SlideShare una empresa de Scribd logo
1 de 20
Descargar para leer sin conexión
132 Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005) et al.
Child                            Dombrowski
Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/car.888



  Stefan C.                            A Positive
  Dombrowski*
  Rider University
  Lawrenceville, USA
                                       Behavioural
  Susan G. Timmer                      Intervention for
  Dawn M. Blacker
  Anthony J. Urquiza
                                       Toddlers: Parent–
  University of California
  Davis Children’s Hospital
  Sacramento, USA
                                       Child Attunement
                                       Therapy
                                       Parent–child attunement therapy (PCAT) is a promising intervention for
                                       toddlers (aged 12–30 months) who have experienced maltreatment.
                                       PCAT has two overall purposes: (1) to strengthen caregivers’
                                       relationship with their children; and (2) to facilitate caregivers’ learning
                                       of appropriate child management techniques. PCAT represents an
                                       adaptation of parent–child interaction therapy (PCIT), which has
                                       been empirically documented in preschool and early elementary
                                       schoolchildren to improve behavioural adjustment and engender a
  ‘A noted paucity                     stronger bond between caregiver and child. There is, however, a noted
                                       paucity of intervention research for toddlers, specifically maltreated
  of intervention                      toddlers. As toddlerhood represents a critical period for enhancing the
  research for                         relationship between caregivers and children and is a stage when
                                       youngsters are at increased risk for maltreatment, the objectives of
  toddlers,                            PCAT become even more salient during the toddler years. The purpose
  specifically                          of this study, therefore, is to introduce PCAT and then examine its
                                       effectiveness through a single case study of a 23-month-old maltreated
  maltreated                           toddler and his biological mother. Pre- and post-assessment measures
  toddlers’                            included the Parenting Stress Index, the Dyadic Parent–Child
                                       Interaction Coding System (DPICS), the Achenbach Child Behavior
                                       Checklist (CBCL), the Emotional Availability (EA) Scales and the
                                       Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory (ECBI). The results of this study
                                       demonstrate the effectiveness of PCAT in increasing the number
                                       of positive caregiver–toddler interactions and enhancing the overall
                                       quality of the caregiver–toddler relationship. Practitioners will be able
                                       to use the techniques described in this manuscript to improve the
                                       parent–toddler relationship and ameliorate many commonly
                                       experienced behavioural difficulties found among maltreatment-prone
                                       parent–toddler dyads. Therapeutic progress is easily charted so that




                                       ∗ Correspondence to: Stefan C. Dombrowski, 202 Memorial Hall, Rider University,
                                       Lawrenceville, New Jersey 08648, USA. Tel: 609 895 5448.
                                       E-mail: sdombrowski@rider.edu


Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
                                                                          Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: December 2004
                                                                                          Accepted 7 132–151 (2005)
Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers                                         133


effectiveness may be documented and termination of therapy may be
easily discerned. Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

KEY WORDS: toddler; maltreatment; parent training; behavioural
           intervention




     arent-child attunement therapy (PCAT) is a behaviour-
P    ally oriented intervention for maltreated toddlers (aged
12–30 months) and toddlers experiencing attachment dif-
ficulties. It is adapted from parent–child interaction therapy
(PCIT), an empirically validated parent skills training pro-
gramme that has a solid research basis and uses therapist
coaching of caregivers through a bug-in-the-ear device and a
two-way mirrored window (Eyberg, 1988; Hembree-Kigin and
McNeil, 1994). Like PCIT, PCAT focuses on enhancing the
caregiver–child relationship by improving the toddler’s beha-
viour. This is accomplished by increasing parents’ attention
to children’s positive and appropriate behaviour, decreasing
attention to inappropriate behaviour, and teaching parents to             ‘Teaching parents
follow their children’s lead in play, supporting and elaborat-            to follow their
ing upon it rather than directing it (Dombrowski and Timmer,
2001; Paravicini, 2000; Paravicini et al., 2000). Because the             children’s lead in
goals of PCAT involve consistent attention to their children’s            play, supporting
good behaviour, parents become more accessible and their                  and elaborating
behaviour becomes more predictable. Since inconsistent re-
sponsiveness and a lack of positive interactions characterize
                                                                          upon it rather than
abusive family contexts (Cerezo and D’Ocon, 1999; Wolfe,                  directing it’
1987), we argue that PCAT is well suited for toddlers who
have experienced maltreatment and who might possibly
suffer attachment difficulties. The purpose of this study is to
introduce PCAT through an examination of its effectiveness
with a 23-month-old maltreated toddler and his biological
mother. Although this study offers a useful introduction to
PCAT, the effectiveness of PCAT will require further empir-
ical validation in subsequent research.


Background Information

Toddlerhood is an important stage in the development
of children. This period represents a significant period for
developing attachment security, enhancing the relationship
between caregivers and children and acquiring cognitive
and language skills (Cicchetti and Toth, 1995). It is also
a period in which the attachment relationship emerges and
then is continually negotiated between caregiver and child

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                            Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
134                                        Dombrowski et al.


                                           (Cummings et al., 2000). Emerging out of this process is a new
                                           set of developmental tasks that focus on self-development
                                           (Cole, 1990), emotion regulation (Smetana et al., 1999) and
                                           social representational models (Toth et al., 1997). Research
                                           is also clear in indicating that toddlerhood represents a
                                           stage where children are at increased risk for maltreatment
                                           (Wolfe, 1987, 1999). Child maltreatment during any phase
                                           of development has adverse implications, robbing children
                                           of their fundamental human dignity and placing them at
                                           great developmental disadvantage compared with non-
  ‘During the                              abused children (Shengold, 1989). During the toddler years,
  toddler years,                           maltreatment may interrupt critical developmental processes
                                           that, without intervention, may continue to disrupt later
  maltreatment may                         cognitive, social and emotional functioning (Manly et al.,
  interrupt critical                       2001).
  developmental                               During the toddler stage, neurological development
                                           progresses at an accelerated rate and maltreatment has been
  processes’                               found to perturb the development of specific neurological
                                           structures implicated in behavioural, cognitive and social–
                                           emotional functioning (Lowenthal, 1998; Schore, 2001;
                                           Teicher et al., 2002). For instance, early maltreatment has
                                           been linked to abnormal cerebral cortex and limbic system
                                           development (Glaser, 2000; Teicher et al., 2002). These struc-
                                           tures are associated with regulation of cognition, attention and
                                           emotion (Kalat, 2002). The toddler years are also thought
                                           to be important for the formation of attachment security
                                           (Youngblade and Belsky, 1989) and self-representation
                                           (Cichetti and Toth, 1994). Maltreatment during this stage
                                           of development disrupts these formative processes, contribut-
                                           ing to a host of later cognitive, academic, psychological and
                                           relationship problems that may persist well into adulthood
                                           (Dombrowski, 2003; Dombrowski et al., 2003). Unfortun-
                                           ately, there is a relative paucity of interventions not only for
                                           maltreated toddlers specifically, but for maltreated children
                                           more generally.
                                              There are, however, numerous parent training programmes
                                           targeted at improving parental sensitivity and children’s
                                           attachment security. Bakermans-Kranenburg et al. (2003)
                                           conducted a meta-analysis of 70 parent training programmes
                                           targeted at improving parental sensitivity and children’s attach-
                                           ment security. These authors found evidence for the effective-
                                           ness of parent training programmes, particularly if the training
                                           programmes had a clear-cut behavioural emphasis. Of the
                                           70 programmes evaluated within the meta-analysis, none were
                                           focused on children or toddlers who experienced maltreat-
                                           ment. Thus, there is significant need for intervention research
                                           for those who have been maltreated or who may be at risk for

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                               Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers                                    135


maltreatment. There is an even greater need for intervention
research on maltreated toddlers.
   The purpose of this study is to describe PCAT, an initial
step in filling the intervention gap, and its effectiveness for a
mother–toddler dyad with a history of abuse and domestic
violence. We also discuss some of the supports and barriers
to treatment success. Subsequent research on PCAT will be            ‘Subsequent
necessary to ultimately determine its effectiveness. A discus-       research on PCAT
sion of the tenets of parent–child interaction therapy, the pro-
genitor of PCAT, will be helpful in understanding PCAT, as           will be necessary
both the philosophy and procedures overlap.                          to ultimately
                                                                     determine its
Parent–Child Interaction Therapy (PCIT)
                                                                     effectiveness’

PCIT is a behaviourally oriented, empirically validated
parent training model that has been cited as effective in
increasing positive caregiver–child interactions, enhancing
the parent–child relationship, reducing parenting stress and
decreasing dysfunctional parent–child relationship patterns
(Eyberg and Robinson, 1982). It has also been documented
to be effective in ameliorating those same problems among
maltreated children (Borrego et al., 1999; Urquiza and
McNeil, 1996). PCIT contains two phases, each of which is
preceded by a didactic session. During the didactic session,
the caregiver and child are instructed in the mechanics of the
particular phase and provided with a rationale for practising
concepts. In subsequent treatment sessions, the parent prac-
tises these skills as they play with their children. While parent
and child play together, the parent is coached by their ther-        ‘While parent and
apist. The coaching is conducted from an observation room            child play together,
via a ‘bug-in-the-ear’ receiver that the parent wears behind
the ear. Parents continue to be taught and practise specific          the parent is
communication skills with their children. In the relationship        coached by their
enhancement phase (typically seven to ten sessions), termed          therapist’
child directed interaction (CDI), the primary goal is to create
or strengthen a positive and mutually rewarding relationship
between parents and their children by modifying the way
parents speak to their children. Parents are taught to follow
their children’s lead in play by describing their activities and
reflecting their appropriate verbalizations. They are also taught
to praise their children’s positive behaviour and to ignore (in
this phase of treatment) the children’s negative behaviour.
By the end of the ‘relationship enhancement’ phase, parents
have generally shifted from rarely attending to their children’s
positive behaviour to frequently and consistently praising
appropriate child behaviour. They also shift from trying to

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                       Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
136                                        Dombrowski et al.


                                           control their children’s play to describing their play in a way
                                           that conveys their interest in the child’s activity. In the second
                                           phase, termed parent directed interaction (PDI) (typically
                                           seven to ten sessions following CDI), the primary goal is to
                                           provide effective parenting skills for parents to use in manag-
                                           ing their child’s behaviour. Often, many of the child’s dif-
                                           ficult behaviours decrease by the end of CDI, which makes
                                           the process of training parents to obtain compliance with com-
                                           mands easier (Eisenstadt et al., 1983). In PDI, therapists
                                           continue to focus parents’ attention on their children’s posi-
                                           tive behaviours. However, they also begin training in giving
                                           clear, direct commands and avoiding indirect commands such
                                           as those in the form of a question (e.g. ‘Do you want to clean
                                           up your toys now?’) that imply that the child has the option to
  ‘Once parents                            avoid the unpleasant task. Once parents master giving effec-
  master giving                            tive commands, they learn to give children choices to comply
                                           that may result in time-outs or a removal of privileges. By the
  effective                                end of PDI, the processes of giving commands and gaining
  commands,                                compliance are predictable and safe (Eyberg, 1988). Parents
  they learn to                            are generally able to obtain compliance without giving a time-
                                           out, but if they need to give a time-out, it is a comfortable,
  give children                            predictable and well-practised process for which the parent
  choices to comply’                       has acquired mastery (see Hembree-Kigin and McNeil, 1995,
                                           for a full description of the PCIT programme). PCIT offers
                                           a structured, individualized, behavioural yet manualized
                                           approach to attain this goal.


                                           Parent–Child Attunement Therapy (PCAT)

                                           PCAT uses PCIT techniques that have been altered to be
                                           more developmentally appropriate for children younger than
                                           30 months of age. (A comparative summary of PCAT and
                                           PCIT is provided in Table 1.) Like PCIT, PCAT requires the
                                           collection of pre- and post-intervention comprehensive assess-
                                           ment data that include an evaluation of the caregiver–child
                                           interaction pattern. The assessment of the relationship dyad
                                           serves as baseline data against which intervention progress
                                           is monitored. Next, the caregiver is introduced to the mech-
                                           anics and rationale of PCAT in an initial training session.
                                           Following this didactic session and in all subsequent sessions,
                                           caregivers are taught the tenets of child directed interaction
                                           (i.e. how to follow the toddler’s lead in play), which is the
                                           foundation of the relationship enhancement phase of PCIT.
                                           This includes training on the use of non-directive types of
                                           verbalizations or communication (e.g. appropriately praising,
                                           describing and reflecting toddlers’ behaviour).

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                                Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers                                                  137


Table 1. Comparison of PCAT and PCIT

Similarities                           Unique to PCAT

1.             Didactic training session
2.             Use similar technology (e.g. remote hearing device; two-way mirror)
3.             Data collection
4.             Avoid criticism, commands & threats
5.             Mastery criteria
6.             Limit questions
7.             Daily 10-minute practice assignment
8.             Emphasize non-directive play
1.             Simpler language structure
2.             Reduced session length (30–45 min)
3.             Greater focus on parent enthusiasm
4.             Lack of discipline phase (e.g. time-out)
5.             Flexibility to deal with diaper soiling
6.             Toddler fatigue
7.             Focus on increasing positive touching (e.g. hugs)
8.             Requires developmentally younger toys



   In PCAT, therapists teach and serve as a model to caregivers                      ‘In PCAT,
on how to differentially reinforce children for appropriate                          therapists teach
behaviours (e.g. enthusiastic praise after putting the toys away;
ignore when the toddler displays temper outbursts). As in                            and serve as a
PCIT, this is accomplished through real-time coaching via a                          model to
bug-in-the-ear device and a two-way mirror. During initial ses-                      caregivers’
sions, the therapist asks the caregiver to repeat their words as
closely as possible. Therapists model both the verbal (e.g. ‘I
like how you listen to me’) and non-verbal (e.g. give the toddler
a hug after successful completion of a task) aspects of the ses-
sion. Over the course of treatment, the therapist encourages
the parent to tailor the interaction by giving more general
instructions (e.g. ‘Go ahead and describe what Tim is doing’)
or prompting the parent (‘Tim is really playing gently with
the toys’). In this way, therapists gradually lead the caregivers
towards an understanding of their children and the skills they
need to ensure their children’s psychological well-being.
   Caregivers are taught specific strategies that are aimed at                        ‘Specific strategies
giving the toddler control during play. For instance, parents                        that are aimed at
are taught to describe what the toddler is doing and how to
reflect and imitate the toddler’s own verbalizations or actions                       giving the toddler
(when they are appropriate). Reflection might include the                             control during
caregiver repeating, imitating or elaborating on what the                            play’
toddler has just said or done. Additionally, the caregiver is
taught to use simple, more developmentally appropriate lan-
guage, a high degree of enthusiasm and non-verbal signs of
approval (e.g. ‘Good job’ stated enthusiastically followed by
a hug or hand clapping).
   Caregivers are also coached to limit their use of commands
and questions. These verbalizations are perceived as demand-
ing a response from the toddler, and therefore taking control

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                                       Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
138                                        Dombrowski et al.


