SlideShare una empresa de Scribd logo
1 de 15
Descargar para leer sin conexión
Writing a Short Literature Review
                              William Ashton, Ph.D.
                              York College, CUNY

A student began a short literature review on the stigma of the mentally ill and
perceptions of dangerousness. Working through PsychArticles she found three
likely articles. When she read each, she wrote a paragraph description of each:

Alexander, L.A., & Link, B.G. (2003). The impact of contact on stigmatizing
     attitudes towards people with mental illness. Journal of Mental Health, 12,
     271-289.

       Alexander and Link (2003) examined the stigma of mental illness,
perceptions of dangerousness and social distance in a telephone survey. They
found that, as a participant’s own life contact with mentally ill individuals
increased, participants were both less likely to perceive a target mentally ill
individual in a vignette as physically dangerous and less likely to desire social
distance from the target. This relationship remained after controlling for
demographic and confound variables, such as gender, ethnicity, education,
income and political conservatism. They also found that any type of contact –
with a friend, a spouse, a family member, a work contact, or a contact in a public
place – with mentally ill individuals reduced perceptions of dangerousness of the
target in the vignette.



Corrigan, P. W., Rowan, D., Green, A., Lundin, R., River, P., Uphoff-Wasowski, K.,
      White, K., & Kubiak, M.A. (2002). Challenging two mental illness stigmas:
      Personality responsibility and dangerousness. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 28,
      293-309.

       Corrigan, Rowan, Green, Lundin, River, Uphoff-Wasowski, White and
Kubiak (2002) conducted two studies to investigate the strength of the theoretical
relationship between stigma and personality responsibility, and stigma and
dangerousness. Corrigan et al. posited two models to account for stigmatizing
reactions. In the first model, labeled personal responsibility, personality
responsibility influences both the level of pity and anger displayed toward mental
patients. Additionally, the variables of pity and anger influence helping behavior.
In the second model, labeled dangerousness, perceived dangerousness
influences fear of mental patients, which in turn influences the avoidance of the
mentally ill.
       In their first study, Corrigan etal. (2002), administered a questionnaire to
216 community college students. This questionnaire contained items which
would allow the examination of the two models. The results of a path analysis
indicated that while both models fit the data, the results for the dangerousness
model seemed far more consistent with the data. Their second study was an
attempt to manipulate variables in the models. Participants met with either an
educational group or a mental patient. During the meetings, either myths about
the personality responsibility or the dangerousness of mental patients were
discussed and debunked. While education yielded some positive results, contact
with mental patients produced stronger results.



Martin, J. K., Pescosolido, B. A., & Tuch, S. A. (2000). Of fear and loathing: The
       role of ‘disturbing behavior’ labels, and causal attributions in shaping
       public attitudes toward people with mental illness. Journal of Health and
       Social Behavior, 41, 208-223.

       Martin, Pescosolido & Tuch (2000) examined the effects of descriptions of
the targets’ behavior, causal attributions about the source of the behavior, the
target’s perceived dangerousness, labeling and participants’ sociodemographic
characteristics. Twenty percent of the participants labeled a target described with
depressed symptoms as having a mental illness (as compared with 54% for those
described with schizophrenic symptoms or 1% with normal troubles); 37% would
be unwilling to interact with the depressed person (48% for the schizophrenic and
21% for normal troubles); and 33% felt that the depressed person would do
violence to others (61% for the schizophrenic and 17% for the normal troubles).

Next she decided upon the order of the paragraphs in the paper:

Martin, Pescosolido & Tuch (2000) examined the effects of descriptions of the

targets’ behavior, causal attributions about the source of the behavior, the target’s

perceived dangerousness, labeling and participants’ sociodemographic

characteristics. Twenty percent of the participants labeled a target described with

depressed symptoms as having a mental illness (as compared with 54% for those

described with schizophrenic symptoms or 1% with normal troubles); 37% would

be unwilling to interact with the depressed person (48% for the schizophrenic and

21% for normal troubles); and 33% felt that the depressed person would do

violence to others (61% for the schizophrenic and 17% for the normal troubles).
Alexander and Link (2003) examined the stigma of mental illness,

perceptions of dangerousness and social distance in a telephone survey. They

found that, as a participant’s own life contact with mentally ill individuals

increased, participants were both less likely to perceive a target mentally ill

individual in a vignette as physically dangerous and less likely to desire social

distance from the target. This relationship remained after controlling for

demographic and confound variables, such as gender, ethnicity, education,

income and political conservatism. They also found that any type of contact –

with a friend, a spouse, a family member, a work contact, or a contact in a public

place – with mentally ill individuals reduced perceptions of dangerousness of the

target in the vignette.

       Corrigan, Rowan, Green, Lundin, River, Uphoff-Wasowski, White and

Kubiak (2002) conducted two studies to investigate the strength of the theoretical

relationship between stigma and personality responsibility, and stigma and

dangerousness. Corrigan et al. posited two models to account for stigmatizing

reactions. In the first model, labeled personal responsibility, personality

responsibility influences both the level of pity and anger displayed toward mental

patients. Additionally, the variables of pity and anger influence helping behavior.

In the second model, labeled dangerousness, perceived dangerousness

influences fear of mental patients, which in turn influences the avoidance of the

mentally ill.

       In their first study, Corrigan etal. (2002), administered a questionnaire to

216 community college students. This questionnaire contained items which
would allow the examination of the two models. The results of a path analysis

indicated that while both models fit the data, the results for the dangerousness

model seemed far more consistent with the data. Their second study was an

attempt to manipulate variables in the models. Participants met with either an

educational group or a mental patient. During the meetings, either myths about

the personality responsibility or the dangerousness of mental patients were

discussed and debunked. While education yielded some positive results, contact

with mental patients produced stronger results.

She carefully chose the order of the paragraphs so she could talk about: (1) that
people respond to the mentally ill with fear and rejection, (2) contact reduces both
rejection and fear and (3) how to best arrange the contact to reduce stigma.

Now she added introductory and concluding sentences, paragraph hooks and
short transition paragraphs to help the flow of ideas.

