index DAX from January 1997 to March 2001 and the number of investors with more than 12 portfolio return observations in our 51 month sample period. Parametric and non-parametric tests show that the distribution of monthly returns is not significantly different in the two groups. All Respondents to investors the questionnaire Mean 0.54% 0.30% Minimum −16.02% −10.73% 1st percentile −5.83% −8.15% 5th percentile −2.99% −3.96% 10th percentile −1.90% −2.11% 25th percentile −0.49% −0.58% Median 0.57% 0.53% 75th percentile 1.50% 1.40% 90th percentile 2.75% 2.52% 95th percentile 3.92% 3.42% 99th percentile 7.80% 6.06% Maximum 23.81% 7.09% DAX (arithmetic monthly return) 2.02% 2.02% Number of households 2,793 195 (91% of 3,079) (91% of 215) 21
  • 22. Table 2: Return Estimates We asked the investors to give an estimate of their portfolio performance in the past (from January 1997 to December 2000): Please try to estimate your past performance of your stock portfolio at your online broker. Please estimate the return of your stock portfolio from January 1997 to December 2000: [Answer] percent per year on average. This table presents the answers to this question (mean, median, standard deviation, skewness, kurtosis, minimum, maximum, and various percentiles). Number of observations 210 Mean 14.93 % Standard deviation 13.11 % Skewness 2.01 Kurtosis 24.33 Minimum −50 % 1st percentile −15 % 5th percentile 0 % 10th percentile 5 % 25th percentile 10 % Median 15 % 75th percentile 20 % 90th percentile 27 % 95th percentile 35 % 99th percentile 41 % Maximum 120 % 22
  • 23. Table 3: Return Estimates, Self-Assessments, and Experience This table presents mean, median, the number of observations as well as the p-value of a Wilcoxon test (null hypothesis: value is equal to 0) of the absolute return difference, the difference between the perceived return and the actual return, the absolute performance percentile difference and the difference between actual performance percentile and the perceived performance percentile for all investors (with stock market investment experience variable available) and investors with low (less than 5 years) and high (more than 5 years) stock market investment experience. 5 years is the median experience level in our data set so that we obtain two groups of approximately equal size. See Sections 3 and 4 for details. Furthermore, the table shows the number of cases and the percentage of cases in which the sign of the past return assessment was correct. Moreover, the table shows the number of investors who are reasonably close with their estimates (see the lines which show the number of investors who are less than 5 percentage points or 10 percentage points wrong). Variables are winsorized at the 10 percent level. * indicates significance at 10%; ** indicates significance at 5%; *** indicates significance at 1%. All investors Low investment High investment p-value (with experience experience experience (Mann-Whitney) variable available) (less than 5 years) (more than 5 years) (difference in experience groups) Absolute return Mean 20.96 23.68 18.73 0.098* difference Median 17.84 21.01 16.71 Observations 142 64 78 Different from 0 p < 0.0001*** p < 0.0001*** p < 0.0001*** (Wilcoxon) Correct sign 87 37 50 Wrong sign 55 27 28 Percent correct 61.27% 57.81% 64.10% Less than 5 percentage 35 13 22 points wrong Less than 10 percentage 48 18 30 points wrong Perceived return Mean 11.61 13.18 10.32 0.42 -actual return Median 13.85 14.13 12.41 Observations 142 64 78 Different from 0 p < 0.0001*** p < 0.0001*** p < 0.0001*** (Wilcoxon) Absolute percentile Mean 25.33 25.31 25.35 0.99 difference Median 26.00 23.00 27.00 Observations 140 62 78 Different from 0 p < 0.0001*** p < 0.0001*** p < 0.0001*** (Wilcoxon) Actual percentile Mean 4.44 5.61 3.50 0.73 -perceived percentile Median 4.00 3.50 4.00 Observations 140 62 78 Different from 0 0.0485** 0.103 0.2328 (Wilcoxon) 23
  • 24. Table 4: Self-Ratings We asked the investors to answer the following question: What percentage of customers of your discount brokerage house had higher returns than you in the four-year period from January 1997 to December 2000? (Please give a number between 0 % and 100 %) [Answer] percent of other customers had higher returns than I did. This table presents the answers to this question (mean, median, standard deviation, skewness, kurtosis, minimum, maximum, and various percentiles). Number of observations 212 Mean 46.99 Standard deviation 19.33 Skewness 0.04 Kurtosis 2.87 Minimum 2 1st percentile 5 5th percentile 15 10th percentile 20 25th percentile 30 Median 50 75th percentile 60 90th percentile 70 95th percentile 80 99th percentile 90 Maximum 95 24