                                           of the interaction, rather than following the toddler’s lead in
  ‘Excessive parent                        play. Excessive parent control during playtime is common in
  control during                           abusive mother–child dyads (Bousha and Twentyman, 1984)
                                           and this coercive interaction style may foster resistance to
  playtime is                              parents’ commands (Kochanska and Aksan, 1995). These
  common in abusive                        characteristics detract from efforts to enhance the parent–
  mother–child                             toddler relationship.
                                             Caregivers are taught to control their toddler’s behaviour
  dyads’                                   by ignoring inappropriate behaviour while redirecting beha-
                                           viour towards more appropriate and praiseable activity. For
                                           instance, the caregiver might remove an undesirable toy from
                                           the toddler’s hands and provide the child with a different, but
                                           functionally similar toy when the toddler engages in inappro-
                                           priate behaviour with that toy. Or, for example, the caregiver
                                           might physically redirect a child if the toddler attempts to run
                                           out of the door. In this instance, the toddler may be picked
                                           up, returned to the play location and redirected to another
                                           activity while the caregiver places him/herself in between the
                                           toddler and the door, blocking any further attempts to leave.
  ‘The therapist must                      Throughout all PCAT sessions, the therapist must remain
  remain flexible and                       flexible and make allowances for unexpected events (e.g.
                                           soiling of diapers, extreme crankiness). This might include
  make allowances                          abbreviating the session or cancelling the session altogether.
  for unexpected                           Finally, PCAT sessions are shorter (only 30–45 minutes) than
  events’                                  an average PCIT session (60 minutes), generally less struc-
                                           tured and allow the toddler greater freedom to move around
                                           the room and play on the floor or at the table. The follow-
                                           ing is a brief example of a typical PCAT coaching session
                                           [Caregiver and toddler are sitting on the floor while the
                                           toddler plays with a red truck]:

                                           Therapist: Tell Tim, ‘Tim, you’re driving the truck’.
                                           Caregiver: ‘Tim, you’re driving the truck.’ [Describing appro-
                                           priate behaviour]
                                           Therapist: That was a terrific description of Tim’s behaviour.
                                           You will help him maintain his interest in playing with the toy.
                                           Toddler: Verbalizes ‘red truck’.
                                           Therapist: Now state to Tim, ‘That’s right you’re playing with
                                           a red truck’.
                                           Caregiver: ‘That’s right! You’re playing with a red truck!’
                                           [Reflecting Tim’s verbalizations and describing his behaviour]
                                           Therapist: That was a great reflection of what Tim has just said.
                                           You show interest in what he is doing when you restate what
                                           he says.
                                           Toddler: Want candy. [Knocking the truck over as he says this]
                                           Therapist: Ignore his request for candy and his knocking
                                           over of the truck. Instead, get the other truck and begin to

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                               Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers                                      139


enthusiastically roll it on the floor, making an engine sound
as you roll it.
Caregiver: ‘Vroom, Vroom, Vroom.’ [While rolling the truck
on the floor]
[Tim resumes his interest in the toy and forgets about the
candy]
Therapist: Notice how Tim has forgotten about the candy and
resumed playing with the toy. Very good job ignoring his
inappropriate behaviour.
Therapist: Tell Tim that you really like playing with him and
give him a hug.
Caregiver: ‘Tim, I really like playing with you.’ [Hugs Tim]
Therapist: Very good praise. I also really liked how you hugged
Tim. Tim seemed to like it as well as he smiled when you
hugged him. This shows interest in Tim and will help to make
him feel special.

   Throughout each session, caregivers are provided with
instruction regarding simple behavioural reinforcement tech-
niques (e.g. praising and positive attention for appropriate
behaviours). Caregivers are also taught to avoid using state-          ‘Caregivers are
ments that promote coercive or negative interactions (e.g.             also taught to avoid
commands, criticism, threats) as these behaviours are thought
to harm the relationship process and promote a cycle of nega-          using statements
tive interactions. When a toddler engages in behaviour that is         that promote
inappropriate, the caregiver is taught to either ignore that beha-     coercive or negative
viour or redirect the toddler to a more appropriate behaviour.
                                                                       interactions’
Method

Subject
The subject of this study is L, a 23-month-old male toddler,
and his 25-year-old biological mother. The referral for treat-
ment stated that the toddler had been removed at birth from
his parents’ care due to mother’s drug use and father’s un-
willingness and inability to care for L and his three siblings (all
under 4 years of age) on his own. However, when L was born,
his mother was in jail, which also contributed to her inability
to care for the child. It is suspected that L had been exposed
prenatally to drugs since his mother was reported to have used
drugs before going to jail, though not documented. L lived
with one foster family until the time he was reunified with his
biological parents and siblings at 22 months of age. Shortly
after reunification, the toddler was referred to the University
of California, Davis Children’s Hospital for court-mandated
PCAT following concerns about temper tantrums (including

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                         Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
140                                        Dombrowski et al.


                                           throwing objects), aggression towards younger sister and head-
                                           banging when his parents set limits on his behaviour.

                                           Measures
  ‘Five measures                           Five measures were used to evaluate the effectiveness of PCAT
  were used to                             in this case study: the Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist
                                           (CBCL), Dyadic Parent–Child Interaction Coding System
  evaluate the                             (DPICS), Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory (ECBI), the Par-
  effectiveness of                         enting Stress Index—Short Form (PSI-SF) and the Emotional
  PCAT in this case                        Availability Scales (EA).
  study’                                   Dyadic Parent–Child Interaction Coding System (DPICS)
                                           The DPICS is a behavioural coding system designed to assess
                                           the quality of parent–child social interactions through observ-
                                           ation of parent–child dyads (Eyberg and Robinson, 1982). The
                                           DPICS consists of behavioural categories for both children and
                                           caregivers. Although the DPICS was designed to measure 20
                                           different non-verbal and verbal parent–child behaviours and
                                           verbalizations (e.g. whining, laughing), parental verbal beha-
                                           viour was given primary consideration during PCAT coding
                                           sessions. The therapist coded the mothers’ use of praise, de-
                                           scriptions, reflections, questions, commands and critical state-
                                           ments in the first 5 minutes of each treatment session. Half of
                                           these sessions were recoded in order to ensure the reliability
                                           of the therapist’s codes. Intraclass correlation coefficients (a
                                           measure of intercoder reliability) for the coding categories were
                                           the following: descriptions/reflections = 0.76; praises = 0.94;
                                           questions/commands = 0.97.
                                              Consistent with the PCIT coding approach, PCAT coding
                                           focuses on verbalizations that are thought to be important
                                           in enhancing the caregiver–child relationship and reducing
                                           dysfunctional relationship patterns. Specific ‘mastery’ criteria
                                           are established at PCIT levels with respect to descriptive and
                                           reflective statements (25) and praises (15). Question mastery
  ‘Caregiver                               criteria are established at PCIT levels of 3 or less. Caregiver
  commands are                             commands are discouraged during playtime as they serve to
                                           remove control from the child. The following is a more pre-
  discouraged during                       cise operational definition of descriptions, reflections, praises,
  playtime as they                         questions and commands:
  serve to remove                             Descriptions: statements or sentences that describe the child,
                                           the objects with which the child is playing and the activity in
  control from the                         which the child is engaging (e.g. ‘You’re playing with the red
  child’                                   truck’, ‘You’re sitting in your seat’).
                                              Reflections: statements that repeat or rephrase a preceding
                                           toddler verbalization (e.g. toddler states ‘Want car’; caregiver
                                           states ‘You want the car’).

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                               Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers                                       141


  Praise: positive evaluation of attributes or behaviour of the
child. There are generally two types of praise that are coded:
labelled and unlabelled. Labelled praises are very precise,
describing the reason the praise was offered (e.g. ‘Good job
rolling the truck!’). Unlabelled praises are vague and non-
specific (e.g. ‘Great job!’).
  Questions: verbal statements that invite or call for a reply (e.g.
‘Do you want to play with the dolls?’).
  Commands: directions from caregivers to toddler indicating
that a verbal or motor activity should be performed (e.g. ‘Pick
up the truck from the floor’).

Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)
The CBCL (Achenbach, 1991) is a standardized instru-
ment that lists 112 problem child behaviours. This version is
completed by a parent or regular caregiver and describes the
behaviour of children between the ages of 4 and 16 years.
Separate norms are provided for both boys and girls in three            ‘Separate norms
age groups. Normative data are derived from a large sociolog-           are provided for
ically diverse population of both non-referred and clinically
referred children and their parents. The CBCL is composed               both boys and girls
of a total problem score, two broadband scales (internalizing,          in three age
externalizing) and eight narrow-band scales for each age group          groups’
and sex (e.g. withdrawn, somatic complaints, delinquent beha-
viour, aggressive behaviour). The clinical cutoff score for the
broadband scales is a T-score greater than or equal to 65.

Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory (ECBI)
The ECBI is a 36-item scale that measures specific behaviour
problems exhibited by children aged 2–16 years. Parents
indicate the frequency of certain behaviours (intensity score)
and whether they are considered to be problems (problem
score) (Eyberg and Robinson, 1982; Eyberg and Ross, 1978).
The ECBI has been standardized on a number of populations
(Eyberg and Robinson, 1982; Eyberg and Ross, 1978). Test–
retest reliability scores across a 3-week timespan on the ECBI
intensity and problem scales were 0.86 and 0.88 respectively
(Robinson and Eyberg, 1981; Robinson et al., 1980). The
published clinical cutoff scores are an intensity score of greater
than 131 or a problem score of greater than 16.

Parenting Stress Index—Short Form (PSI-SF)
The PSI-SF (Abidin, 1990) is a 36-item inventory designed
to identify parent–child dyads that are experiencing stress and
are at risk for developing dysfunctional parenting and child
behaviour problems. The PSI-SF consists of three subscales:
difficult child, parent distress, and dysfunctional parent–child

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                          Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
142                                        Dombrowski et al.


                                           relationship. The PSI-SF also contains a measure of defensive
                                           responding. Low scores (<15th percentile) on this scale have
                                           been shown to indicate that parents are minimizing problems
                                           associated with the parent role, that they do not have a close
                                           relationship with the children and are therefore unaffected by
                                           their behaviour, or that they are simply not stressed by situa-
                                           tions that stress normal parents. The test–retest and internal
                                           consistency reliability of the PSI on various scales ranges from
                                           0.68 to 0.84 (Abidin, 1990).

                                           Emotional Availability Scales (EA)
  ‘Four global parent                      The EA Scales (Biringen et al., 1998) consist of four global
  scales and two                           parent scales and two child scales that measure specific
                                           dimensions of the caregiver–child relationship. Parent scales
  child scales that                        measure their sensitivity to the child, their non-hostility,
  measure specific                          non-intrusiveness and ability to structure the interaction. Child
  dimensions of the                        scales measure their responsiveness to the parent and the
                                           degree to which they involve the parent in their activities.
  caregiver–child                          Parent sensitivity scores reflect the degree to which the parent
  relationship’                            perceives and responds to the child’s cues, the parent’s engage-
                                           ment and interest in the child’s activity, as well as the affec-
                                           tive quality and conflict management. Parental structuring
                                           scores reflect the parent’s ability to give structure to an inter-
                                           action (i.e. scaffold) so that the child responds positively.
                                           Parent non-intrusiveness refers to the parent’s ability to
                                           give structure to the interaction without over-controlling and
                                           diminishing the child’s autonomy. Parental non-hostility
                                           scores reflect the degree to which the parent’s actions and tone
                                           of voice convey anger, impatience or boredom. Child respons-
                                           iveness refers to the degree to which the child responds to the
                                           parent in a positive, emotionally available manner and the
                                           degree to which the child balances autonomous pursuits and
                                           interest in the parent’s activities. Child involvement measures
                                           the degree to which the child involves the parent in his or her
                                           play, taking into account the balance of child-initiated and
                                           parent-initiated interactions. Higher scores reflect more opti-
                                           mal emotional availability. According to Biringen et al. (1998,
                                           cited in Easterbrooks et al., 2000), parent sensitivity scores
                                           above 4 are in the optimal range. Additionally, non-hostility
                                           scores above 4 and non-intrusiveness and structuring scores
                                           above 3, and child responsiveness and involvement scores
                                           above 4, are in the optimal range.

                                           Procedure
                                           While learning PCAT, caregivers are taught to follow
                                           their child’s lead in play, to praise the child for behaving

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                               Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers                                   143


appropriately and to limit the number of questions and
commands elicited during play. Caregivers are also taught to        ‘Caregivers
ignore inappropriate behaviour and to redirect the child’s          are also taught
behaviour when appropriate. In this study, PCAT was taught
to the mother through an initial child directed interaction         to ignore
(CDI) didactic training session. Prior to the initial didactic      inappropriate
session, before each weekly coaching session and at the end of      behaviour’
treatment, the mother and toddler were coded for 5 minutes
using the DPICS. (The caregiver was instructed to follow the
toddler’s lead in play during which time the trained therapist
coded the parent–child interactions.) Following the 5 minute
DPICS coding, the therapist coached the parent on appropri-
ate parent–child interaction through the use of a one-way
mirror and a bug-in-the-ear device. In this case study, the
mother and the toddler participated in an initial pre-treatment
DPICS coding session followed by nine coaching sessions and
a post-treatment session. During the post-treatment session,
the caregiver was coded using the DPICS and the EA Scales.
The caregiver was asked to complete a battery of standardized
measures (e.g. ECBI, PSI, CBCL) during the initial intake
interview and again after the last PCAT session.