      Regarding the mentally ill, it appears that people respond to the mentally ill

with feelings of fear and rejection. Martin, Pescosolido & Tuch (2000) examined

the effects of descriptions of the targets’ behavior, causal attributions about the

source of the behavior, the target’s perceived dangerousness, labeling and

participants’ sociodemographic characteristics. Twenty percent of the

participants labeled a target described with depressed symptoms as having a

mental illness (as compared with 54% for those described with schizophrenic

symptoms or 1% with normal troubles); 37% would be unwilling to interact with

the depressed person (48% for the schizophrenic and 21% for normal troubles);

and 33% felt that the depressed person would do violence to others (61% for the
schizophrenic and 17% for the normal troubles). Thus, a common respond to the

mentally ill are rejection and fear of violence.

      While, based upon research, the common response to a mentally ill person

is to fear violence, diagnosed mental patients commit violence at the same rates

as non-diagnosed people (Martin, et al., 2000). Public perceptions may not match

reality due to the public’s lack of contact with the mentally ill.

      Alexander and Link (2003) examined contact with the mentally ill and the

stigma of mental illness, perceptions of dangerousness and social distance in a

telephone survey. They found that, as a participant’s own life contact with

mentally ill individuals increased, participants were both less likely to perceive a

target mentally ill individual in a vignette as physically dangerous and less likely

to desire social distance from the target. This relationship remained after

controlling for demographic and confound variables, such as gender, ethnicity,

education, income and political conservatism. They also found that any type of

contact – with a friend, a spouse, a family member, a work contact, or a contact in

a public place – with mentally ill individuals reduced perceptions of

dangerousness of the target in the vignette. Thus, according to Alexander and

Link (2003), any contact with the mentally ill is associated with reduced fear and

rejection. However, since this study was observational in nature, we cannot know

if contact reduces fear or having lower fear increased contact.

      Corrigan, Rowan, Green, Lundin, River, Uphoff-Wasowski, White and

Kubiak (2002) conducted two studies examining the causal processes in contact,

fear and rejection. Corrigan et al. posited two models to account for stigmatizing
reactions. In the first model, labeled personal responsibility, beliefs about

personality responsibility influences both the level of pity and anger displayed

toward mental patients. Additionally, the variables of pity and anger influence

helping behavior. In the second model, labeled dangerousness, perceived

dangerousness influences fear of mental patients, which in turn influences the

avoidance of the mentally ill.

        In their first study, Corrigan et al. (2002) administered a questionnaire to

216 community college students. This questionnaire contained items which

would allow the examination of the two models. The results of a path analysis

indicated that while both models fit the data, the results for the dangerousness

model seemed far more consistent with the data. Their second study was an

attempt to manipulate variables in the models. Participants met with either an

educational group or with a mental patient. During the meetings, myths about the

personality responsibility or the dangerousness of mental patients were

discussed and debunked. While education yielded some positive results

regarding fear and rejection, contact with mental patients produced stronger

results. Thus, Corrigan et al. demonstrated that contact causes less rejection and

fear.

In the final stage, she needs to write an introductory and concluding paragraph.
She wrote the concluding paragraph first. In this paragraph she needs to
overview the paper and make a conclusion.

It appears that the mentally ill are rejected because of the public’s fear of the

mentally ill. At least one-third of the people sampled in one study said that they

would both reject socially and fear violence from someone displaying behaviors
associated with different mentally illnesses. Other research discovered that this

rejection is associated to lack of contact with the mentally ill and that as contact

increased, fear of the mentally ill decreased. The direction of the relationship

between fear and rejection seems to be that fear (possibly based upon myths

about mental illness) causes rejection. Taken as a whole, it appears that

exposing these myths as myths increases the acceptance of the mentally ill and

that staged contact with a mentally person to expose myths has an even more

powerful effect.

Now she needs to say something about the research methods.

Caution must be advised, though; Martin et al.’s (2002) and Alexander and Link’s

(2003) studies and the first study of Corrigan et al. (2002) were based upon paper

and pencil methodologies. And while Corrigan et al.’s (2002) second study

involved staged presentations, it was conducted in a college setting with a college

sample. Future research should replicate these findings in more natural settings

with different populations.

The student then brought her draft to me. After I read it I asked her about the first
sentence of her conclusion. I asked her if she could phrase it as a clear and
strong statement. She did:

The rejection of the mentally ill is caused by the public’s belief in myths about the

dangerousness of the mentally ill and exposing those myths can reduce rejection.


Now she needs to write the Introduction. With an introduction, begin broad and
narrow down to the thesis statement. The thesis statement is the last sentence in
the introduction and the first sentence in the conclusion.
The mentally ill face a multitude of challenges. One of those challenges is the

stigmatization they face. Stigmatization is social rejection; they are rejected by

people because of the label they carry or that their behaviors clearly indicate that

they belong to a certain labeled group. Stigmatization of the mentally ill is caused

by the public’s belief in myths about the dangerousness of the mentally ill and

exposing those myths can reduce stigmatization.

Here’s how her whole paper looked:
Myths of violence 1




         Myths of violence and the stigma of mental illness12



                                     Suzie Student

                                 York College, CUNY




1For the text of an APA article, use Courier or New Times Roman font at 12 pts.
2Before the page number is the Page Header (sometimes called, Key Words) the first few words
of your title.
Myths of violence 2


              Myths of violence and the stigma of mental illness3

          The mentally ill face a multitude of challenges.                         One of

those challenges is the stigmatization they face.

Stigmatization is social rejection; those stigmatized are

rejected by people because of the label they carry or that their

behaviors clearly indicate that they belong to a certain labeled

group.         Stigmatization of the mentally ill is caused by the

public’s belief in myths about the dangerousness of the mentally

ill and exposing those myths can reduce stigmatization.

          Regarding the mentally ill, it appears that people respond

to the mentally ill with feelings of fear and rejection.

Martin, Pescosolido & Tuch (2000)4 examined the effects of

descriptions of the targets’ behavior, causal attributions about

the source of the behavior, the target’s perceived

dangerousness, labeling and participants’ sociodemographic

characteristics.                Twenty percent of the participants labeled a

target described with depressed symptoms as having a mental

illness (as compared with 54% for those described with

schizophrenic symptoms or 1% with normal troubles); 37% would be

unwilling to interact with the depressed person (48% for the

schizophrenic and 21% for normal troubles); and 33% felt that

the depressed person would do violence to others (61% for the



3   before the text begins, repeat the title, centered.
4   the first time you cite an article, list all of the authors’ names.
Myths of violence 3


schizophrenic and 17% for the normal troubles).                            Thus, a common

respond to the mentally ill are rejection and fear of violence.