  • 25. Table5:DeterminantsoftheDifferenceBetweenReturnEstimatesandRealizedReturnsandBetweenPerceivedandActualReturn Percentile:Cross-SectionalRegressions Thistablepresentscross-sectionalregressionresultsonthedeterminantsoftheabsolutedifferencebetweenreturnestimatesandrealizedreturns(Regressions(1)and(2)), thedifferencebetweenreturnestimatesandrealizedreturns(Regressions(3)and(4)),theabsolutedifferencebetweenperceivedandactualreturnpercentile(Regressions (5)and(6)),andthedifferencebetweenactualandperceivedreturnpercentile(Regressions(7)and(8))asdependentvariablesandstockmarketinvestmentexperience,a genderdummyvariable(thevariableissetequalto1iftheinvestorismale),age,amutualfundinvestordummy(thevariableissetequalto1iftheinvestortradesfunds atleastonceinthetimeperiodfromJanuary1997untilApril2001),awarranttraderdummyvariable(thevariableissetequalto1iftheinvestortradeswarrantsat leastonceinthetimeperiodfromJanuary1997untilApril2001),ahighriskdummy(thevariableissetequalto1iftheinvestorclassifiesherorhisinvestmentstrategy ashighrisk),thelogarithmofmeanmonthlystockportfoliovalue,thetime-seriesaverageofthemonthlystockportfolioperformanceofaninvestor,thelogarithmof thestandarddeviationofmonthlystockportfolioperformanceasameasureofportfoliorisk,andthelogarithmofnumberofstocksinportfolio.Investmentexperience isreportedwithinfiveranges,wherethetoprangeismorethan15years.Intheregressionsweusethemidpointofeachrangeand17.5yearsforthetoprange.The dependentvariablesandthemonthlystockportfolioperformancearewinsorizedatthe10percentlevel.Thetablereportsstandardizedbetacoefficients(exceptforthe intercept).Robustp-valuesareinparentheses.*indicatessignificanceat10%;**indicatessignificanceat5%;***indicatessignificanceat1%. AbsolutedifferenceDifferenceAbsolutedifferenceDifference betweenbetweenbetweenbetween returnestimateandreturnestimateandactualpercentileactualpercentile realizedreturnrealizedreturnandperceivedpercentileandperceivedpercentile (1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8) Stockmarketinvestmentexperience-0.202-0.186-0.088-0.057-0.040-0.036-0.088-0.056 (inyears)(0.010)***(0.029)**(0.051)*(0.519)(0.646)(0.691)(0.179)(0.543) Gender-0.074-0.0560.0110.047-0.032-0.0280.0280.056 (Dummy;men=1)(0.523)(0.619)(0.557)(0.686)(0.722)(0.749)(0.225)(0.596) Age-0.014-0.0600.038-0.051-0.096-0.1070.027-0.047 (0.854)(0.538)(0.490)(0.622)(0.403)(0.344)(0.737)(0.637) Mutualfundinvestor-0.102-0.034-0.07580.058-0.051-0.033-0.0740.041 (Dummy)(0.197)(0.694)(0.076)*(0.539)(0.579)(0.712)(0.196)(0.676) Warranttrader-0.063-0.0290.0310.0990.0460.057-0.0440.026 (Dummy)(0.434)(0.759)(0.417)(0.311)(0.647)(0.574)(0.447)(0.785) Highriskinvestmentstrategy0.031-0.054-0.036-0.2010.0280.0050.0158-0.131 (basedonself-report;dummy)(0.717)(0.460)(0.427)(0.026)**(0.780)(0.960)(0.784)(0.189) ln(stockportfoliovalue)0.0370.062-0.0250.0241-0.236-0.226-0.0180.043 (inEUR;time-seriesaverageperinvestor)(0.771)(0.645)(0.704)(0.879)(0.136)(0.150)(0.857)(0.800) Stockportfolioperformance-0.467-0.919-0.126-0.813 (time-seriesaverageperinvestor)(0.000)***(0.000)***(0.232)(0.000)*** ln(portfoliorisk)0.2210.231-0.048-0.027-0.011-0.0080.0570.079 (standarddeviationofmonthlyportfolioreturns)(0.063)*(0.044)**(0.351)(0.839)(0.918)(0.943)(0.385)(0.505) ln(numberofstocksinportfolio)-0.151-0.1750.012-0.0360.0720.0650.1460.100 (time-seriesaverageperinvestor)(0.231)(0.230)(0.882)(0.825)(0.628)(0.658)(0.150)(0.498) Constant55.60652.04214.8005.35259.10157.77214.235-0.679 (0.000)***(0.001)***(0.039)**(0.820)(0.000)***(0.000)***(0.327)(0.983) Observations121121121121119119119119 AdjustedR-squared0.3250.1100.8280.0000.0020.0000.6290.000 25
  • 26. Figure 1: Return Estimates and Realized Returns This figure plots return estimates versus realized portfolio returns of the individual investors who answered the questionnaire. Furthermore, the figure shows a 45-degree line. Variables are winsorized at the 10 percent level. -30 -20 -10 0 10 20 30 40 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 Return estimates (% p.a.) Realizedreturns(%p.a.) 26
  • 27. Figure 2: Self-Ratings in Percentiles and Actual Percentiles This figure plots the self-ratings in percentiles versus actual percentiles of the individual investors who answered the questionnaire. 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 Self-rating (past returns in percentiles) Actualpercentile 27