Results

Figure 1 presents the results of DPICS coding. As shown, the
mother’s use of praise, descriptions and reflections significantly    ‘Mother’s use of
increased from pre-treatment to post-treatment. In the first         praise, descriptions
5 minutes of the DPIC session at pre-treatment, the mother
praised her son five times. In contrast, during the first 5 min-      and reflections
utes of the post-treatment DPICS, the mother used 46 praises,       significantly
well beyond the 15-praise mastery threshold. A similar increase     increased from
                                                                    pre-treatment to
                                                                    post-treatment’




Figure 1.   Performance in Weekly Treatment Sessions


Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                      Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
144                                        Dombrowski et al.


                                           Table 2. Measures of child and parent functioning: pre- vs post-treatment

                                           Measures                                     Pre-treatment        Post-treatment

                                           ECBI   (T-scores)
                                                  Intensity score                           55                    65*
                                                  Problem score                             59                    —
                                           CBCL (T-scores)
                                                  Internalizing score                       53                    58
                                                  Externalizing score                       45                    56
                                                  Total problem score                       45                    57
                                           PSI-SF (percentile scores)
                                                  Parent distress                            7.5                   5
                                                  Dysfunctional relationship                50                    45
                                                  Difficult child                            90*                   85*
                                                  Defensive responding                       5*                    5*

                                           * Scores at or above the clinical cutoff.


                                           was reflected in the number of descriptions and reflections
                                           provided by the mother. At pre-treatment, the mother had
                                           12 descriptions/reflections, below the mastery threshold of
                                           25. This increased to 59 descriptions/reflections by post-
                                           treatment, representing another significant gain. The number
                                           of questions the mother asked her child decreased from a pre-
                                           treatment level of 50 to a post-treatment level of two. Overall,
                                           the mother attained mastery on all criteria coded within PCAT.
                                              Table 2 shows the mother’s ratings of her child’s behaviour
                                           at pre-treatment and post-treatment, as well as ratings of
                                           her own functioning. In contrast to scores on observational
                                           measures, the mother rated the child’s problem behaviours as
                                           having increased slightly from pre- to post-treatment on both
                                           the ECBI and the CBCL. In the case of the ECBI intensity
                                           score, a measure of more everyday problem behaviours, the
                                           score moved from below the clinical cutoff into the clinical
                                           range. In contrast, scores on the PSI-SF decreased very
  ‘The mother                              slightly. It is interesting to note that the mother showed elev-
  showed elevated                          ated levels of defensive responding (lower percentile scores
                                           indicate higher defensive responding), perhaps indicating a
  levels of defensive                      desire to present herself and her relationship with her son in
  responding’                              a positive light.
                                              Table 3 shows the results of the EA rating of this dyad. Pre-
                                           treatment scores on parent emotional availability scales are
                                           mixed: some are optimal (non-hostility), some are in the low-
                                           optimal range (sensitivity) and some are non-optimal (struc-
                                           turing, non-intrusiveness). The child’s emotional availability
                                           scores were all non-optimal. The mother’s sensitivity scores
                                           were in the low-optimal range throughout the DPICS session.
                                           Throughout pre-treatment CDI and clean-up, her affect was
                                           positive and consistent and she was engaged in her inter-
                                           action with the child, although the child did not respond to
                                           her overtures with a similar level of enthusiasm. In CDI, the

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                                         Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers                                                145


Table 3. EA results: mean scores and per cent score across DPICS scenarios

                                        Pre-treatment          Post-treatment
                                            scores                 scores

                                      CDI          CU         CDI            CU

Parent scales
Sensitivity (9-pt scale)                6           5           7            7
Non-hostility (5-pt scale)              5           5           5            5
Non-intrusiveness (5-pt scale)          3           2           4            4
Structuring (5-pt scale)                3           3           4            5
Child scales
Responsiveness (7-pt scale)             4           2           6            6
Involvement (7-pt scale)                4           2           6            5

CDI, child directed interaction; CU, clean-up.


mother played with the child for a short time, but the child’s
interest in the play waned and the mother questioned him
about what he wanted to do. The child got down from his chair
and walked around the therapy room and the mother directed
his attention to the toys on the table, asking him to help her
clean them up. She continued to direct his cleaning up until
the therapist switched the activity. During the ‘clean-up,’
L ignored her requests to pick up the toys and tantrumed.
Her inability to support his play, and her over-directing                          ‘Her inability to
of the ‘child-directed’ play, reduced her structuring and                          support his play,
non-intrusiveness scores. The child’s continual need to be
prompted by his mother in order to sustain his engagement                          and her over-
with her and his tantrum during ‘clean-up’ reduced his respon-                     directing of the
siveness and involvement scores.                                                   ‘child-directed’
   At post-treatment, most of the mother and child’s emotional
availability scores improved to the optimal range. The dyad
                                                                                   play’
showed one non-optimal score in parent structuring and child
responsiveness and involvement. During the post-treatment
DPICS, mother and son sat together on the floor playing with
playdough forms. The mother followed the child’s lead and
elaborated his play. The child was highly engaged in play with
his mother, handing her the playdough forms and saying,
‘Here, Mommy!’. At one point, he tried to take a sticker off
a toy, but his mother successfully redirected his attention
elsewhere.
   The child and his mother’s emotional availability scores
showed great improvement from pre- to post-treatment. Over-
all, the mother tried to verbally engage the child pre- and
post-treatment. However, the mother was less directive, more
positive and more creative in her play with the child at post-
treatment than pre-treatment. She also seemed more comfort-
able playing with her son at post-treatment and involved
herself in his play more easily. The mother did not seem as
disturbed by his non-compliance by post-treatment, handling

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                                     Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
146                                        Dombrowski et al.


                                           the ‘active ignore’ with ease, although she did need some as-
  ‘The child, for his                      sistance from the therapist. The child, for his part, appeared
  part, appeared                           more cheerful and talkative. He spent more time interacting
                                           positively with his mother and was able to control his negative
  more cheerful                            emotions quickly (once given the ‘active ignore’ framework).
  and talkative’

                                           Discussion

                                           The results of this single case study suggest that PCAT with
                                           this 23-month-old maltreated toddler was successful, increas-
                                           ing the number of positive caregiver–toddler interactions and
                                           improving the emotional availability of the dyad. The mother
                                           learned how to play with L, responding appropriately to his
                                           cues. In turn, L responded positively to his mother’s praise and
                                           involved her in his play. While the treatment itself may have
                                           contributed to the improved emotional availability scores, it
                                           is also possible that the toddler’s reunification with his biolog-
                                           ical mother may have also played a role in increased EA scores.
                                           In our experience, newly reunified parents lack self-assurance
                                           in their interactions with their children and the children tend
                                           to be less responsive to them. Furthermore, the scores on the
                                           standardized measures suggest that PCAT might not have
                                           been completely successful in shifting the mother’s percep-
                                           tion of her son’s behaviours. However, for several reasons,
  ‘It is difficult to                       it is difficult to take this mother’s evaluation as reflective of
  take this mother’s                       the effectiveness of the treatment. First, the court mandated
                                           PCAT for this mother and her toddler following their re-
  evaluation as                            unification. In our experience, biological parents who are
  reflective of the                         ordered to participate in PCAT by the court often minimize
  effectiveness of                         their children’s problems. This mother’s tendency to minimize
                                           is reflected in her elevated defensive responding score on the
  the treatment’                           PSI-SF. It is also reflected in the fact that the ECBI and CBCL
                                           scores were all in the normal range, although she reported
                                           concerns about temper tantrums (including throwing objects),
                                           aggression towards his younger sister, head-banging and non-
                                           compliance. It is difficult to observe expected treatment-
                                           related reductions in behaviour problems when parents
                                           minimize the child’s problems.
                                              Another obstacle to measurable treatment success in any
                                           treatment involving parent–child dyads is the quality of the
                                           marital relationship, and environmental stressors. Over the
                                           course of treatment, the therapist reported several incidences
                                           where conflict between the parents led to one parent leaving
                                           for a while. Also noted was L’s increased tendency to tantrum
                                           when the parents were not getting along. The parents were
                                           advised of their need to improve their own relationship and

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                               Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers                                    147


referred to marriage counselling in order to provide more sta-
bility for L. High levels of marital conflict can affect parents’     ‘High levels of
ratings of their children’s behaviour in two ways. First, L could    marital conflict
have reacted to the conflict in the household by continuing to
tantrum and act out aggressively. Research has shown that            can affect parents’
children in families with high levels of domestic violence act       ratings of their
out more than children in families with low levels of domestic       children’s
violence (Davies et al., 2002; Jouriles et al., 1989). In fact,
future research studies using behavioural-oriented parent
                                                                     behaviour in
training models might benefit from incorporating a more               two ways’
global assessment of both marital relationship and environ-
mental risk factors (e.g. unemployment, unstable housing,
community violence). Second, because the mother was already
highly stressed by difficulties in her marital relationship post-
PCAT, she could have been more sensitive to any additional
stress caused by L’s negative behaviour than she had been pre-
PCAT. Research in the child abuse literature has shown that
levels of distress increase the chances of physically abusing
children (Milner and Chilamkurti, 1991). Finally, L’s biolog-
ical father was incarcerated about midway through PCAT and
L’s mother reported that she was quite depressed by this event.
It is possible that her elevated levels of depression influenced      ‘It is possible that
her ratings (see Kamphaus and Frick, 2001) such that she             her elevated levels
rated L as being more difficult, even though clinic-based ob-
servations noted improved behavioural performance. Although          of depression
the mother reported more behaviour problems, she also                influenced her
reported to the therapist (recorded in clinical case notes) that     ratings’
the toddler seemed less of a behaviour problem and that her
relationship with him had improved considerably. While clinic-
based observations (e.g. DPICS) and emotional availability
ratings indicated improvement, standardized measures indi-
cated a degree of deterioration, providing equivocal evidence
for therapeutic improvement.
   Overall, the results of this study suggest that PCAT is a
promising therapeutic programme that increases the number
of positive parent–child interactions and may contribute to
enhancing the parent–toddler relationship. At the same time,
we point out the possibility that environmental risks (e.g.
marital conflict) may be barriers or increase the difficulty of
making progress in treatment. In so far as proper parent–
toddler interactions set the stage for more harmonious inter-
actions at later stages of development, a more harmonious
relationship might help steer the parent–child dyad away from
coercive interactional patterns that are common among mal-
treated children and their caregivers even under the most dif-
ficult circumstances (Urquiza and McNeil, 1996). Further, the
formation of a secure parent–child relationship can enhance

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                       Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
148                                        Dombrowski et al.


                                           the toddler’s ability to form future healthy relationships
                                           (Cicchetti and Olsen, 1990; Sroufe and Fleeson, 1986). It
                                           may also buffer the toddler from the onset of later psycho-
                                           pathology and language/cognitive delays that are commonly
                                           associated with maltreated children (Aber and Allen, 1987).
  ‘Treatment does                          These findings remind us that treatment does not take place
  not take place in                        in a vacuum and that external stressors often impinge upon
                                           the magnitude of its effectiveness. In the present case, we ob-
  a vacuum’                                served improvements in parent–child relationship functioning
                                           despite the possibly traumatic effects of concurrent marital
                                           conflict.
                                              This paper represents an important initial step in establish-
                                           ing PCAT as a potentially effective behavioural treatment for
                                           improving toddler–parent relationships and decreasing beha-
                                           viour problems. PCAT is also a short-term, positive beha-
                                           vioural intervention for maltreated toddlers and toddlers who
                                           experience attachment difficulties. We have presented some
                                           of the difficulties of demonstrating treatment effectiveness
                                           in this population. These findings remind us that treatment
  ‘The context of a                        occurs within the context of a larger environment that does
  larger environment                       not always support positive change. However, these same
                                           limitations also show the robustness of the treatment. In spite
  that does not                            of the high stress level of the family environment, we were able
  always support                           to observe positive changes in the quality of the parent–child
  positive change’                         relationship. Future research with a larger sample of toddlers
                                           and their parents that includes follow-up data will more
                                           appropriately determine the effectiveness of PCAT for the
                                           treatment of toddlers and for the enhancement of the parent–
                                           toddler relationship.


                                           References

                                           Aber JL, Allen JP. 1987. Effects of maltreatment on young children’s
                                             socioemotional development: an attachment theory perspective.
                                             Developmental Psychology 23: 406–414.
                                           Abidin RR. 1990. Parenting Stress Index (3rd edn). Pediatric Psychology
                                             Press: Charlottesville, VA.
                                           Achenbach TM. 1991. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18
                                             and 1991 Profile. University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry:
                                             Burlington, VT.
                                           Bakermans-Kranenburg MH, van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F. 2003. Less
                                             is more: meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions
                                             in early childhood. Psychological Bulletin 129: 195–215.
                                           Biringen Z, Robinson J, Emde R. 1998. Emotional Availability Scales
                                             (3rd edn). Unpublished manual, Colorado State University.
                                           Borrego J, Urquiza AJ, Rasmussen RA, Zebell N. 1999. Parent–child
                                             interaction therapy with a family at high risk for physical abuse. Child
                                             Maltreatment 4: 331–342.