       While, based upon research, the common response to a

mentally ill person is to fear violence, diagnosed mental

patients commit violence at the same rates as non-diagnosed

people (Martin, et al., 2000)5.                   Public perceptions may not match

reality due to the public’s lack of contact with the mentally

ill.

       Alexander and Link (2003) examined contact with the

mentally ill and the stigma of mental illness, perceptions of

dangerousness and social distance in a telephone survey.                                 They

found that, as a participant’s own life contact with mentally

ill individuals increased, participants were both less likely to

perceive a target mentally ill individual in a vignette as

physically dangerous and less likely to desire social distance

from the target.           This relationship remained after controlling

for demographic and confound variables, such as gender,

ethnicity, education, income and political conservatism.                                 They

also found that any type of contact – with a friend, a spouse, a

family member, a work contact, or a contact in a public place –

with mentally ill individuals reduced perceptions of

dangerousness of the target in the vignette.                          Thus, according to



5the second (etc) times you cite an article, you can use et al. if the article has more than 2
authors
Myths of violence 4


Alexander and Link (2003), any contact with the mentally ill is

associated with reduced fear and rejection.   However, since this

study was observational in nature, we cannot know if contact

reduces fear or having lower fear increased contact.

     Corrigan, Rowan, Green, Lundin, River, Uphoff-Wasowski,

White and Kubiak (2002) conducted two studies examining the

causal processes in contact, fear and rejection.   Corrigan et

al. posited two models to account for stigmatizing reactions.

In the first model, labeled personal responsibility, beliefs

about personality responsibility influences both the level of

pity and anger displayed toward mental patients.   Additionally,

the variables of pity and anger influence helping behavior.     In

the second model, labeled dangerousness, perceived dangerousness

influences fear of mental patients, which in turn influences the

avoidance of the mentally ill.

     In their first study, Corrigan et al. (2002) administered a

questionnaire to 216 community college students.   This

questionnaire contained items which would allow the examination

of the two models.   The results of a path analysis indicated

that while both models fit the data, the results for the

dangerousness model seemed far more consistent with the data.

Their second study was an attempt to manipulate variables in the

models.   Participants met with either an educational group or

with a mental patient.   During the meetings, myths about the
Myths of violence 5


personality responsibility or the dangerousness of mental

patients were discussed and debunked.   While education yielded

some positive results regarding fear and rejection, contact with

mental patients produced stronger results.   Thus, Corrigan et

al. demonstrated that contact causes less rejection and fear.

     Stigmatization of the mentally ill is caused by the

public’s belief in myths about the dangerousness of the mentally

ill and exposing those myths can reduce stigmatization.     At

least one-third of the people sampled in one study said that

they would both reject socially and fear violence from someone

displaying behaviors associated with different mentally

illnesses.   Other research discovered that this rejection is

associated to lack of contact with the mentally ill and that as

contact increased, fear of the mentally ill decreased.    The

direction of the relationship between fear and rejection seems

to be that fear (possibly based upon myths about mental illness)

causes rejection.   Taken as a whole, it appears that exposing

these myths as myths increases the acceptance of the mentally

ill and that staged contact with a mentally person to expose

myths has an even more powerful effect.   Caution must be

advised, though; Martin et al.’s (2002) and Alexander and Link’s

(2003) studies and the first study of Corrigan et al. (2002)

were based upon paper and pencil methodologies.   And while

Corrigan et al.’s (2002) second study involved staged
Myths of violence 6


presentations, it was conducted in a college setting with a

college sample.   Future research should replicate these findings

in more natural settings with different populations.
Myths of violence 7


                                      References6

Alexander, L.A., & Link, B.G. (2003). The impact of contact on

         stigmatizing attitudes towards people with mental illness.

         Journal of Mental Health, 12, 271-289.

Corrigan, P. W., Rowan, D., Green, A., Lundin, R., River, P.,

         Uphoff-Wasowski, K., White, K., & Kubiak, M.A. (2002).

         Challenging two mental illness stigmas: Personality

         responsibility and dangerousness. Schizophrenia Bulletin,

         28, 293-309.

Martin, J. K., Pescosolido, B. A., & Tuch, S. A. (2000). Of fear

          and loathing: The role of ‘disturbing behavior’ labels, and

          causal attributions in shaping public attitudes toward

          people with mental illness. Journal of Health and Social

          Behavior, 41, 208-223.




6   references begin on a new page.

Más contenido relacionado

La actualidad más candente

Wallace, Grotzinger, Howard, & Parkhill (2015)
Wallace, Grotzinger, Howard, & Parkhill (2015)Wallace, Grotzinger, Howard, & Parkhill (2015)
Wallace, Grotzinger, Howard, & Parkhill (2015)Tyler Howard
 
Violence prevention programs an exploratory study of the ch
Violence prevention programs an exploratory study of the chViolence prevention programs an exploratory study of the ch
Violence prevention programs an exploratory study of the chojas18
 
Final Field Experience Paper
Final Field Experience PaperFinal Field Experience Paper
Final Field Experience PaperTiffany Thomas
 
Is the sociopath socially intelligent? A new framework for understanding soci...
Is the sociopath socially intelligent? A new framework for understanding soci...Is the sociopath socially intelligent? A new framework for understanding soci...
Is the sociopath socially intelligent? A new framework for understanding soci...Rula alsawalqa
 
The Influence of Cognitive Behavioral group Interventions for HIV Seropositiv...
The Influence of Cognitive Behavioral group Interventions for HIV Seropositiv...The Influence of Cognitive Behavioral group Interventions for HIV Seropositiv...
The Influence of Cognitive Behavioral group Interventions for HIV Seropositiv...Madridge Publishers Pvt Ltd
 
The social stigma associated with gun ownership in urban versus rural environ...
The social stigma associated with gun ownership in urban versus rural environ...The social stigma associated with gun ownership in urban versus rural environ...
The social stigma associated with gun ownership in urban versus rural environ...Kate Novak
 
NSSI and Identity Research Poster
NSSI and Identity Research PosterNSSI and Identity Research Poster
NSSI and Identity Research PosterRachel Keith
 
The relationship between types ofchildhood victimisation and
The relationship between types ofchildhood victimisation andThe relationship between types ofchildhood victimisation and
The relationship between types ofchildhood victimisation andojas18
 
Sample 3 bipolar on female adult population
Sample 3  bipolar on female adult populationSample 3  bipolar on female adult population
Sample 3 bipolar on female adult populationNicole Valerio
 
Attachment Studies With Borderline Patients
Attachment Studies With Borderline PatientsAttachment Studies With Borderline Patients
Attachment Studies With Borderline PatientsDemona Demona
 
Child Maltreatment in Abnormal Psychology Textbooks
Child Maltreatment in Abnormal Psychology TextbooksChild Maltreatment in Abnormal Psychology Textbooks
Child Maltreatment in Abnormal Psychology TextbooksParisa Kaliush
 
Childhood Maltreatment and PTSD Literature Review and Proposed Study
Childhood Maltreatment and PTSD Literature Review and Proposed StudyChildhood Maltreatment and PTSD Literature Review and Proposed Study
Childhood Maltreatment and PTSD Literature Review and Proposed StudyAlexandraPerkins5
 
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior p
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior pPrepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior p
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior pamit657720
 
Glenthøj et al. - 2016 - Social cognition in patients at ultra-high risk for ...
Glenthøj et al. - 2016 - Social cognition in patients at ultra-high risk for ...Glenthøj et al. - 2016 - Social cognition in patients at ultra-high risk for ...
Glenthøj et al. - 2016 - Social cognition in patients at ultra-high risk for ...Tina Dam Kristensen
 
Literature Review_final draft
Literature Review_final draftLiterature Review_final draft
Literature Review_final draftSherri Wielgos
 
CONSIDERATIONS FROM THE MALTREATMENT AND ADOLESCENT PATHWAYS (MAP) LONGITUDIN...
CONSIDERATIONS FROM THE MALTREATMENT AND ADOLESCENT PATHWAYS (MAP) LONGITUDIN...CONSIDERATIONS FROM THE MALTREATMENT AND ADOLESCENT PATHWAYS (MAP) LONGITUDIN...
CONSIDERATIONS FROM THE MALTREATMENT AND ADOLESCENT PATHWAYS (MAP) LONGITUDIN...Christine Wekerle
 
Intimate Partner Violence: Prevalence, impact, treatment, prevention
Intimate Partner Violence: Prevalence, impact, treatment, preventionIntimate Partner Violence: Prevalence, impact, treatment, prevention
Intimate Partner Violence: Prevalence, impact, treatment, preventionParisa Kaliush
 

La actualidad más candente (20)

Wallace, Grotzinger, Howard, & Parkhill (2015)
Wallace, Grotzinger, Howard, & Parkhill (2015)Wallace, Grotzinger, Howard, & Parkhill (2015)
Wallace, Grotzinger, Howard, & Parkhill (2015)
 
Pinto gouveia
Pinto gouveia Pinto gouveia
Pinto gouveia
 
Violence prevention programs an exploratory study of the ch
Violence prevention programs an exploratory study of the chViolence prevention programs an exploratory study of the ch
Violence prevention programs an exploratory study of the ch
 
Final Field Experience Paper
Final Field Experience PaperFinal Field Experience Paper
Final Field Experience Paper
 
Is the sociopath socially intelligent? A new framework for understanding soci...
Is the sociopath socially intelligent? A new framework for understanding soci...Is the sociopath socially intelligent? A new framework for understanding soci...
Is the sociopath socially intelligent? A new framework for understanding soci...
 
The Influence of Cognitive Behavioral group Interventions for HIV Seropositiv...
The Influence of Cognitive Behavioral group Interventions for HIV Seropositiv...The Influence of Cognitive Behavioral group Interventions for HIV Seropositiv...
The Influence of Cognitive Behavioral group Interventions for HIV Seropositiv...
 
The social stigma associated with gun ownership in urban versus rural environ...
The social stigma associated with gun ownership in urban versus rural environ...The social stigma associated with gun ownership in urban versus rural environ...
The social stigma associated with gun ownership in urban versus rural environ...
 
NSSI and Identity Research Poster
NSSI and Identity Research PosterNSSI and Identity Research Poster
NSSI and Identity Research Poster
 
The relationship between types ofchildhood victimisation and
The relationship between types ofchildhood victimisation andThe relationship between types ofchildhood victimisation and
The relationship between types ofchildhood victimisation and
 
Sample 3 bipolar on female adult population
Sample 3  bipolar on female adult populationSample 3  bipolar on female adult population
Sample 3 bipolar on female adult population
 
Attachment Studies With Borderline Patients
Attachment Studies With Borderline PatientsAttachment Studies With Borderline Patients
Attachment Studies With Borderline Patients
 
Child Maltreatment in Abnormal Psychology Textbooks
Child Maltreatment in Abnormal Psychology TextbooksChild Maltreatment in Abnormal Psychology Textbooks
Child Maltreatment in Abnormal Psychology Textbooks
 
Halford Capstone Paper
Halford Capstone PaperHalford Capstone Paper
Halford Capstone Paper
 
Childhood Maltreatment and PTSD Literature Review and Proposed Study
Childhood Maltreatment and PTSD Literature Review and Proposed StudyChildhood Maltreatment and PTSD Literature Review and Proposed Study
Childhood Maltreatment and PTSD Literature Review and Proposed Study
 
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior p
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior pPrepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior p
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior p
 
Glenthøj et al. - 2016 - Social cognition in patients at ultra-high risk for ...
Glenthøj et al. - 2016 - Social cognition in patients at ultra-high risk for ...Glenthøj et al. - 2016 - Social cognition in patients at ultra-high risk for ...
Glenthøj et al. - 2016 - Social cognition in patients at ultra-high risk for ...
 
Literature Review_final draft
Literature Review_final draftLiterature Review_final draft
Literature Review_final draft
 
CONSIDERATIONS FROM THE MALTREATMENT AND ADOLESCENT PATHWAYS (MAP) LONGITUDIN...
CONSIDERATIONS FROM THE MALTREATMENT AND ADOLESCENT PATHWAYS (MAP) LONGITUDIN...CONSIDERATIONS FROM THE MALTREATMENT AND ADOLESCENT PATHWAYS (MAP) LONGITUDIN...
CONSIDERATIONS FROM THE MALTREATMENT AND ADOLESCENT PATHWAYS (MAP) LONGITUDIN...
 