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                                   Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers                                           149


Bousha DM, Twentyman CT. 1984. Mother–child interactional style in
  abuse, neglect, and control groups: naturalistic observations in the
  home. Child Development 49: 1163–1173.
Cerezo MA, D’Ocon AD. 1999. Sequential analyses in coercive mother–
  child interaction: the predictability hypothesis in abusive versus
  nonabusive dyads. Child Abuse & Neglect 23: 99–113.
Cicchetti D, Olsen K. 1990. The developmental psychopathology of child
  maltreatment. In Handbook of Developmental Psychopathology, Lewis
  M, Miller MS (eds). Plenum Press: New York; 261–279.
Cicchetti D, Toth SL. 1994. Rochester Symposium on Developmental
  Psychopathology (Vol. 5). University of Rochester Press: Rochester,
  NY.
Cicchetti D, Toth SL. 1995. A developmental psychopathology perspec-
  tive on child abuse and neglect. Journal of the American Academy of
  Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 34: 541–565.
Cole DA. 1990. Relation of social and academic competence to depres-
  sive symptoms in childhood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 99:
  422–429.
Cummings M, Davies PT, Campbell S. 2000. Developmental Psycho-
  pathology & Family Process. Guilford Press: New York.
Davies PT, Forman EM, Rasi JA, Stevens KI. 2002. Assessing children’s
  emotional security in the interparental subsystem: the Security in the
  Interparental Subsystem (SIS) scales. Child Development 73: 544–
  562.
Dombrowski SC. 2003. Mandated reporting for mental health profession-
  als: an overview. Directions in Mental Health Counseling 14: 77–88.
Dombrowski SC, Timmer SG. 2001. Parent–child attunement therapy
  with a 23 month-old maltreated toddler. Paper presented at the second
  annual PCIT conference, Sacramento, CA.
Dombrowski SC, Ahia CE, McQuillan K. 2003. Protecting children
  through mandated child abuse reporting. The Educational Forum
  67: 76–85.
Easterbrooks A, Biesecker G, Lyons-Ruth K. 2000. Infancy predictors
  of emotional availability in middle childhood: the roles of attachment
  security and maternal depressive symptomatology. Attachment &
  Human Development 2: 170–187.
Eisenstadt TH, Eyberg SM, McNeil CB, Newcomb K, Funderburk BW.
  1983. Parent–child interaction therapy with behavior problem children:
  relative effectiveness of two stages and overall treatment outcomes.
  Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 22: 42–51.
Eyberg SM. 1988. PCIT: integration of traditional and behavioral con-
  cerns. Child and Family Behavior Therapy 10: 33–46.
Eyberg SM, Robinson EA. 1982. Parent–child interaction training: effects
  on family functioning. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 11: 130–
  137.
Eyberg SM, Ross AW. 1978. Assessment of child behavior problems: the
  validation of a new inventory. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 7:
  113–116.
Glaser D. 2000. Child abuse and neglect and the brain—a review. Jour-
  nal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines 41: 97–116.
Hembree-Kigin TL, McNeil CB. 1995. Parent–Child Interaction Therapy.
  Plenum: New York.
Jouriles E, Murphy C, O’Leary KD. 1989. Interspousal aggression,
  marital discord, and child problems. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
  Psychology 57: 453–455.


Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                              Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
150                                        Dombrowski et al.


                                           Kalat JW. 2002. Biological Psychology (7th edn). Wadsworth/Thomson
                                             Learning: Belmont, CA.
                                           Kamphaus RW, Frick PJ. 2001. Clinical Assessment of Child and
                                             Adolescent Personality and Behavior (2nd edn). Allyn & Bacon:
                                             Needham Heights, MA.
                                           Kochanska G, Aksan N. 1995. Mother–child mutually positive affect, the
                                             quality of child compliance to requests and prohibitions, and maternal
                                             control as correlates of early internalization. Child Development 66:
                                             236–254.
                                           Lowenthal B. 1998. The effects of early childhood abuse and the
                                             development of resiliency. Early Child Development & Care 142: 43–
                                             52.
                                           Manly JT, Jungmeen E, Rogosch FA, Cicchetti D. 2001. Dimensions
                                             of child maltreatment and children’s adjustment: contributions of
                                             developmental timing and subtype. Development & Psychopathology
                                             13: 759–782.
                                           Milner JS, Chilamkurti C. 1991. Physical child abuse perpetrator charac-
                                             teristics: a review of the literature. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 6:
                                             345–366.
                                           Paravicini SF. 2000. Parent–child attunement therapy: development of
                                             a program for children one to three years old. Dissertation Abstracts
                                             International 60(9-B) (UMI No. AAI9945876).
                                           Paravicini SF, Urquiza AJ, Blacker DM. 2000. Parent–child attunement
                                             therapy: development of a program for children one to three years old.
                                             Paper presented at the first annual PCIT conference, Sacramento, CA.
                                           Robinson EA, Eyberg SM. 1981. The dyadic parent–child interaction
                                             coding system: standardization and validation. Journal of Consulting
                                             and Clinical Psychology 49: 245–250.
                                           Robinson EA, Eyberg SM, Ross WA. 1980. The standardization of an
                                             inventory of child conduct problem behaviors. Journal of Clinical Child
                                             Psychology 9: 22–29.
                                           Schore AN. 2001. Effects of a secure attachment relationship on right
                                             brain development, affect regulation, and infant mental health. Infant
                                             Mental Health Journal 22: 7–66.
                                           Shengold L. 1989. Soul Murder: The Effects of Childhood Abuse and
                                             Deprivation. Yale University Press: New Haven, CT.
                                           Smetana J, Daddis C, Toth SL, Cicchetti D, Bruce J, Kane P. 1999.
                                             Effects of provocation on maltreated and non-maltreated preschoolers’
                                             understanding of moral transgressions. Social Development 8: 335–
                                             348.
                                           Sroufe LA, Fleeson J. 1986. Attachment and the construction of relation-
                                             ships. In Relationships and Development, Hartup WW, Rubin Z (eds).
                                             Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ; 51–71.
                                           Teicher MH, Andersen SL, Polcari A, Anderson CM, Navalta CP. 2002.
                                             Developmental neurobiology of childhood stress and trauma. Psy-
                                             chiatric Clinics of North America 25: 397–426.
                                           Toth SL, Cicchetti D, Macfie J, Emde RN. 1997. Representations of self
                                             and other in the narratives of neglected, physically abused, and
                                             sexually abused preschoolers. Development & Psychopathology 9:
                                             781–796.
                                           Urquiza AJ, McNeil CB. 1996. Parent–child interaction therapy: an
                                             intensive dyadic intervention for physically abusive families. Child
                                             Maltreatment 1: 134–144.
                                           Wolfe DA. 1987. Child-abusive parents: an empirical review and ana-
                                             lysis. Psychological Bulletin 97: 462–482.


Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                                    Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers                                      151


Wolfe DA. 1999. Child Abuse: Implication for Child Development and
  Psychopathology. Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA.
Youngblade LM, Belsky J. 1989. Child maltreatment, infant–parent
  attachment security, and dysfunctional peer relationships in
  toddlerhood. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 9: 1–15.




Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.                         Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)

Más contenido relacionado

La actualidad más candente

Play Therapy research paper
Play Therapy research paperPlay Therapy research paper
Play Therapy research paperJenice Crutcher
 
Parental Military Deployment and Early Child Development 2012
Parental Military Deployment and Early Child Development 2012Parental Military Deployment and Early Child Development 2012
Parental Military Deployment and Early Child Development 2012Jacquie Pinkerton
 
Growth and development in children
Growth and development in childrenGrowth and development in children
Growth and development in childrenbirhanu abie
 
Trauma Safe Schools - Trauma safe education a neurocognitive approach to tea...
Trauma Safe Schools - Trauma safe education a neurocognitive approach to  tea...Trauma Safe Schools - Trauma safe education a neurocognitive approach to  tea...
Trauma Safe Schools - Trauma safe education a neurocognitive approach to tea...Michael Changaris
 
Dweck (1975) Attributions
Dweck (1975) AttributionsDweck (1975) Attributions
Dweck (1975) AttributionsFRANCOIS JUNG
 
Early childhood special education
Early childhood special educationEarly childhood special education
Early childhood special educationchildhgd20
 
Genetic Influences on Parental Expressed Emotion. A Novel Approach to the Nat...
Genetic Influences on Parental Expressed Emotion. A Novel Approach to the Nat...Genetic Influences on Parental Expressed Emotion. A Novel Approach to the Nat...
Genetic Influences on Parental Expressed Emotion. A Novel Approach to the Nat...Florence Walsh
 
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...worldwideww
 
Children's longing for everydayness after tbi
Children's longing for everydayness after tbiChildren's longing for everydayness after tbi
Children's longing for everydayness after tbiRichard Radecki
 
A study of home environment and reasoning ability among secondary school ...
A study of   home   environment and reasoning ability among secondary school ...A study of   home   environment and reasoning ability among secondary school ...
A study of home environment and reasoning ability among secondary school ...Alexander Decker
 
11.a study of home environment and reasoning ability among secondary scho...
11.a study of   home   environment and reasoning ability among secondary scho...11.a study of   home   environment and reasoning ability among secondary scho...
11.a study of home environment and reasoning ability among secondary scho...Alexander Decker
 
A Qualitative Case Study: Fathers' Experiences Massaging Their Infants.
A Qualitative Case Study: Fathers' Experiences Massaging Their Infants. A Qualitative Case Study: Fathers' Experiences Massaging Their Infants.
A Qualitative Case Study: Fathers' Experiences Massaging Their Infants. Mary Kay Keller, MPA, PhD
 
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...inventionjournals
 
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And PeersAdolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And Peersgaz12000
 
Children transition to mainstream education
Children transition to mainstream educationChildren transition to mainstream education
Children transition to mainstream educationfestus mwangi
 
Rose, Nelson & Hardiman 2016
Rose, Nelson & Hardiman 2016Rose, Nelson & Hardiman 2016
Rose, Nelson & Hardiman 2016Becky Hardiman
 

La actualidad más candente (19)

Play Therapy research paper
Play Therapy research paperPlay Therapy research paper
Play Therapy research paper
 
Parental Military Deployment and Early Child Development 2012
Parental Military Deployment and Early Child Development 2012Parental Military Deployment and Early Child Development 2012
Parental Military Deployment and Early Child Development 2012
 
Growth and development in children
Growth and development in childrenGrowth and development in children
Growth and development in children
 
Trauma Safe Schools - Trauma safe education a neurocognitive approach to tea...
Trauma Safe Schools - Trauma safe education a neurocognitive approach to  tea...Trauma Safe Schools - Trauma safe education a neurocognitive approach to  tea...
Trauma Safe Schools - Trauma safe education a neurocognitive approach to tea...
 
Dweck (1975) Attributions
Dweck (1975) AttributionsDweck (1975) Attributions
Dweck (1975) Attributions
 
Fpsyg 06-01210
Fpsyg 06-01210Fpsyg 06-01210
Fpsyg 06-01210
 
Early childhood special education
Early childhood special educationEarly childhood special education
Early childhood special education
 
Genetic Influences on Parental Expressed Emotion. A Novel Approach to the Nat...
Genetic Influences on Parental Expressed Emotion. A Novel Approach to the Nat...Genetic Influences on Parental Expressed Emotion. A Novel Approach to the Nat...
Genetic Influences on Parental Expressed Emotion. A Novel Approach to the Nat...
 
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
 
Children's longing for everydayness after tbi
Children's longing for everydayness after tbiChildren's longing for everydayness after tbi
Children's longing for everydayness after tbi
 
Gay1
Gay1Gay1
Gay1
 
A study of home environment and reasoning ability among secondary school ...
A study of   home   environment and reasoning ability among secondary school ...A study of   home   environment and reasoning ability among secondary school ...
A study of home environment and reasoning ability among secondary school ...
 
11.a study of home environment and reasoning ability among secondary scho...
11.a study of   home   environment and reasoning ability among secondary scho...11.a study of   home   environment and reasoning ability among secondary scho...
11.a study of home environment and reasoning ability among secondary scho...
 
A Qualitative Case Study: Fathers' Experiences Massaging Their Infants.
A Qualitative Case Study: Fathers' Experiences Massaging Their Infants. A Qualitative Case Study: Fathers' Experiences Massaging Their Infants.
A Qualitative Case Study: Fathers' Experiences Massaging Their Infants.
 
Journal of Behavioral and Social Sciences
Journal of Behavioral and Social SciencesJournal of Behavioral and Social Sciences
Journal of Behavioral and Social Sciences
 
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
 
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And PeersAdolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
 
Children transition to mainstream education
Children transition to mainstream educationChildren transition to mainstream education
Children transition to mainstream education
 
Rose, Nelson & Hardiman 2016
Rose, Nelson & Hardiman 2016Rose, Nelson & Hardiman 2016
Rose, Nelson & Hardiman 2016
 

Destacado

Títulos valores
Títulos valores Títulos valores
Títulos valores Mafe Bedoya
 
Orientações sobre as atividades de alfabetização módulo 1
Orientações sobre as atividades de alfabetização módulo 1Orientações sobre as atividades de alfabetização módulo 1
Orientações sobre as atividades de alfabetização módulo 1con_seguir
 
Modelos de negocio
Modelos de negocioModelos de negocio
Modelos de negocioOmar Vite
 
Taller etapas del ciclo vital del documento
Taller etapas del ciclo vital del documentoTaller etapas del ciclo vital del documento
Taller etapas del ciclo vital del documentoGuio Tor A.
 