RJunaid Final Paper
RJunaid Final PaperRJunaid Final Paper
RJunaid Final Paper
 
Intimate Partner Violence: Prevalence, impact, treatment, prevention
Intimate Partner Violence: Prevalence, impact, treatment, preventionIntimate Partner Violence: Prevalence, impact, treatment, prevention
Intimate Partner Violence: Prevalence, impact, treatment, prevention
 

Similar a Lit rev eg

International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)inventionjournals
 
Junxian KuangLaura SinaiENG099101572018In the essay O.docx
Junxian KuangLaura SinaiENG099101572018In the essay O.docxJunxian KuangLaura SinaiENG099101572018In the essay O.docx
Junxian KuangLaura SinaiENG099101572018In the essay O.docxtawnyataylor528
 
Lesson 11 Mental Health StigmaReadings Please note that th.docx
Lesson 11  Mental Health StigmaReadings  Please note that th.docxLesson 11  Mental Health StigmaReadings  Please note that th.docx
Lesson 11 Mental Health StigmaReadings Please note that th.docxSHIVA101531
 
For your first reflection please watch the following video and res.docx
For your first reflection please watch the following video and res.docxFor your first reflection please watch the following video and res.docx
For your first reflection please watch the following video and res.docxbudbarber38650
 
Honors Symposium Paper
Honors Symposium PaperHonors Symposium Paper
Honors Symposium PaperIsaac Suh
 
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of Disabled
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of DisabledPeer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of Disabled
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of DisabledAbigail Burmeister
 
Concepts of stigma and its relevance in Mental Health
Concepts of stigma and its relevance in Mental HealthConcepts of stigma and its relevance in Mental Health
Concepts of stigma and its relevance in Mental HealthSabana Nasrin Islam
 
IMPLICATIONS FOR TRAUMA-INFORMED CARE12Implications for Trauma-In
IMPLICATIONS FOR TRAUMA-INFORMED CARE12Implications for Trauma-InIMPLICATIONS FOR TRAUMA-INFORMED CARE12Implications for Trauma-In
IMPLICATIONS FOR TRAUMA-INFORMED CARE12Implications for Trauma-InMalikPinckney86
 
ARTICLE REVIEW10Why do people discriminate a.docx
ARTICLE REVIEW10Why do people discriminate a.docxARTICLE REVIEW10Why do people discriminate a.docx
ARTICLE REVIEW10Why do people discriminate a.docxrossskuddershamus
 
0c96052be437ddbf1f000000(2)
0c96052be437ddbf1f000000(2)0c96052be437ddbf1f000000(2)
0c96052be437ddbf1f000000(2)Helton Lima
 
Article 2 Direct And Indirect Intergroup Friendship(Real I Hope)
Article 2 Direct And Indirect Intergroup Friendship(Real I Hope)Article 2 Direct And Indirect Intergroup Friendship(Real I Hope)
Article 2 Direct And Indirect Intergroup Friendship(Real I Hope)socialprejudice2008
 
M7 A2 Domestic Violence
M7 A2 Domestic ViolenceM7 A2 Domestic Violence
M7 A2 Domestic Violencewerts4now
 
Literature ReviewAshley AshleyColumbia Southern .docx
Literature ReviewAshley AshleyColumbia Southern .docxLiterature ReviewAshley AshleyColumbia Southern .docx
Literature ReviewAshley AshleyColumbia Southern .docxpauline234567
 
ANGELO.Psychosis characteristics (thesis)May25th2015 (final version) (1)
ANGELO.Psychosis characteristics (thesis)May25th2015 (final version) (1)ANGELO.Psychosis characteristics (thesis)May25th2015 (final version) (1)
ANGELO.Psychosis characteristics (thesis)May25th2015 (final version) (1)Angelo Laine
 
Criminal behavior among the homeless individuals
Criminal behavior among the homeless individualsCriminal behavior among the homeless individuals
Criminal behavior among the homeless individualsRichardWahaba1
 
The Socioeconomic Consequences and Costs of Mental Illness
The Socioeconomic Consequences and Costs of Mental IllnessThe Socioeconomic Consequences and Costs of Mental Illness
The Socioeconomic Consequences and Costs of Mental IllnessMika Truly
 
Mental Illness Stigma and the Fundamental Components ofSuppo.docx
Mental Illness Stigma and the Fundamental Components ofSuppo.docxMental Illness Stigma and the Fundamental Components ofSuppo.docx
Mental Illness Stigma and the Fundamental Components ofSuppo.docxandreecapon
 
Running head Examining the reasons 1Examining the reasons 16.docx
Running head Examining the reasons 1Examining the reasons 16.docxRunning head Examining the reasons 1Examining the reasons 16.docx
Running head Examining the reasons 1Examining the reasons 16.docxcharisellington63520
 
Student Name Annotated Bibliography Bares, D.S., T.docx
Student Name  Annotated Bibliography  Bares, D.S., T.docxStudent Name  Annotated Bibliography  Bares, D.S., T.docx
Student Name Annotated Bibliography Bares, D.S., T.docxemelyvalg9
 

Similar a Lit rev eg (20)

International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
 
Junxian KuangLaura SinaiENG099101572018In the essay O.docx
Junxian KuangLaura SinaiENG099101572018In the essay O.docxJunxian KuangLaura SinaiENG099101572018In the essay O.docx
Junxian KuangLaura SinaiENG099101572018In the essay O.docx
 
Lesson 11 Mental Health StigmaReadings Please note that th.docx
Lesson 11  Mental Health StigmaReadings  Please note that th.docxLesson 11  Mental Health StigmaReadings  Please note that th.docx
Lesson 11 Mental Health StigmaReadings Please note that th.docx
 
Social psychology
Social psychologySocial psychology
Social psychology
 
For your first reflection please watch the following video and res.docx
For your first reflection please watch the following video and res.docxFor your first reflection please watch the following video and res.docx
For your first reflection please watch the following video and res.docx
 
Honors Symposium Paper
Honors Symposium PaperHonors Symposium Paper
Honors Symposium Paper
 
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of Disabled
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of DisabledPeer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of Disabled
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of Disabled
 
Concepts of stigma and its relevance in Mental Health
Concepts of stigma and its relevance in Mental HealthConcepts of stigma and its relevance in Mental Health
Concepts of stigma and its relevance in Mental Health
 
IMPLICATIONS FOR TRAUMA-INFORMED CARE12Implications for Trauma-In
IMPLICATIONS FOR TRAUMA-INFORMED CARE12Implications for Trauma-InIMPLICATIONS FOR TRAUMA-INFORMED CARE12Implications for Trauma-In
IMPLICATIONS FOR TRAUMA-INFORMED CARE12Implications for Trauma-In
 