Estrategias de comunicación en medios digitales - Sesión 3
Estrategias de comunicación en medios digitales - Sesión 3Estrategias de comunicación en medios digitales - Sesión 3
Estrategias de comunicación en medios digitales - Sesión 3Omar Vite
 
funciones especiales y transformaciones de graficas
funciones especiales y transformaciones de graficasfunciones especiales y transformaciones de graficas
funciones especiales y transformaciones de graficasEszme Hdez
 
Management Vs Leadership Linked 2 Leadership
Management Vs Leadership   Linked 2 LeadershipManagement Vs Leadership   Linked 2 Leadership
Management Vs Leadership Linked 2 LeadershipPrashanth Raajanna
 
Management Vs Leadership On Linkedin
Management Vs Leadership On LinkedinManagement Vs Leadership On Linkedin
Management Vs Leadership On LinkedinMir_Ali
 
Reingenieria de procesos
Reingenieria de procesosReingenieria de procesos
Reingenieria de procesosaclle
 
Fundamentos de bpm gestion de procesos de negocio control y mejora
Fundamentos de bpm   gestion  de procesos de negocio control y mejoraFundamentos de bpm   gestion  de procesos de negocio control y mejora
Fundamentos de bpm gestion de procesos de negocio control y mejoraMeinzul ND
 
Caderno Educação Ambiental do Governo do Estado de SP - Resíduos Sólidos -
Caderno Educação Ambiental do Governo do Estado de SP - Resíduos Sólidos - Caderno Educação Ambiental do Governo do Estado de SP - Resíduos Sólidos -
Caderno Educação Ambiental do Governo do Estado de SP - Resíduos Sólidos - Tainá Bimbati
 
Manejo del lugar de los hechos
Manejo del lugar de los hechosManejo del lugar de los hechos
Manejo del lugar de los hechosjoshito23
 
Material monster is ii emco
Material  monster is ii emcoMaterial  monster is ii emco
Material monster is ii emcoFranz Marulanda
 
¿Qué es un modelo de negocio?
¿Qué es un modelo de negocio?¿Qué es un modelo de negocio?
¿Qué es un modelo de negocio?Omar Vite
 

Destacado (20)

effective communication skills
effective communication skillseffective communication skills
effective communication skills
 
Títulos valores
Títulos valores Títulos valores
Títulos valores
 
Orientações sobre as atividades de alfabetização módulo 1
Orientações sobre as atividades de alfabetização módulo 1Orientações sobre as atividades de alfabetização módulo 1
Orientações sobre as atividades de alfabetização módulo 1
 
Antologia redes convergentes (4)
Antologia redes convergentes (4)Antologia redes convergentes (4)
Antologia redes convergentes (4)
 
Modelos de negocio
Modelos de negocioModelos de negocio
Modelos de negocio
 
Taller etapas del ciclo vital del documento
Taller etapas del ciclo vital del documentoTaller etapas del ciclo vital del documento
Taller etapas del ciclo vital del documento
 
Estrategias de comunicación en medios digitales - Sesión 3
Estrategias de comunicación en medios digitales - Sesión 3Estrategias de comunicación en medios digitales - Sesión 3
Estrategias de comunicación en medios digitales - Sesión 3
 
inteligencia tactica
inteligencia tacticainteligencia tactica
inteligencia tactica
 
funciones especiales y transformaciones de graficas
funciones especiales y transformaciones de graficasfunciones especiales y transformaciones de graficas
funciones especiales y transformaciones de graficas
 
Management Vs Leadership Linked 2 Leadership
Management Vs Leadership   Linked 2 LeadershipManagement Vs Leadership   Linked 2 Leadership
Management Vs Leadership Linked 2 Leadership
 
Management Vs Leadership On Linkedin
Management Vs Leadership On LinkedinManagement Vs Leadership On Linkedin
Management Vs Leadership On Linkedin
 
Reingenieria de procesos
Reingenieria de procesosReingenieria de procesos
Reingenieria de procesos
 
Ley federal de responsabilidades de los servidores publicos
Ley federal de responsabilidades de los servidores publicosLey federal de responsabilidades de los servidores publicos
Ley federal de responsabilidades de los servidores publicos
 
Fundamentos de bpm gestion de procesos de negocio control y mejora
Fundamentos de bpm   gestion  de procesos de negocio control y mejoraFundamentos de bpm   gestion  de procesos de negocio control y mejora
Fundamentos de bpm gestion de procesos de negocio control y mejora
 
Conflictos laborales
Conflictos laboralesConflictos laborales
Conflictos laborales
 
Metodología Canvas
Metodología CanvasMetodología Canvas
Metodología Canvas
 
Caderno Educação Ambiental do Governo do Estado de SP - Resíduos Sólidos -
Caderno Educação Ambiental do Governo do Estado de SP - Resíduos Sólidos - Caderno Educação Ambiental do Governo do Estado de SP - Resíduos Sólidos -
Caderno Educação Ambiental do Governo do Estado de SP - Resíduos Sólidos -
 
Manejo del lugar de los hechos
Manejo del lugar de los hechosManejo del lugar de los hechos
Manejo del lugar de los hechos
 
Material monster is ii emco
Material  monster is ii emcoMaterial  monster is ii emco
Material monster is ii emco
 
¿Qué es un modelo de negocio?
¿Qué es un modelo de negocio?¿Qué es un modelo de negocio?
¿Qué es un modelo de negocio?
 

Similar a Attunement and parents

Colleagues ResponsesColleagues responsesAssignment 4 8080
Colleagues ResponsesColleagues responsesAssignment 4 8080 Colleagues ResponsesColleagues responsesAssignment 4 8080
Colleagues ResponsesColleagues responsesAssignment 4 8080 WilheminaRossi174
 
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...Felicia Nicole Ghrist
 
COVID-19 Impact on Child Development
COVID-19 Impact on Child DevelopmentCOVID-19 Impact on Child Development
COVID-19 Impact on Child DevelopmentNicoleNadler2
 
Running head IMPACTS OF CHILD ABUSE ON GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT.docx
Running head IMPACTS OF CHILD ABUSE ON GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT.docxRunning head IMPACTS OF CHILD ABUSE ON GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT.docx
Running head IMPACTS OF CHILD ABUSE ON GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT.docxcowinhelen
 
Article Review.docx
Article Review.docxArticle Review.docx
Article Review.docxAliBullock1
 
Real children. real parents. real places. real parent involvement. handout
Real children. real parents. real places. real parent involvement. handoutReal children. real parents. real places. real parent involvement. handout
Real children. real parents. real places. real parent involvement. handoutChris Shade
 
Running Head LIFE SPAN PARENTING PROJECT1LIFE SPAN PARENTING.docx
Running Head LIFE SPAN PARENTING PROJECT1LIFE SPAN PARENTING.docxRunning Head LIFE SPAN PARENTING PROJECT1LIFE SPAN PARENTING.docx
Running Head LIFE SPAN PARENTING PROJECT1LIFE SPAN PARENTING.docxwlynn1
 
Current Events and Childhood Essay.pdf
Current Events and Childhood Essay.pdfCurrent Events and Childhood Essay.pdf
Current Events and Childhood Essay.pdfOliviaTheisen
 
Parental Influence in Development (Final)
Parental Influence in Development (Final)Parental Influence in Development (Final)
Parental Influence in Development (Final)Randall Noggle
 
Supporting Wellness in Children with Mental Illness
Supporting Wellness in Children with Mental IllnessSupporting Wellness in Children with Mental Illness
Supporting Wellness in Children with Mental IllnessHouse of New Hope
 
4080 Current Events and Child Development Paper.docx
4080 Current Events and Child Development Paper.docx4080 Current Events and Child Development Paper.docx
4080 Current Events and Child Development Paper.docxAliBullock1
 
DISSERTATION PROPOSAL PRO FORMAFORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY & CRIMINAL IN.docx
DISSERTATION PROPOSAL PRO FORMAFORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY & CRIMINAL IN.docxDISSERTATION PROPOSAL PRO FORMAFORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY & CRIMINAL IN.docx
DISSERTATION PROPOSAL PRO FORMAFORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY & CRIMINAL IN.docxelinoraudley582231
 
Blair raver ap_exp_canalization
Blair raver ap_exp_canalizationBlair raver ap_exp_canalization
Blair raver ap_exp_canalizationyovi yova
 
Blair raver ap_exp_canalization
Blair raver ap_exp_canalizationBlair raver ap_exp_canalization
Blair raver ap_exp_canalizationyovi yova
 
Unifying Your Community Around Education Handout
Unifying Your Community Around Education HandoutUnifying Your Community Around Education Handout
Unifying Your Community Around Education HandoutChris Shade
 
A Child And Youth Care Approach To Professional Development And Training
A Child And Youth Care Approach To Professional Development And TrainingA Child And Youth Care Approach To Professional Development And Training
A Child And Youth Care Approach To Professional Development And TrainingSarah Morrow
 
week 3 forum.docx
week 3 forum.docxweek 3 forum.docx
week 3 forum.docxwrite5
 
Current Events and Child Development
Current Events and Child DevelopmentCurrent Events and Child Development
Current Events and Child DevelopmentParissaShedd
 

Similar a Attunement and parents (20)

Colleagues ResponsesColleagues responsesAssignment 4 8080
Colleagues ResponsesColleagues responsesAssignment 4 8080 Colleagues ResponsesColleagues responsesAssignment 4 8080
Colleagues ResponsesColleagues responsesAssignment 4 8080
 
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
 
COVID-19 Impact on Child Development
COVID-19 Impact on Child DevelopmentCOVID-19 Impact on Child Development
COVID-19 Impact on Child Development
 
Running head IMPACTS OF CHILD ABUSE ON GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT.docx
Running head IMPACTS OF CHILD ABUSE ON GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT.docxRunning head IMPACTS OF CHILD ABUSE ON GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT.docx
Running head IMPACTS OF CHILD ABUSE ON GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT.docx
 
Article Review.docx
Article Review.docxArticle Review.docx
Article Review.docx
 
Real children. real parents. real places. real parent involvement. handout
Real children. real parents. real places. real parent involvement. handoutReal children. real parents. real places. real parent involvement. handout
Real children. real parents. real places. real parent involvement. handout
 
Running Head LIFE SPAN PARENTING PROJECT1LIFE SPAN PARENTING.docx
Running Head LIFE SPAN PARENTING PROJECT1LIFE SPAN PARENTING.docxRunning Head LIFE SPAN PARENTING PROJECT1LIFE SPAN PARENTING.docx
Running Head LIFE SPAN PARENTING PROJECT1LIFE SPAN PARENTING.docx
 
Current Events and Childhood Essay.pdf
Current Events and Childhood Essay.pdfCurrent Events and Childhood Essay.pdf
Current Events and Childhood Essay.pdf
 
Parental Influence in Development (Final)
Parental Influence in Development (Final)Parental Influence in Development (Final)
Parental Influence in Development (Final)
 
TMA03
TMA03TMA03
TMA03
 
Supporting Wellness in Children with Mental Illness
Supporting Wellness in Children with Mental IllnessSupporting Wellness in Children with Mental Illness
Supporting Wellness in Children with Mental Illness
 
4080 Current Events and Child Development Paper.docx
4080 Current Events and Child Development Paper.docx4080 Current Events and Child Development Paper.docx
4080 Current Events and Child Development Paper.docx
 
DISSERTATION PROPOSAL PRO FORMAFORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY & CRIMINAL IN.docx
DISSERTATION PROPOSAL PRO FORMAFORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY & CRIMINAL IN.docxDISSERTATION PROPOSAL PRO FORMAFORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY & CRIMINAL IN.docx
DISSERTATION PROPOSAL PRO FORMAFORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY & CRIMINAL IN.docx
 
Blair raver ap_exp_canalization
Blair raver ap_exp_canalizationBlair raver ap_exp_canalization
Blair raver ap_exp_canalization
 
Blair raver ap_exp_canalization
Blair raver ap_exp_canalizationBlair raver ap_exp_canalization
Blair raver ap_exp_canalization
 
Unifying Your Community Around Education Handout
Unifying Your Community Around Education HandoutUnifying Your Community Around Education Handout
Unifying Your Community Around Education Handout
 
A Child And Youth Care Approach To Professional Development And Training
A Child And Youth Care Approach To Professional Development And TrainingA Child And Youth Care Approach To Professional Development And Training
A Child And Youth Care Approach To Professional Development And Training
 
week 3 forum.docx
week 3 forum.docxweek 3 forum.docx
week 3 forum.docx
 
Lifespan research (1)
Lifespan research (1)Lifespan research (1)
Lifespan research (1)
 
Current Events and Child Development
Current Events and Child DevelopmentCurrent Events and Child Development
Current Events and Child Development
 

Último

Transaction Management in Database Management System
Transaction Management in Database Management SystemTransaction Management in Database Management System
Transaction Management in Database Management SystemChristalin Nelson
 
Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)
Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)
Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)cama23
 
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY - GERBNER.pptx
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY -  GERBNER.pptxAUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY -  GERBNER.pptx
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY - GERBNER.pptxiammrhaywood
 
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptxmary850239
 
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...JhezDiaz1
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Concurrency Control in Database Management system
Concurrency Control in Database Management systemConcurrency Control in Database Management system
Concurrency Control in Database Management systemChristalin Nelson
 
Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdf
Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdfGrade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdf
Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdfJemuel Francisco
 
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptx
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptxCulture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptx
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptxPoojaSen20
 
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptxJudging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptxSherlyMaeNeri
 
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdfACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdfSpandanaRallapalli
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERPHow to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...Seán Kennedy
 
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for ParentsChoosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parentsnavabharathschool99
 
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATIONTHEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATIONHumphrey A Beña
 

Último (20)

Transaction Management in Database Management System
Transaction Management in Database Management SystemTransaction Management in Database Management System
Transaction Management in Database Management System
 
Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)
Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)
Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)
 
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptxFINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
 
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY - GERBNER.pptx
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY -  GERBNER.pptxAUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY -  GERBNER.pptx
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY - GERBNER.pptx
 
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx
 
YOUVE_GOT_EMAIL_PRELIMS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
YOUVE_GOT_EMAIL_PRELIMS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptxYOUVE_GOT_EMAIL_PRELIMS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
YOUVE_GOT_EMAIL_PRELIMS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
 
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
 
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
 
Concurrency Control in Database Management system
Concurrency Control in Database Management systemConcurrency Control in Database Management system
Concurrency Control in Database Management system
 
Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdf
Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdfGrade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdf
Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdf
 
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptx
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptxCulture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptx
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptx
 
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
 
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptxJudging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
 
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdfACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
 
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERPHow to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
 
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
 
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for ParentsChoosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
 
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATIONTHEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
 

Attunement and parents

  • 1. 132 Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005) et al. Child Dombrowski Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/car.888 Stefan C. A Positive Dombrowski* Rider University Lawrenceville, USA Behavioural Susan G. Timmer Intervention for Dawn M. Blacker Anthony J. Urquiza Toddlers: Parent– University of California Davis Children’s Hospital Sacramento, USA Child Attunement Therapy Parent–child attunement therapy (PCAT) is a promising intervention for toddlers (aged 12–30 months) who have experienced maltreatment. PCAT has two overall purposes: (1) to strengthen caregivers’ relationship with their children; and (2) to facilitate caregivers’ learning of appropriate child management techniques. PCAT represents an adaptation of parent–child interaction therapy (PCIT), which has been empirically documented in preschool and early elementary schoolchildren to improve behavioural adjustment and engender a ‘A noted paucity stronger bond between caregiver and child. There is, however, a noted paucity of intervention research for toddlers, specifically maltreated of intervention toddlers. As toddlerhood represents a critical period for enhancing the research for relationship between caregivers and children and is a stage when youngsters are at increased risk for maltreatment, the objectives of toddlers, PCAT become even more salient during the toddler years. The purpose specifically of this study, therefore, is to introduce PCAT and then examine its effectiveness through a single case study of a 23-month-old maltreated maltreated toddler and his biological mother. Pre- and post-assessment measures toddlers’ included the Parenting Stress Index, the Dyadic Parent–Child Interaction Coding System (DPICS), the Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), the Emotional Availability (EA) Scales and the Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory (ECBI). The results of this study demonstrate the effectiveness of PCAT in increasing the number of positive caregiver–toddler interactions and enhancing the overall quality of the caregiver–toddler relationship. Practitioners will be able to use the techniques described in this manuscript to improve the parent–toddler relationship and ameliorate many commonly experienced behavioural difficulties found among maltreatment-prone parent–toddler dyads. Therapeutic progress is easily charted so that ∗ Correspondence to: Stefan C. Dombrowski, 202 Memorial Hall, Rider University, Lawrenceville, New Jersey 08648, USA. Tel: 609 895 5448. E-mail: sdombrowski@rider.edu Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: December 2004 Accepted 7 132–151 (2005)
  • 2. Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers 133 effectiveness may be documented and termination of therapy may be easily discerned. Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. KEY WORDS: toddler; maltreatment; parent training; behavioural intervention arent-child attunement therapy (PCAT) is a behaviour- P ally oriented intervention for maltreated toddlers (aged 12–30 months) and toddlers experiencing attachment dif- ficulties. It is adapted from parent–child interaction therapy (PCIT), an empirically validated parent skills training pro- gramme that has a solid research basis and uses therapist coaching of caregivers through a bug-in-the-ear device and a two-way mirrored window (Eyberg, 1988; Hembree-Kigin and McNeil, 1994). Like PCIT, PCAT focuses on enhancing the caregiver–child relationship by improving the toddler’s beha- viour. This is accomplished by increasing parents’ attention to children’s positive and appropriate behaviour, decreasing attention to inappropriate behaviour, and teaching parents to ‘Teaching parents follow their children’s lead in play, supporting and elaborat- to follow their ing upon it rather than directing it (Dombrowski and Timmer, 2001; Paravicini, 2000; Paravicini et al., 2000). Because the children’s lead in goals of PCAT involve consistent attention to their children’s play, supporting good behaviour, parents become more accessible and their and elaborating behaviour becomes more predictable. Since inconsistent re- sponsiveness and a lack of positive interactions characterize upon it rather than abusive family contexts (Cerezo and D’Ocon, 1999; Wolfe, directing it’ 1987), we argue that PCAT is well suited for toddlers who have experienced maltreatment and who might possibly suffer attachment difficulties. The purpose of this study is to introduce PCAT through an examination of its effectiveness with a 23-month-old maltreated toddler and his biological mother. Although this study offers a useful introduction to PCAT, the effectiveness of PCAT will require further empir- ical validation in subsequent research. Background Information Toddlerhood is an important stage in the development of children. This period represents a significant period for developing attachment security, enhancing the relationship between caregivers and children and acquiring cognitive and language skills (Cicchetti and Toth, 1995). It is also a period in which the attachment relationship emerges and then is continually negotiated between caregiver and child Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 3. 134 Dombrowski et al. (Cummings et al., 2000). Emerging out of this process is a new set of developmental tasks that focus on self-development (Cole, 1990), emotion regulation (Smetana et al., 1999) and social representational models (Toth et al., 1997). Research is also clear in indicating that toddlerhood represents a stage where children are at increased risk for maltreatment (Wolfe, 1987, 1999). Child maltreatment during any phase of development has adverse implications, robbing children of their fundamental human dignity and placing them at great developmental disadvantage compared with non- ‘During the abused children (Shengold, 1989). During the toddler years, toddler years, maltreatment may interrupt critical developmental processes that, without intervention, may continue to disrupt later maltreatment may cognitive, social and emotional functioning (Manly et al., interrupt critical 2001). developmental During the toddler stage, neurological development progresses at an accelerated rate and maltreatment has been processes’ found to perturb the development of specific neurological structures implicated in behavioural, cognitive and social– emotional functioning (Lowenthal, 1998; Schore, 2001; Teicher et al., 2002). For instance, early maltreatment has been linked to abnormal cerebral cortex and limbic system development (Glaser, 2000; Teicher et al., 2002). These struc- tures are associated with regulation of cognition, attention and emotion (Kalat, 2002). The toddler years are also thought to be important for the formation of attachment security (Youngblade and Belsky, 1989) and self-representation (Cichetti and Toth, 1994). Maltreatment during this stage of development disrupts these formative processes, contribut- ing to a host of later cognitive, academic, psychological and relationship problems that may persist well into adulthood (Dombrowski, 2003; Dombrowski et al., 2003). Unfortun- ately, there is a relative paucity of interventions not only for maltreated toddlers specifically, but for maltreated children more generally. There are, however, numerous parent training programmes targeted at improving parental sensitivity and children’s attachment security. Bakermans-Kranenburg et al. (2003) conducted a meta-analysis of 70 parent training programmes targeted at improving parental sensitivity and children’s attach- ment security. These authors found evidence for the effective- ness of parent training programmes, particularly if the training programmes had a clear-cut behavioural emphasis. Of the 70 programmes evaluated within the meta-analysis, none were focused on children or toddlers who experienced maltreat- ment. Thus, there is significant need for intervention research for those who have been maltreated or who may be at risk for Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 4. Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers 135 maltreatment. There is an even greater need for intervention research on maltreated toddlers. The purpose of this study is to describe PCAT, an initial step in filling the intervention gap, and its effectiveness for a mother–toddler dyad with a history of abuse and domestic violence. We also discuss some of the supports and barriers to treatment success. Subsequent research on PCAT will be ‘Subsequent necessary to ultimately determine its effectiveness. A discus- research on PCAT sion of the tenets of parent–child interaction therapy, the pro- genitor of PCAT, will be helpful in understanding PCAT, as will be necessary both the philosophy and procedures overlap. to ultimately determine its Parent–Child Interaction Therapy (PCIT) effectiveness’ PCIT is a behaviourally oriented, empirically validated parent training model that has been cited as effective in increasing positive caregiver–child interactions, enhancing the parent–child relationship, reducing parenting stress and decreasing dysfunctional parent–child relationship patterns (Eyberg and Robinson, 1982). It has also been documented to be effective in ameliorating those same problems among maltreated children (Borrego et al., 1999; Urquiza and McNeil, 1996). PCIT contains two phases, each of which is preceded by a didactic session. During the didactic session, the caregiver and child are instructed in the mechanics of the particular phase and provided with a rationale for practising concepts. In subsequent treatment sessions, the parent prac- tises these skills as they play with their children. While parent and child play together, the parent is coached by their ther- ‘While parent and apist. The coaching is conducted from an observation room child play together, via a ‘bug-in-the-ear’ receiver that the parent wears behind the ear. Parents continue to be taught and practise specific the parent is communication skills with their children. In the relationship coached by their enhancement phase (typically seven to ten sessions), termed therapist’ child directed interaction (CDI), the primary goal is to create or strengthen a positive and mutually rewarding relationship between parents and their children by modifying the way parents speak to their children. Parents are taught to follow their children’s lead in play by describing their activities and reflecting their appropriate verbalizations. They are also taught to praise their children’s positive behaviour and to ignore (in this phase of treatment) the children’s negative behaviour. By the end of the ‘relationship enhancement’ phase, parents have generally shifted from rarely attending to their children’s positive behaviour to frequently and consistently praising appropriate child behaviour. They also shift from trying to Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 5. 136 Dombrowski et al. control their children’s play to describing their play in a way that conveys their interest in the child’s activity. In the second phase, termed parent directed interaction (PDI) (typically seven to ten sessions following CDI), the primary goal is to provide effective parenting skills for parents to use in manag- ing their child’s behaviour. Often, many of the child’s dif- ficult behaviours decrease by the end of CDI, which makes the process of training parents to obtain compliance with com- mands easier (Eisenstadt et al., 1983). In PDI, therapists continue to focus parents’ attention on their children’s posi- tive behaviours. However, they also begin training in giving clear, direct commands and avoiding indirect commands such as those in the form of a question (e.g. ‘Do you want to clean up your toys now?’) that imply that the child has the option to ‘Once parents avoid the unpleasant task. Once parents master giving effec- master giving tive commands, they learn to give children choices to comply that may result in time-outs or a removal of privileges. By the effective end of PDI, the processes of giving commands and gaining commands, compliance are predictable and safe (Eyberg, 1988). Parents they learn to are generally able to obtain compliance without giving a time- out, but if they need to give a time-out, it is a comfortable, give children predictable and well-practised process for which the parent choices to comply’ has acquired mastery (see Hembree-Kigin and McNeil, 1995, for a full description of the PCIT programme). PCIT offers a structured, individualized, behavioural yet manualized approach to attain this goal. Parent–Child Attunement Therapy (PCAT) PCAT uses PCIT techniques that have been altered to be more developmentally appropriate for children younger than 30 months of age. (A comparative summary of PCAT and PCIT is provided in Table 1.) Like PCIT, PCAT requires the collection of pre- and post-intervention comprehensive assess- ment data that include an evaluation of the caregiver–child interaction pattern. The assessment of the relationship dyad serves as baseline data against which intervention progress is monitored. Next, the caregiver is introduced to the mech- anics and rationale of PCAT in an initial training session. Following this didactic session and in all subsequent sessions, caregivers are taught the tenets of child directed interaction (i.e. how to follow the toddler’s lead in play), which is the foundation of the relationship enhancement phase of PCIT. This includes training on the use of non-directive types of verbalizations or communication (e.g. appropriately praising, describing and reflecting toddlers’ behaviour). Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 6. Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers 137 Table 1. Comparison of PCAT and PCIT Similarities Unique to PCAT 1. Didactic training session 2. Use similar technology (e.g. remote hearing device; two-way mirror) 3. Data collection 4. Avoid criticism, commands & threats 5. Mastery criteria 6. Limit questions 7. Daily 10-minute practice assignment 8. Emphasize non-directive play 1. Simpler language structure 2. Reduced session length (30–45 min) 3. Greater focus on parent enthusiasm 4. Lack of discipline phase (e.g. time-out) 5. Flexibility to deal with diaper soiling 6. Toddler fatigue 7. Focus on increasing positive touching (e.g. hugs) 8. Requires developmentally younger toys In PCAT, therapists teach and serve as a model to caregivers ‘In PCAT, on how to differentially reinforce children for appropriate therapists teach behaviours (e.g. enthusiastic praise after putting the toys away; ignore when the toddler displays temper outbursts). As in and serve as a PCIT, this is accomplished through real-time coaching via a model to bug-in-the-ear device and a two-way mirror. During initial ses- caregivers’ sions, the therapist asks the caregiver to repeat their words as closely as possible. Therapists model both the verbal (e.g. ‘I like how you listen to me’) and non-verbal (e.g. give the toddler a hug after successful completion of a task) aspects of the ses- sion. Over the course of treatment, the therapist encourages the parent to tailor the interaction by giving more general instructions (e.g. ‘Go ahead and describe what Tim is doing’) or prompting the parent (‘Tim is really playing gently with the toys’). In this way, therapists gradually lead the caregivers towards an understanding of their children and the skills they need to ensure their children’s psychological well-being. Caregivers are taught specific strategies that are aimed at ‘Specific strategies giving the toddler control during play. For instance, parents that are aimed at are taught to describe what the toddler is doing and how to reflect and imitate the toddler’s own verbalizations or actions giving the toddler (when they are appropriate). Reflection might include the control during caregiver repeating, imitating or elaborating on what the play’ toddler has just said or done. Additionally, the caregiver is taught to use simple, more developmentally appropriate lan- guage, a high degree of enthusiasm and non-verbal signs of approval (e.g. ‘Good job’ stated enthusiastically followed by a hug or hand clapping). Caregivers are also coached to limit their use of commands and questions. These verbalizations are perceived as demand- ing a response from the toddler, and therefore taking control Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 7. 138 Dombrowski et al. of the interaction, rather than following the toddler’s lead in ‘Excessive parent play. Excessive parent control during playtime is common in control during abusive mother–child dyads (Bousha and Twentyman, 1984) and this coercive interaction style may foster resistance to playtime is parents’ commands (Kochanska and Aksan, 1995). These common in abusive characteristics detract from efforts to enhance the parent– mother–child toddler relationship. Caregivers are taught to control their toddler’s behaviour dyads’ by ignoring inappropriate behaviour while redirecting beha- viour towards more appropriate and praiseable activity. For instance, the caregiver might remove an undesirable toy from the toddler’s hands and provide the child with a different, but functionally similar toy when the toddler engages in inappro- priate behaviour with that toy. Or, for example, the caregiver might physically redirect a child if the toddler attempts to run out of the door. In this instance, the toddler may be picked up, returned to the play location and redirected to another activity while the caregiver places him/herself in between the toddler and the door, blocking any further attempts to leave. ‘The therapist must Throughout all PCAT sessions, the therapist must remain remain flexible and flexible and make allowances for unexpected events (e.g. soiling of diapers, extreme crankiness). This might include make allowances abbreviating the session or cancelling the session altogether. for unexpected Finally, PCAT sessions are shorter (only 30–45 minutes) than events’ an average PCIT session (60 minutes), generally less struc- tured and allow the toddler greater freedom to move around the room and play on the floor or at the table. The follow- ing is a brief example of a typical PCAT coaching session [Caregiver and toddler are sitting on the floor while the toddler plays with a red truck]: Therapist: Tell Tim, ‘Tim, you’re driving the truck’. Caregiver: ‘Tim, you’re driving the truck.’ [Describing appro- priate behaviour] Therapist: That was a terrific description of Tim’s behaviour. You will help him maintain his interest in playing with the toy. Toddler: Verbalizes ‘red truck’. Therapist: Now state to Tim, ‘That’s right you’re playing with a red truck’. Caregiver: ‘That’s right! You’re playing with a red truck!’ [Reflecting Tim’s verbalizations and describing his behaviour] Therapist: That was a great reflection of what Tim has just said. You show interest in what he is doing when you restate what he says. Toddler: Want candy. [Knocking the truck over as he says this] Therapist: Ignore his request for candy and his knocking over of the truck. Instead, get the other truck and begin to Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 8. Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers 139 enthusiastically roll it on the floor, making an engine sound as you roll it. Caregiver: ‘Vroom, Vroom, Vroom.’ [While rolling the truck on the floor] [Tim resumes his interest in the toy and forgets about the candy] Therapist: Notice how Tim has forgotten about the candy and resumed playing with the toy. Very good job ignoring his inappropriate behaviour. Therapist: Tell Tim that you really like playing with him and give him a hug. Caregiver: ‘Tim, I really like playing with you.’ [Hugs Tim] Therapist: Very good praise. I also really liked how you hugged Tim. Tim seemed to like it as well as he smiled when you hugged him. This shows interest in Tim and will help to make him feel special. Throughout each session, caregivers are provided with instruction regarding simple behavioural reinforcement tech- niques (e.g. praising and positive attention for appropriate behaviours). Caregivers are also taught to avoid using state- ‘Caregivers are ments that promote coercive or negative interactions (e.g. also taught to avoid commands, criticism, threats) as these behaviours are thought to harm the relationship process and promote a cycle of nega- using statements tive interactions. When a toddler engages in behaviour that is that promote inappropriate, the caregiver is taught to either ignore that beha- coercive or negative viour or redirect the toddler to a more appropriate behaviour. interactions’ Method Subject The subject of this study is L, a 23-month-old male toddler, and his 25-year-old biological mother. The referral for treat- ment stated that the toddler had been removed at birth from his parents’ care due to mother’s drug use and father’s un- willingness and inability to care for L and his three siblings (all under 4 years of age) on his own. However, when L was born, his mother was in jail, which also contributed to her inability to care for the child. It is suspected that L had been exposed prenatally to drugs since his mother was reported to have used drugs before going to jail, though not documented. L lived with one foster family until the time he was reunified with his biological parents and siblings at 22 months of age. Shortly after reunification, the toddler was referred to the University of California, Davis Children’s Hospital for court-mandated PCAT following concerns about temper tantrums (including Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 9. 140 Dombrowski et al. throwing objects), aggression towards younger sister and head- banging when his parents set limits on his behaviour. Measures ‘Five measures Five measures were used to evaluate the effectiveness of PCAT were used to in this case study: the Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), Dyadic Parent–Child Interaction Coding System evaluate the (DPICS), Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory (ECBI), the Par- effectiveness of enting Stress Index—Short Form (PSI-SF) and the Emotional PCAT in this case Availability Scales (EA). study’ Dyadic Parent–Child Interaction Coding System (DPICS) The DPICS is a behavioural coding system designed to assess the quality of parent–child social interactions through observ- ation of parent–child dyads (Eyberg and Robinson, 1982). The DPICS consists of behavioural categories for both children and caregivers. Although the DPICS was designed to measure 20 different non-verbal and verbal parent–child behaviours and verbalizations (e.g. whining, laughing), parental verbal beha- viour was given primary consideration during PCAT coding sessions. The therapist coded the mothers’ use of praise, de- scriptions, reflections, questions, commands and critical state- ments in the first 5 minutes of each treatment session. Half of these sessions were recoded in order to ensure the reliability of the therapist’s codes. Intraclass correlation coefficients (a measure of intercoder reliability) for the coding categories were the following: descriptions/reflections = 0.76; praises = 0.94; questions/commands = 0.97. Consistent with the PCIT coding approach, PCAT coding focuses on verbalizations that are thought to be important in enhancing the caregiver–child relationship and reducing dysfunctional relationship patterns. Specific ‘mastery’ criteria are established at PCIT levels with respect to descriptive and reflective statements (25) and praises (15). Question mastery ‘Caregiver criteria are established at PCIT levels of 3 or less. Caregiver commands are commands are discouraged during playtime as they serve to remove control from the child. The following is a more pre- discouraged during cise operational definition of descriptions, reflections, praises, playtime as they questions and commands: serve to remove Descriptions: statements or sentences that describe the child, the objects with which the child is playing and the activity in control from the which the child is engaging (e.g. ‘You’re playing with the red child’ truck’, ‘You’re sitting in your seat’). Reflections: statements that repeat or rephrase a preceding toddler verbalization (e.g. toddler states ‘Want car’; caregiver states ‘You want the car’). Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 10. Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers 141 Praise: positive evaluation of attributes or behaviour of the child. There are generally two types of praise that are coded: labelled and unlabelled. Labelled praises are very precise, describing the reason the praise was offered (e.g. ‘Good job rolling the truck!’). Unlabelled praises are vague and non- specific (e.g. ‘Great job!’). Questions: verbal statements that invite or call for a reply (e.g. ‘Do you want to play with the dolls?’). Commands: directions from caregivers to toddler indicating that a verbal or motor activity should be performed (e.g. ‘Pick up the truck from the floor’). Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) The CBCL (Achenbach, 1991) is a standardized instru- ment that lists 112 problem child behaviours. This version is completed by a parent or regular caregiver and describes the behaviour of children between the ages of 4 and 16 years. Separate norms are provided for both boys and girls in three ‘Separate norms age groups. Normative data are derived from a large sociolog- are provided for ically diverse population of both non-referred and clinically referred children and their parents. The CBCL is composed both boys and girls of a total problem score, two broadband scales (internalizing, in three age externalizing) and eight narrow-band scales for each age group groups’ and sex (e.g. withdrawn, somatic complaints, delinquent beha- viour, aggressive behaviour). The clinical cutoff score for the broadband scales is a T-score greater than or equal to 65. Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory (ECBI) The ECBI is a 36-item scale that measures specific behaviour problems exhibited by children aged 2–16 years. Parents indicate the frequency of certain behaviours (intensity score) and whether they are considered to be problems (problem score) (Eyberg and Robinson, 1982; Eyberg and Ross, 1978). The ECBI has been standardized on a number of populations (Eyberg and Robinson, 1982; Eyberg and Ross, 1978). Test– retest reliability scores across a 3-week timespan on the ECBI intensity and problem scales were 0.86 and 0.88 respectively (Robinson and Eyberg, 1981; Robinson et al., 1980). The published clinical cutoff scores are an intensity score of greater than 131 or a problem score of greater than 16. Parenting Stress Index—Short Form (PSI-SF) The PSI-SF (Abidin, 1990) is a 36-item inventory designed to identify parent–child dyads that are experiencing stress and are at risk for developing dysfunctional parenting and child behaviour problems. The PSI-SF consists of three subscales: difficult child, parent distress, and dysfunctional parent–child Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 11. 142 Dombrowski et al. relationship. The PSI-SF also contains a measure of defensive responding. Low scores (<15th percentile) on this scale have been shown to indicate that parents are minimizing problems associated with the parent role, that they do not have a close relationship with the children and are therefore unaffected by their behaviour, or that they are simply not stressed by situa- tions that stress normal parents. The test–retest and internal consistency reliability of the PSI on various scales ranges from 0.68 to 0.84 (Abidin, 1990). Emotional Availability Scales (EA) ‘Four global parent The EA Scales (Biringen et al., 1998) consist of four global scales and two parent scales and two child scales that measure specific dimensions of the caregiver–child relationship. Parent scales child scales that measure their sensitivity to the child, their non-hostility, measure specific non-intrusiveness and ability to structure the interaction. Child dimensions of the scales measure their responsiveness to the parent and the degree to which they involve the parent in their activities. caregiver–child Parent sensitivity scores reflect the degree to which the parent relationship’ perceives and responds to the child’s cues, the parent’s engage- ment and interest in the child’s activity, as well as the affec- tive quality and conflict management. Parental structuring scores reflect the parent’s ability to give structure to an inter- action (i.e. scaffold) so that the child responds positively. Parent non-intrusiveness refers to the parent’s ability to give structure to the interaction without over-controlling and diminishing the child’s autonomy. Parental non-hostility scores reflect the degree to which the parent’s actions and tone of voice convey anger, impatience or boredom. Child respons- iveness refers to the degree to which the child responds to the parent in a positive, emotionally available manner and the degree to which the child balances autonomous pursuits and interest in the parent’s activities. Child involvement measures the degree to which the child involves the parent in his or her play, taking into account the balance of child-initiated and parent-initiated interactions. Higher scores reflect more opti- mal emotional availability. According to Biringen et al. (1998, cited in Easterbrooks et al., 2000), parent sensitivity scores above 4 are in the optimal range. Additionally, non-hostility scores above 4 and non-intrusiveness and structuring scores above 3, and child responsiveness and involvement scores above 4, are in the optimal range. Procedure While learning PCAT, caregivers are taught to follow their child’s lead in play, to praise the child for behaving Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 12. Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers 143 appropriately and to limit the number of questions and commands elicited during play. Caregivers are also taught to ‘Caregivers ignore inappropriate behaviour and to redirect the child’s are also taught behaviour when appropriate. In this study, PCAT was taught to the mother through an initial child directed interaction to ignore (CDI) didactic training session. Prior to the initial didactic inappropriate session, before each weekly coaching session and at the end of behaviour’ treatment, the mother and toddler were coded for 5 minutes using the DPICS. (The caregiver was instructed to follow the toddler’s lead in play during which time the trained therapist coded the parent–child interactions.) Following the 5 minute DPICS coding, the therapist coached the parent on appropri- ate parent–child interaction through the use of a one-way mirror and a bug-in-the-ear device. In this case study, the mother and the toddler participated in an initial pre-treatment DPICS coding session followed by nine coaching sessions and a post-treatment session. During the post-treatment session, the caregiver was coded using the DPICS and the EA Scales. The caregiver was asked to complete a battery of standardized measures (e.g. ECBI, PSI, CBCL) during the initial intake interview and again after the last PCAT session. Results Figure 1 presents the results of DPICS coding. As shown, the mother’s use of praise, descriptions and reflections significantly ‘Mother’s use of increased from pre-treatment to post-treatment. In the first praise, descriptions 5 minutes of the DPIC session at pre-treatment, the mother praised her son five times. In contrast, during the first 5 min- and reflections utes of the post-treatment DPICS, the mother used 46 praises, significantly well beyond the 15-praise mastery threshold. A similar increase increased from pre-treatment to post-treatment’ Figure 1. Performance in Weekly Treatment Sessions Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 13. 144 Dombrowski et al. Table 2. Measures of child and parent functioning: pre- vs post-treatment Measures Pre-treatment Post-treatment ECBI (T-scores) Intensity score 55 65* Problem score 59 — CBCL (T-scores) Internalizing score 53 58 Externalizing score 45 56 Total problem score 45 57 PSI-SF (percentile scores) Parent distress 7.5 5 Dysfunctional relationship 50 45 Difficult child 90* 85* Defensive responding 5* 5* * Scores at or above the clinical cutoff. was reflected in the number of descriptions and reflections provided by the mother. At pre-treatment, the mother had 12 descriptions/reflections, below the mastery threshold of 25. This increased to 59 descriptions/reflections by post- treatment, representing another significant gain. The number of questions the mother asked her child decreased from a pre- treatment level of 50 to a post-treatment level of two. Overall, the mother attained mastery on all criteria coded within PCAT. Table 2 shows the mother’s ratings of her child’s behaviour at pre-treatment and post-treatment, as well as ratings of her own functioning. In contrast to scores on observational measures, the mother rated the child’s problem behaviours as having increased slightly from pre- to post-treatment on both the ECBI and the CBCL. In the case of the ECBI intensity score, a measure of more everyday problem behaviours, the score moved from below the clinical cutoff into the clinical range. In contrast, scores on the PSI-SF decreased very ‘The mother slightly. It is interesting to note that the mother showed elev- showed elevated ated levels of defensive responding (lower percentile scores indicate higher defensive responding), perhaps indicating a levels of defensive desire to present herself and her relationship with her son in responding’ a positive light. Table 3 shows the results of the EA rating of this dyad. Pre- treatment scores on parent emotional availability scales are mixed: some are optimal (non-hostility), some are in the low- optimal range (sensitivity) and some are non-optimal (struc- turing, non-intrusiveness). The child’s emotional availability scores were all non-optimal. The mother’s sensitivity scores were in the low-optimal range throughout the DPICS session. Throughout pre-treatment CDI and clean-up, her affect was positive and consistent and she was engaged in her inter- action with the child, although the child did not respond to her overtures with a similar level of enthusiasm. In CDI, the Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 14. Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers 145 Table 3. EA results: mean scores and per cent score across DPICS scenarios Pre-treatment Post-treatment scores scores CDI CU CDI CU Parent scales Sensitivity (9-pt scale) 6 5 7 7 Non-hostility (5-pt scale) 5 5 5 5 Non-intrusiveness (5-pt scale) 3 2 4 4 Structuring (5-pt scale) 3 3 4 5 Child scales Responsiveness (7-pt scale) 4 2 6 6 Involvement (7-pt scale) 4 2 6 5 CDI, child directed interaction; CU, clean-up. mother played with the child for a short time, but the child’s interest in the play waned and the mother questioned him about what he wanted to do. The child got down from his chair and walked around the therapy room and the mother directed his attention to the toys on the table, asking him to help her clean them up. She continued to direct his cleaning up until the therapist switched the activity. During the ‘clean-up,’ L ignored her requests to pick up the toys and tantrumed. Her inability to support his play, and her over-directing ‘Her inability to of the ‘child-directed’ play, reduced her structuring and support his play, non-intrusiveness scores. The child’s continual need to be prompted by his mother in order to sustain his engagement and her over- with her and his tantrum during ‘clean-up’ reduced his respon- directing of the siveness and involvement scores. ‘child-directed’ At post-treatment, most of the mother and child’s emotional availability scores improved to the optimal range. The dyad play’ showed one non-optimal score in parent structuring and child responsiveness and involvement. During the post-treatment DPICS, mother and son sat together on the floor playing with playdough forms. The mother followed the child’s lead and elaborated his play. The child was highly engaged in play with his mother, handing her the playdough forms and saying, ‘Here, Mommy!’. At one point, he tried to take a sticker off a toy, but his mother successfully redirected his attention elsewhere. The child and his mother’s emotional availability scores showed great improvement from pre- to post-treatment. Over- all, the mother tried to verbally engage the child pre- and post-treatment. However, the mother was less directive, more positive and more creative in her play with the child at post- treatment than pre-treatment. She also seemed more comfort- able playing with her son at post-treatment and involved herself in his play more easily. The mother did not seem as disturbed by his non-compliance by post-treatment, handling Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 15. 146 Dombrowski et al. the ‘active ignore’ with ease, although she did need some as- ‘The child, for his sistance from the therapist. The child, for his part, appeared part, appeared more cheerful and talkative. He spent more time interacting positively with his mother and was able to control his negative more cheerful emotions quickly (once given the ‘active ignore’ framework). and talkative’ Discussion The results of this single case study suggest that PCAT with this 23-month-old maltreated toddler was successful, increas- ing the number of positive caregiver–toddler interactions and improving the emotional availability of the dyad. The mother learned how to play with L, responding appropriately to his cues. In turn, L responded positively to his mother’s praise and involved her in his play. While the treatment itself may have contributed to the improved emotional availability scores, it is also possible that the toddler’s reunification with his biolog- ical mother may have also played a role in increased EA scores. In our experience, newly reunified parents lack self-assurance in their interactions with their children and the children tend to be less responsive to them. Furthermore, the scores on the standardized measures suggest that PCAT might not have been completely successful in shifting the mother’s percep- tion of her son’s behaviours. However, for several reasons, ‘It is difficult to it is difficult to take this mother’s evaluation as reflective of take this mother’s the effectiveness of the treatment. First, the court mandated PCAT for this mother and her toddler following their re- evaluation as unification. In our experience, biological parents who are reflective of the ordered to participate in PCAT by the court often minimize effectiveness of their children’s problems. This mother’s tendency to minimize is reflected in her elevated defensive responding score on the the treatment’ PSI-SF. It is also reflected in the fact that the ECBI and CBCL scores were all in the normal range, although she reported concerns about temper tantrums (including throwing objects), aggression towards his younger sister, head-banging and non- compliance. It is difficult to observe expected treatment- related reductions in behaviour problems when parents minimize the child’s problems. Another obstacle to measurable treatment success in any treatment involving parent–child dyads is the quality of the marital relationship, and environmental stressors. Over the course of treatment, the therapist reported several incidences where conflict between the parents led to one parent leaving for a while. Also noted was L’s increased tendency to tantrum when the parents were not getting along. The parents were advised of their need to improve their own relationship and Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 16. Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers 147 referred to marriage counselling in order to provide more sta- bility for L. High levels of marital conflict can affect parents’ ‘High levels of ratings of their children’s behaviour in two ways. First, L could marital conflict have reacted to the conflict in the household by continuing to tantrum and act out aggressively. Research has shown that can affect parents’ children in families with high levels of domestic violence act ratings of their out more than children in families with low levels of domestic children’s violence (Davies et al., 2002; Jouriles et al., 1989). In fact, future research studies using behavioural-oriented parent behaviour in training models might benefit from incorporating a more two ways’ global assessment of both marital relationship and environ- mental risk factors (e.g. unemployment, unstable housing, community violence). Second, because the mother was already highly stressed by difficulties in her marital relationship post- PCAT, she could have been more sensitive to any additional stress caused by L’s negative behaviour than she had been pre- PCAT. Research in the child abuse literature has shown that levels of distress increase the chances of physically abusing children (Milner and Chilamkurti, 1991). Finally, L’s biolog- ical father was incarcerated about midway through PCAT and L’s mother reported that she was quite depressed by this event. It is possible that her elevated levels of depression influenced ‘It is possible that her ratings (see Kamphaus and Frick, 2001) such that she her elevated levels rated L as being more difficult, even though clinic-based ob- servations noted improved behavioural performance. Although of depression the mother reported more behaviour problems, she also influenced her reported to the therapist (recorded in clinical case notes) that ratings’ the toddler seemed less of a behaviour problem and that her relationship with him had improved considerably. While clinic- based observations (e.g. DPICS) and emotional availability ratings indicated improvement, standardized measures indi- cated a degree of deterioration, providing equivocal evidence for therapeutic improvement. Overall, the results of this study suggest that PCAT is a promising therapeutic programme that increases the number of positive parent–child interactions and may contribute to enhancing the parent–toddler relationship. At the same time, we point out the possibility that environmental risks (e.g. marital conflict) may be barriers or increase the difficulty of making progress in treatment. In so far as proper parent– toddler interactions set the stage for more harmonious inter- actions at later stages of development, a more harmonious relationship might help steer the parent–child dyad away from coercive interactional patterns that are common among mal- treated children and their caregivers even under the most dif- ficult circumstances (Urquiza and McNeil, 1996). Further, the formation of a secure parent–child relationship can enhance Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 17. 148 Dombrowski et al. the toddler’s ability to form future healthy relationships (Cicchetti and Olsen, 1990; Sroufe and Fleeson, 1986). It may also buffer the toddler from the onset of later psycho- pathology and language/cognitive delays that are commonly associated with maltreated children (Aber and Allen, 1987). ‘Treatment does These findings remind us that treatment does not take place not take place in in a vacuum and that external stressors often impinge upon the magnitude of its effectiveness. In the present case, we ob- a vacuum’ served improvements in parent–child relationship functioning despite the possibly traumatic effects of concurrent marital conflict. This paper represents an important initial step in establish- ing PCAT as a potentially effective behavioural treatment for improving toddler–parent relationships and decreasing beha- viour problems. PCAT is also a short-term, positive beha- vioural intervention for maltreated toddlers and toddlers who experience attachment difficulties. We have presented some of the difficulties of demonstrating treatment effectiveness in this population. These findings remind us that treatment ‘The context of a occurs within the context of a larger environment that does larger environment not always support positive change. However, these same limitations also show the robustness of the treatment. In spite that does not of the high stress level of the family environment, we were able always support to observe positive changes in the quality of the parent–child positive change’ relationship. Future research with a larger sample of toddlers and their parents that includes follow-up data will more appropriately determine the effectiveness of PCAT for the treatment of toddlers and for the enhancement of the parent– toddler relationship. References Aber JL, Allen JP. 1987. Effects of maltreatment on young children’s socioemotional development: an attachment theory perspective. Developmental Psychology 23: 406–414. Abidin RR. 1990. Parenting Stress Index (3rd edn). Pediatric Psychology Press: Charlottesville, VA. Achenbach TM. 1991. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 Profile. University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry: Burlington, VT. Bakermans-Kranenburg MH, van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F. 2003. Less is more: meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychological Bulletin 129: 195–215. Biringen Z, Robinson J, Emde R. 1998. Emotional Availability Scales (3rd edn). Unpublished manual, Colorado State University. Borrego J, Urquiza AJ, Rasmussen RA, Zebell N. 1999. Parent–child interaction therapy with a family at high risk for physical abuse. Child Maltreatment 4: 331–342. Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 18. Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers 149 Bousha DM, Twentyman CT. 1984. Mother–child interactional style in abuse, neglect, and control groups: naturalistic observations in the home. Child Development 49: 1163–1173. Cerezo MA, D’Ocon AD. 1999. Sequential analyses in coercive mother– child interaction: the predictability hypothesis in abusive versus nonabusive dyads. Child Abuse & Neglect 23: 99–113. Cicchetti D, Olsen K. 1990. The developmental psychopathology of child maltreatment. In Handbook of Developmental Psychopathology, Lewis M, Miller MS (eds). Plenum Press: New York; 261–279. Cicchetti D, Toth SL. 1994. Rochester Symposium on Developmental Psychopathology (Vol. 5). University of Rochester Press: Rochester, NY. Cicchetti D, Toth SL. 1995. A developmental psychopathology perspec- tive on child abuse and neglect. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 34: 541–565. Cole DA. 1990. Relation of social and academic competence to depres- sive symptoms in childhood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 99: 422–429. Cummings M, Davies PT, Campbell S. 2000. Developmental Psycho- pathology & Family Process. Guilford Press: New York. Davies PT, Forman EM, Rasi JA, Stevens KI. 2002. Assessing children’s emotional security in the interparental subsystem: the Security in the Interparental Subsystem (SIS) scales. Child Development 73: 544– 562. Dombrowski SC. 2003. Mandated reporting for mental health profession- als: an overview. Directions in Mental Health Counseling 14: 77–88. Dombrowski SC, Timmer SG. 2001. Parent–child attunement therapy with a 23 month-old maltreated toddler. Paper presented at the second annual PCIT conference, Sacramento, CA. Dombrowski SC, Ahia CE, McQuillan K. 2003. Protecting children through mandated child abuse reporting. The Educational Forum 67: 76–85. Easterbrooks A, Biesecker G, Lyons-Ruth K. 2000. Infancy predictors of emotional availability in middle childhood: the roles of attachment security and maternal depressive symptomatology. Attachment & Human Development 2: 170–187. Eisenstadt TH, Eyberg SM, McNeil CB, Newcomb K, Funderburk BW. 1983. Parent–child interaction therapy with behavior problem children: relative effectiveness of two stages and overall treatment outcomes. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 22: 42–51. Eyberg SM. 1988. PCIT: integration of traditional and behavioral con- cerns. Child and Family Behavior Therapy 10: 33–46. Eyberg SM, Robinson EA. 1982. Parent–child interaction training: effects on family functioning. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 11: 130– 137. Eyberg SM, Ross AW. 1978. Assessment of child behavior problems: the validation of a new inventory. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 7: 113–116. Glaser D. 2000. Child abuse and neglect and the brain—a review. Jour- nal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines 41: 97–116. Hembree-Kigin TL, McNeil CB. 1995. Parent–Child Interaction Therapy. Plenum: New York. Jouriles E, Murphy C, O’Leary KD. 1989. Interspousal aggression, marital discord, and child problems. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 57: 453–455. Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 19. 150 Dombrowski et al. Kalat JW. 2002. Biological Psychology (7th edn). Wadsworth/Thomson Learning: Belmont, CA. Kamphaus RW, Frick PJ. 2001. Clinical Assessment of Child and Adolescent Personality and Behavior (2nd edn). Allyn & Bacon: Needham Heights, MA. Kochanska G, Aksan N. 1995. Mother–child mutually positive affect, the quality of child compliance to requests and prohibitions, and maternal control as correlates of early internalization. Child Development 66: 236–254. Lowenthal B. 1998. The effects of early childhood abuse and the development of resiliency. Early Child Development & Care 142: 43– 52. Manly JT, Jungmeen E, Rogosch FA, Cicchetti D. 2001. Dimensions of child maltreatment and children’s adjustment: contributions of developmental timing and subtype. Development & Psychopathology 13: 759–782. Milner JS, Chilamkurti C. 1991. Physical child abuse perpetrator charac- teristics: a review of the literature. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 6: 345–366. Paravicini SF. 2000. Parent–child attunement therapy: development of a program for children one to three years old. Dissertation Abstracts International 60(9-B) (UMI No. AAI9945876). Paravicini SF, Urquiza AJ, Blacker DM. 2000. Parent–child attunement therapy: development of a program for children one to three years old. Paper presented at the first annual PCIT conference, Sacramento, CA. Robinson EA, Eyberg SM. 1981. The dyadic parent–child interaction coding system: standardization and validation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 49: 245–250. Robinson EA, Eyberg SM, Ross WA. 1980. The standardization of an inventory of child conduct problem behaviors. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 9: 22–29. Schore AN. 2001. Effects of a secure attachment relationship on right brain development, affect regulation, and infant mental health. Infant Mental Health Journal 22: 7–66. Shengold L. 1989. Soul Murder: The Effects of Childhood Abuse and Deprivation. Yale University Press: New Haven, CT. Smetana J, Daddis C, Toth SL, Cicchetti D, Bruce J, Kane P. 1999. Effects of provocation on maltreated and non-maltreated preschoolers’ understanding of moral transgressions. Social Development 8: 335– 348. Sroufe LA, Fleeson J. 1986. Attachment and the construction of relation- ships. In Relationships and Development, Hartup WW, Rubin Z (eds). Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ; 51–71. Teicher MH, Andersen SL, Polcari A, Anderson CM, Navalta CP. 2002. Developmental neurobiology of childhood stress and trauma. Psy- chiatric Clinics of North America 25: 397–426. Toth SL, Cicchetti D, Macfie J, Emde RN. 1997. Representations of self and other in the narratives of neglected, physically abused, and sexually abused preschoolers. Development & Psychopathology 9: 781–796. Urquiza AJ, McNeil CB. 1996. Parent–child interaction therapy: an intensive dyadic intervention for physically abusive families. Child Maltreatment 1: 134–144. Wolfe DA. 1987. Child-abusive parents: an empirical review and ana- lysis. Psychological Bulletin 97: 462–482. Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)
  • 20. Parent–Child Attunement Therapy for Toddlers 151 Wolfe DA. 1999. Child Abuse: Implication for Child Development and Psychopathology. Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA. Youngblade LM, Belsky J. 1989. Child maltreatment, infant–parent attachment security, and dysfunctional peer relationships in toddlerhood. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 9: 1–15. Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Child Abuse Review Vol. 14: 132–151 (2005)