ARTICLE REVIEW10Why do people discriminate a.docx
ARTICLE REVIEW10Why do people discriminate a.docxARTICLE REVIEW10Why do people discriminate a.docx
ARTICLE REVIEW10Why do people discriminate a.docx
 
0c96052be437ddbf1f000000(2)
0c96052be437ddbf1f000000(2)0c96052be437ddbf1f000000(2)
0c96052be437ddbf1f000000(2)
 
Article 2 Direct And Indirect Intergroup Friendship(Real I Hope)
Article 2 Direct And Indirect Intergroup Friendship(Real I Hope)Article 2 Direct And Indirect Intergroup Friendship(Real I Hope)
Article 2 Direct And Indirect Intergroup Friendship(Real I Hope)
 
M7 A2 Domestic Violence
M7 A2 Domestic ViolenceM7 A2 Domestic Violence
M7 A2 Domestic Violence
 
Literature ReviewAshley AshleyColumbia Southern .docx
Literature ReviewAshley AshleyColumbia Southern .docxLiterature ReviewAshley AshleyColumbia Southern .docx
Literature ReviewAshley AshleyColumbia Southern .docx
 
ANGELO.Psychosis characteristics (thesis)May25th2015 (final version) (1)
ANGELO.Psychosis characteristics (thesis)May25th2015 (final version) (1)ANGELO.Psychosis characteristics (thesis)May25th2015 (final version) (1)
ANGELO.Psychosis characteristics (thesis)May25th2015 (final version) (1)
 
Criminal behavior among the homeless individuals
Criminal behavior among the homeless individualsCriminal behavior among the homeless individuals
Criminal behavior among the homeless individuals
 
The Socioeconomic Consequences and Costs of Mental Illness
The Socioeconomic Consequences and Costs of Mental IllnessThe Socioeconomic Consequences and Costs of Mental Illness
The Socioeconomic Consequences and Costs of Mental Illness
 
Mental Illness Stigma and the Fundamental Components ofSuppo.docx
Mental Illness Stigma and the Fundamental Components ofSuppo.docxMental Illness Stigma and the Fundamental Components ofSuppo.docx
Mental Illness Stigma and the Fundamental Components ofSuppo.docx
 
Running head Examining the reasons 1Examining the reasons 16.docx
Running head Examining the reasons 1Examining the reasons 16.docxRunning head Examining the reasons 1Examining the reasons 16.docx
Running head Examining the reasons 1Examining the reasons 16.docx
 
Student Name Annotated Bibliography Bares, D.S., T.docx
Student Name  Annotated Bibliography  Bares, D.S., T.docxStudent Name  Annotated Bibliography  Bares, D.S., T.docx
Student Name Annotated Bibliography Bares, D.S., T.docx
 

Lit rev eg

  • 1. Writing a Short Literature Review William Ashton, Ph.D. York College, CUNY A student began a short literature review on the stigma of the mentally ill and perceptions of dangerousness. Working through PsychArticles she found three likely articles. When she read each, she wrote a paragraph description of each: Alexander, L.A., & Link, B.G. (2003). The impact of contact on stigmatizing attitudes towards people with mental illness. Journal of Mental Health, 12, 271-289. Alexander and Link (2003) examined the stigma of mental illness, perceptions of dangerousness and social distance in a telephone survey. They found that, as a participant’s own life contact with mentally ill individuals increased, participants were both less likely to perceive a target mentally ill individual in a vignette as physically dangerous and less likely to desire social distance from the target. This relationship remained after controlling for demographic and confound variables, such as gender, ethnicity, education, income and political conservatism. They also found that any type of contact – with a friend, a spouse, a family member, a work contact, or a contact in a public place – with mentally ill individuals reduced perceptions of dangerousness of the target in the vignette. Corrigan, P. W., Rowan, D., Green, A., Lundin, R., River, P., Uphoff-Wasowski, K., White, K., & Kubiak, M.A. (2002). Challenging two mental illness stigmas: Personality responsibility and dangerousness. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 28, 293-309. Corrigan, Rowan, Green, Lundin, River, Uphoff-Wasowski, White and Kubiak (2002) conducted two studies to investigate the strength of the theoretical relationship between stigma and personality responsibility, and stigma and dangerousness. Corrigan et al. posited two models to account for stigmatizing reactions. In the first model, labeled personal responsibility, personality responsibility influences both the level of pity and anger displayed toward mental patients. Additionally, the variables of pity and anger influence helping behavior. In the second model, labeled dangerousness, perceived dangerousness influences fear of mental patients, which in turn influences the avoidance of the mentally ill. In their first study, Corrigan etal. (2002), administered a questionnaire to 216 community college students. This questionnaire contained items which would allow the examination of the two models. The results of a path analysis indicated that while both models fit the data, the results for the dangerousness
  • 2. model seemed far more consistent with the data. Their second study was an attempt to manipulate variables in the models. Participants met with either an educational group or a mental patient. During the meetings, either myths about the personality responsibility or the dangerousness of mental patients were discussed and debunked. While education yielded some positive results, contact with mental patients produced stronger results. Martin, J. K., Pescosolido, B. A., & Tuch, S. A. (2000). Of fear and loathing: The role of ‘disturbing behavior’ labels, and causal attributions in shaping public attitudes toward people with mental illness. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 41, 208-223. Martin, Pescosolido & Tuch (2000) examined the effects of descriptions of the targets’ behavior, causal attributions about the source of the behavior, the target’s perceived dangerousness, labeling and participants’ sociodemographic characteristics. Twenty percent of the participants labeled a target described with depressed symptoms as having a mental illness (as compared with 54% for those described with schizophrenic symptoms or 1% with normal troubles); 37% would be unwilling to interact with the depressed person (48% for the schizophrenic and 21% for normal troubles); and 33% felt that the depressed person would do violence to others (61% for the schizophrenic and 17% for the normal troubles). Next she decided upon the order of the paragraphs in the paper: Martin, Pescosolido & Tuch (2000) examined the effects of descriptions of the targets’ behavior, causal attributions about the source of the behavior, the target’s perceived dangerousness, labeling and participants’ sociodemographic characteristics. Twenty percent of the participants labeled a target described with depressed symptoms as having a mental illness (as compared with 54% for those described with schizophrenic symptoms or 1% with normal troubles); 37% would be unwilling to interact with the depressed person (48% for the schizophrenic and 21% for normal troubles); and 33% felt that the depressed person would do violence to others (61% for the schizophrenic and 17% for the normal troubles).
  • 3. Alexander and Link (2003) examined the stigma of mental illness, perceptions of dangerousness and social distance in a telephone survey. They found that, as a participant’s own life contact with mentally ill individuals increased, participants were both less likely to perceive a target mentally ill individual in a vignette as physically dangerous and less likely to desire social distance from the target. This relationship remained after controlling for demographic and confound variables, such as gender, ethnicity, education, income and political conservatism. They also found that any type of contact – with a friend, a spouse, a family member, a work contact, or a contact in a public place – with mentally ill individuals reduced perceptions of dangerousness of the target in the vignette. Corrigan, Rowan, Green, Lundin, River, Uphoff-Wasowski, White and Kubiak (2002) conducted two studies to investigate the strength of the theoretical relationship between stigma and personality responsibility, and stigma and dangerousness. Corrigan et al. posited two models to account for stigmatizing reactions. In the first model, labeled personal responsibility, personality responsibility influences both the level of pity and anger displayed toward mental patients. Additionally, the variables of pity and anger influence helping behavior. In the second model, labeled dangerousness, perceived dangerousness influences fear of mental patients, which in turn influences the avoidance of the mentally ill. In their first study, Corrigan etal. (2002), administered a questionnaire to 216 community college students. This questionnaire contained items which
  • 4. would allow the examination of the two models. The results of a path analysis indicated that while both models fit the data, the results for the dangerousness model seemed far more consistent with the data. Their second study was an attempt to manipulate variables in the models. Participants met with either an educational group or a mental patient. During the meetings, either myths about the personality responsibility or the dangerousness of mental patients were discussed and debunked. While education yielded some positive results, contact with mental patients produced stronger results. She carefully chose the order of the paragraphs so she could talk about: (1) that people respond to the mentally ill with fear and rejection, (2) contact reduces both rejection and fear and (3) how to best arrange the contact to reduce stigma. Now she added introductory and concluding sentences, paragraph hooks and short transition paragraphs to help the flow of ideas. Regarding the mentally ill, it appears that people respond to the mentally ill with feelings of fear and rejection. Martin, Pescosolido & Tuch (2000) examined the effects of descriptions of the targets’ behavior, causal attributions about the source of the behavior, the target’s perceived dangerousness, labeling and participants’ sociodemographic characteristics. Twenty percent of the participants labeled a target described with depressed symptoms as having a mental illness (as compared with 54% for those described with schizophrenic symptoms or 1% with normal troubles); 37% would be unwilling to interact with the depressed person (48% for the schizophrenic and 21% for normal troubles); and 33% felt that the depressed person would do violence to others (61% for the
  • 5. schizophrenic and 17% for the normal troubles). Thus, a common respond to the mentally ill are rejection and fear of violence. While, based upon research, the common response to a mentally ill person is to fear violence, diagnosed mental patients commit violence at the same rates as non-diagnosed people (Martin, et al., 2000). Public perceptions may not match reality due to the public’s lack of contact with the mentally ill. Alexander and Link (2003) examined contact with the mentally ill and the stigma of mental illness, perceptions of dangerousness and social distance in a telephone survey. They found that, as a participant’s own life contact with mentally ill individuals increased, participants were both less likely to perceive a target mentally ill individual in a vignette as physically dangerous and less likely to desire social distance from the target. This relationship remained after controlling for demographic and confound variables, such as gender, ethnicity, education, income and political conservatism. They also found that any type of contact – with a friend, a spouse, a family member, a work contact, or a contact in a public place – with mentally ill individuals reduced perceptions of dangerousness of the target in the vignette. Thus, according to Alexander and Link (2003), any contact with the mentally ill is associated with reduced fear and rejection. However, since this study was observational in nature, we cannot know if contact reduces fear or having lower fear increased contact. Corrigan, Rowan, Green, Lundin, River, Uphoff-Wasowski, White and Kubiak (2002) conducted two studies examining the causal processes in contact, fear and rejection. Corrigan et al. posited two models to account for stigmatizing
  • 6. reactions. In the first model, labeled personal responsibility, beliefs about personality responsibility influences both the level of pity and anger displayed toward mental patients. Additionally, the variables of pity and anger influence helping behavior. In the second model, labeled dangerousness, perceived dangerousness influences fear of mental patients, which in turn influences the avoidance of the mentally ill. In their first study, Corrigan et al. (2002) administered a questionnaire to 216 community college students. This questionnaire contained items which would allow the examination of the two models. The results of a path analysis indicated that while both models fit the data, the results for the dangerousness model seemed far more consistent with the data. Their second study was an attempt to manipulate variables in the models. Participants met with either an educational group or with a mental patient. During the meetings, myths about the personality responsibility or the dangerousness of mental patients were discussed and debunked. While education yielded some positive results regarding fear and rejection, contact with mental patients produced stronger results. Thus, Corrigan et al. demonstrated that contact causes less rejection and fear. In the final stage, she needs to write an introductory and concluding paragraph. She wrote the concluding paragraph first. In this paragraph she needs to overview the paper and make a conclusion. It appears that the mentally ill are rejected because of the public’s fear of the mentally ill. At least one-third of the people sampled in one study said that they would both reject socially and fear violence from someone displaying behaviors
  • 7. associated with different mentally illnesses. Other research discovered that this rejection is associated to lack of contact with the mentally ill and that as contact increased, fear of the mentally ill decreased. The direction of the relationship between fear and rejection seems to be that fear (possibly based upon myths about mental illness) causes rejection. Taken as a whole, it appears that exposing these myths as myths increases the acceptance of the mentally ill and that staged contact with a mentally person to expose myths has an even more powerful effect. Now she needs to say something about the research methods. Caution must be advised, though; Martin et al.’s (2002) and Alexander and Link’s (2003) studies and the first study of Corrigan et al. (2002) were based upon paper and pencil methodologies. And while Corrigan et al.’s (2002) second study involved staged presentations, it was conducted in a college setting with a college sample. Future research should replicate these findings in more natural settings with different populations. The student then brought her draft to me. After I read it I asked her about the first sentence of her conclusion. I asked her if she could phrase it as a clear and strong statement. She did: The rejection of the mentally ill is caused by the public’s belief in myths about the dangerousness of the mentally ill and exposing those myths can reduce rejection. Now she needs to write the Introduction. With an introduction, begin broad and narrow down to the thesis statement. The thesis statement is the last sentence in the introduction and the first sentence in the conclusion.
  • 8. The mentally ill face a multitude of challenges. One of those challenges is the stigmatization they face. Stigmatization is social rejection; they are rejected by people because of the label they carry or that their behaviors clearly indicate that they belong to a certain labeled group. Stigmatization of the mentally ill is caused by the public’s belief in myths about the dangerousness of the mentally ill and exposing those myths can reduce stigmatization. Here’s how her whole paper looked:
  • 9. Myths of violence 1 Myths of violence and the stigma of mental illness12 Suzie Student York College, CUNY 1For the text of an APA article, use Courier or New Times Roman font at 12 pts. 2Before the page number is the Page Header (sometimes called, Key Words) the first few words of your title.
  • 10. Myths of violence 2 Myths of violence and the stigma of mental illness3 The mentally ill face a multitude of challenges. One of those challenges is the stigmatization they face. Stigmatization is social rejection; those stigmatized are rejected by people because of the label they carry or that their behaviors clearly indicate that they belong to a certain labeled group. Stigmatization of the mentally ill is caused by the public’s belief in myths about the dangerousness of the mentally ill and exposing those myths can reduce stigmatization. Regarding the mentally ill, it appears that people respond to the mentally ill with feelings of fear and rejection. Martin, Pescosolido & Tuch (2000)4 examined the effects of descriptions of the targets’ behavior, causal attributions about the source of the behavior, the target’s perceived dangerousness, labeling and participants’ sociodemographic characteristics. Twenty percent of the participants labeled a target described with depressed symptoms as having a mental illness (as compared with 54% for those described with schizophrenic symptoms or 1% with normal troubles); 37% would be unwilling to interact with the depressed person (48% for the schizophrenic and 21% for normal troubles); and 33% felt that the depressed person would do violence to others (61% for the 3 before the text begins, repeat the title, centered. 4 the first time you cite an article, list all of the authors’ names.
  • 11. Myths of violence 3 schizophrenic and 17% for the normal troubles). Thus, a common respond to the mentally ill are rejection and fear of violence. While, based upon research, the common response to a mentally ill person is to fear violence, diagnosed mental patients commit violence at the same rates as non-diagnosed people (Martin, et al., 2000)5. Public perceptions may not match reality due to the public’s lack of contact with the mentally ill. Alexander and Link (2003) examined contact with the mentally ill and the stigma of mental illness, perceptions of dangerousness and social distance in a telephone survey. They found that, as a participant’s own life contact with mentally ill individuals increased, participants were both less likely to perceive a target mentally ill individual in a vignette as physically dangerous and less likely to desire social distance from the target. This relationship remained after controlling for demographic and confound variables, such as gender, ethnicity, education, income and political conservatism. They also found that any type of contact – with a friend, a spouse, a family member, a work contact, or a contact in a public place – with mentally ill individuals reduced perceptions of dangerousness of the target in the vignette. Thus, according to 5the second (etc) times you cite an article, you can use et al. if the article has more than 2 authors
  • 12. Myths of violence 4 Alexander and Link (2003), any contact with the mentally ill is associated with reduced fear and rejection. However, since this study was observational in nature, we cannot know if contact reduces fear or having lower fear increased contact. Corrigan, Rowan, Green, Lundin, River, Uphoff-Wasowski, White and Kubiak (2002) conducted two studies examining the causal processes in contact, fear and rejection. Corrigan et al. posited two models to account for stigmatizing reactions. In the first model, labeled personal responsibility, beliefs about personality responsibility influences both the level of pity and anger displayed toward mental patients. Additionally, the variables of pity and anger influence helping behavior. In the second model, labeled dangerousness, perceived dangerousness influences fear of mental patients, which in turn influences the avoidance of the mentally ill. In their first study, Corrigan et al. (2002) administered a questionnaire to 216 community college students. This questionnaire contained items which would allow the examination of the two models. The results of a path analysis indicated that while both models fit the data, the results for the dangerousness model seemed far more consistent with the data. Their second study was an attempt to manipulate variables in the models. Participants met with either an educational group or with a mental patient. During the meetings, myths about the
  • 13. Myths of violence 5 personality responsibility or the dangerousness of mental patients were discussed and debunked. While education yielded some positive results regarding fear and rejection, contact with mental patients produced stronger results. Thus, Corrigan et al. demonstrated that contact causes less rejection and fear. Stigmatization of the mentally ill is caused by the public’s belief in myths about the dangerousness of the mentally ill and exposing those myths can reduce stigmatization. At least one-third of the people sampled in one study said that they would both reject socially and fear violence from someone displaying behaviors associated with different mentally illnesses. Other research discovered that this rejection is associated to lack of contact with the mentally ill and that as contact increased, fear of the mentally ill decreased. The direction of the relationship between fear and rejection seems to be that fear (possibly based upon myths about mental illness) causes rejection. Taken as a whole, it appears that exposing these myths as myths increases the acceptance of the mentally ill and that staged contact with a mentally person to expose myths has an even more powerful effect. Caution must be advised, though; Martin et al.’s (2002) and Alexander and Link’s (2003) studies and the first study of Corrigan et al. (2002) were based upon paper and pencil methodologies. And while Corrigan et al.’s (2002) second study involved staged
  • 14. Myths of violence 6 presentations, it was conducted in a college setting with a college sample. Future research should replicate these findings in more natural settings with different populations.
  • 15. Myths of violence 7 References6 Alexander, L.A., & Link, B.G. (2003). The impact of contact on stigmatizing attitudes towards people with mental illness. Journal of Mental Health, 12, 271-289. Corrigan, P. W., Rowan, D., Green, A., Lundin, R., River, P., Uphoff-Wasowski, K., White, K., & Kubiak, M.A. (2002). Challenging two mental illness stigmas: Personality responsibility and dangerousness. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 28, 293-309. Martin, J. K., Pescosolido, B. A., & Tuch, S. A. (2000). Of fear and loathing: The role of ‘disturbing behavior’ labels, and causal attributions in shaping public attitudes toward people with mental illness. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 41, 208-223. 6 references begin on a new page.